The Dementias Diagnosis, Treatment, and Research Third Edition
This page intentionally left blank
The Dementias Diagnosis, Treatment, and Research Third Edition
Edited by Myron F. Weiner, M.D. Professor of Psychiatry, Associate Professor of Neurology, Aradine S. Ard Chair in Brain Science, and Dorothy L. and John P. Harbin Chair in Alzheimer’s Disease Research, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas
Anne M. Lipton, M.D., Ph.D. Assistant Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas
Washington, DC London, England
Note: The authors have worked to ensure that all information in this book is accurate at the time of publication and consistent with general psychiatric and medical standards, and that information concerning drug dosages, schedules, and routes of administration is accurate at the time of publication and consistent with standards set by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration and the general medical community. As medical research and practice continue to advance, however, therapeutic standards may change. Moreover, specific situations may require a specific therapeutic response not included in this book. For these reasons and because human and mechanical errors sometimes occur, we recommend that readers follow the advice of physicians directly involved in their care or the care of a member of their family. Books published by American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc., represent the views and opinions of the individual authors and do not necessarily represent the policies and opinions of APPI or the American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic criteria included in this book are reprinted, with permission, from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision. Copyright 2000, American Psychiatric Association. Copyright © 2003 American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc. ALL RIGHTS RESERVED Manufactured in the United States of America on acid-free paper 07 06 05 04 03 5 4 3 2 1 Third Edition Typeset in Adobe’s Janson Text and ITC Highlander Book American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc. 1400 K Street, N.W. Washington, DC 20005 www.appi.org Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data The dementias : diagnosis, treatment, and research / edited by Myron F. Weiner, Anne M. Lipton. — 3rd ed. p. ; cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 1-58562-043-2 (alk. paper) 1. Dementia. I. Weiner, Myron F., 1934– II. Lipton, Anne M., 1966– [DNLM: 1. Dementia—diagnosis. 2. Dementia—therapy. WM 220 D376391 2003] RC521 .D458 2003 616.8'3—dc21 2002074565
British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data A CIP record is available from the British Library.
To Jack Weiner, my father, who struggled valiantly against the ravages of Alzheimer’s disease, and in the hope that others may not have to endure the agony he suffered. M.F.W. For my mother, Anne G. Peternel. A.M.L.
This page intentionally left blank
Contents
Contributors. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .xi Foreword . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xv Marcelle Morrison-Bogorad, Ph.D.
Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xvii Myron F. Weiner, M.D. Anne M. Lipton, M.D., Ph.D.
Chapter 1 Clinical Diagnosis of Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness . . . . 1 Myron F. Weiner, M.D.
Chapter 2 Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 Myron F. Weiner, M.D.
Chapter 3 Medical Evaluation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 Myron F. Weiner, M.D. Anne M. Lipton, M.D., Ph.D. Ramón Diaz-Arrastia, M.D., Ph.D.
Chapter 4 Neuroimaging . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103 Gene E. Alexander, Ph.D. Eric M. Reiman, M.D.
Chapter 5 Differential Diagnosis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 137 Anne M. Lipton, M.D., Ph.D. Myron F. Weiner, M.D.
Chapter 6 Psychological and Behavioral Management . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181 Myron F. Weiner, M.D. Linda Teri, Ph.D.
Chapter 7 Drugs for Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms . . . . . . 219 Myron F. Weiner, M.D. Lon S. Schneider, M.D.
Chapter 8 Evaluation of Cognitive Functions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 285 C. Munro Cullum, Ph.D. Ronald G. Paulman, Ph.D. Elisabeth Koss, Ph.D. Sandra Bond Chapman, Ph.D. Laura Lacritz, Ph.D.
Chapter 9 Supporting Family Caregivers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 321 Kristin Martin-Cook, M.S. Doris Svetlik, B.S.N., R.N., M.S. Myron F. Weiner, M.D.
Chapter 10 Legal and Ethical Issues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 341 John Z. Sadler, M.D. Barton E. Bernstein, J.D., L.M.S.W. Daniel C. Marson, J.D., Ph.D.
Chapter 11 Mobilizing Community Resources . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 371 Doris Svetlik, B.S.N., R.N., M.S. Jan W. Weaver, Ph.D., R.N.
Chapter 12 Nursing Care for Persons With Cognitive Impairment . . . . . . . . . . . . 381 Elaine Souder, Ph.D., R.N. Cornelia Beck, R.N., Ph.D.
Chapter 13 Structuring Environments for Persons With Cognitive Impairment . . . 405 Paul K. Chafetz, Ph.D. Kevan H. Namazi, Ph.D.
Chapter 14 Advances in the Molecular and Genetic Basis of Alzheimer’s Disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 433 Roger N. Rosenberg, M.D.
Chapter 15 Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 453 Steven C. Samuels, M.D. Kenneth L. Davis, M.D.
Appendix A Dementia Questionnaire . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 483 Appendix B Mental Status Examination . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 487 Appendix C Neurological Examination . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 495 Appendix D Blessed Dementia Rating Scale. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 499 Appendix E Washington University Clinical Dementia Rating Scale . . . . . . . . . . . 501 Appendix F Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 503 Appendix G Neuropsychiatric Inventory: Community Dwelling Version . . . . . . . . 513 Appendix H Agitated Behavior in Dementia Scale. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 525
Appendix I Quality of Life in Alzheimer’s Disease Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 529 Appendix J Quality of Life in Late-Stage Dementia (QUALID) Scale . . . . . . . . . . 535 Appendix K Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale (AIMS) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 539 Appendix L Resources . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 541
Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 547
Contributors
Gene E. Alexander, Ph.D. Associate Professor, Department of Psychology and Center for Alzheimer’s Disease Research, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona Cornelia Beck, R.N., Ph.D. Professor, Department of Geriatrics, College of Medicine, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, Little Rock, Arkansas Barton E. Bernstein, J.D., L.M.S.W. Adjunct Professor of Psychiatry, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas Paul K. Chafetz, Ph.D. Adjunct Associate Professor of Rehabilitation Counseling Psychology, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas Sandra Bond Chapman, Ph.D. Research Scientist, University of Texas at Dallas, Texas C. Munro Cullum, Ph.D. Professor of Psychiatry and Neurology, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas Kenneth L. Davis, M.D. Professor of Psychiatry and Pharmacology and Chairman, Department of Psychiatry, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, New York Ramón Diaz-Arrastia, M.D., Ph.D. Associate Professor of Neurology, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas Elisabeth Koss, Ph.D. Assistant Director, Alzheimer’s Disease Centers Program, National Institute on Aging, Bethesda, Maryland
xi
xii
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Laura Lacritz, Ph.D. Assistant Professor of Psychiatry and Neurology, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas Anne M. Lipton, M.D., Ph.D. Assistant Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas Daniel C. Marson, J.D., Ph.D. Associate Professor of Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Alabama Kristin Martin-Cook, M.S. Family Services Coordinator, Clinic for Alzheimer’s and Related Diseases, Department of Psychiatry, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas Marcelle Morrison-Bogorad, Ph.D. Associate Director, Neuroscience and Neuropsychology of Aging Program, National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland Kevan H. Namazi, Ph.D. Associate Professor and Chair, Department of Gerontology and Geriatric Services, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas Ronald G. Paulman, Ph.D. Clinical Professor of Psychiatry, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas Eric M. Reiman, M.D. Professor and Associate Head of Psychiatry, University of Arizona, Phoenix, Arizona Roger N. Rosenberg, M.D. Abe (Brunky) Morris and William Zale Distinguished Chair in Neurology; Professor of Neurology and Physiology, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas John Z. Sadler, M.D Professor of Psychiatry, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas
Contributors
xiii
Steven C. Samuels, M.D. Assistant Professor of Psychiatry, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, New York Lon S. Schneider, M.D. Professor of Psychiatry, Neurology, and Gerontology, University of Southern California Keck School of Medicine, Los Angeles, California Elaine Souder, Ph.D., R.N. Professor, College of Nursing, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, Little Rock, Arkansas Doris Svetlik, B.S.N., R.N., M.S. Administrative Director, Clinic for Alzheimer’s and Related Diseases, Department of Psychiatry, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas Linda Teri, Ph.D. Professor, Department of Psychosocial/Community Health, School of Nursing, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington Jan W. Weaver, Ph.D., R.N. Director of Health and Wellness, Hearthstone Assisted Living, Inc., Houston, Texas Myron F. Weiner, M.D. Professor of Psychiatry, Associate Professor of Neurology, Aradine S. Ard Chair in Brain Science, and Dorothy L. and John P. Harbin Chair in Alzheimer’s Disease Research, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, Texas
This page intentionally left blank
Foreword
This is the third edition of The Dementias: Diagnosis, Treatment, and Research. Primarily aimed at clinicians and other health care professionals who work with dementia patients, this book summarizes state-of-the-art, practical approaches to the diagnosis and treatment of dementing illnesses. Current exciting advances in understanding the etiology, diagnosis, and treatment of these illnesses are reviewed, including a new chapter on advances in diagnosis using neuroimaging. Alzheimer’s disease is a major public health issue for the United States. It is by far the most common form of dementing illness, with estimates of its prevalence in the United States as high as 4 million. If treatments to delay onset are not identified, the demographic trend toward increased numbers of elderly persons in populations worldwide means that this number will continue to climb. The age category most affected by Alzheimer’s disease, those over age 85, is projected to grow from 4.4 million in 2001 to 19.3 million in 2050. The yearly cost of Alzheimer’s disease, now estimated at around $100 billion, is only part of the societal cost. The suffering and stress to individuals and families as their family member gradually becomes more and more impaired is incalculable. Since the first edition of this book was published in 1991, advances in the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of dementing illnesses like Alzheimer’s disease have been accelerating. This edition summarizes the latest strategies for differential diagnosis of the dementing illnesses and evaluation of cognitive functions. One important new focus both in the clinic and in the laboratory is on understanding early, preclinical stages of dementing illness and how they differ from normal aging. With imaging, neuropsychological, and clinical tools, scientists are determining the best ways to identify persons who are at the highest risk for developing Alzheimer’s disease. This helps us to better understand the very beginnings of the disease process and identifies persons for whom treatment will be most effective as new drugs and therapies become available. Genetic, epidemiological, and molecular clues to the disease are also rapidly being discovered. Perhaps most exciting, a number of treatments have been shown to decrease the number of brain plaques in transgenic mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease, including vaccination against plaque amyloid itself. Several of these potential interventions are already at various stages of testing in xv
xvi
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
clinical trials to delay progression in persons with Alzheimer’s disease or to delay onset in cognitively normal persons or individuals with only mild cognitive impairment. Clues to the origin of other dementing illnesses have also been uncovered, and both commonalities and differences are being found. These aid in differential diagnosis and also with identifying underlying disease mechanisms. One common theme emerging from the study of various neurodegenerative diseases is the accumulation of aggregated proteins in particular brain cells and brain regions; avenues for preventing or reversing this aggregation are being explored. Studies identifying causal or risk-factor genes give us powerful clues to the nature of the brain processes that first go awry in particular diseases. There is more reason than ever to believe that advances in understanding Alzheimer’s disease and other dementing illnesses will someday soon pay off in terms of delaying disease onset or preventing Alzheimer’s disease or dementia entirely. Today, however, families and society continue to be faced with the challenge of caring for persons with dementia, and it is vital that (as is done in this volume) materials for clinicians include review of the latest information on disease management and long-term care. Much of the challenge of dealing with dementia patients, for example, comes not from the cognitive loss but from behavioral symptoms such as wandering and agitation. The chapters on psychological and behavioral management and drug management are excellent summaries of practical approaches to maintaining the best possible level of function for individual patients at different stages of illness. They are full of helpful, concrete examples. So, too, is the chapter on family support and that on mobilizing community resources. Nursing care of cognitively impaired individuals is an area in which much research still needs to be done. The chapter on this subject nicely summarizes practical research on coping with a variety of problems faced by nursing staffs. Again emphasizing the human element, the chapter on legal and ethical issues addresses the rights of demented patients, including research on assessment of decisional capacity. In this as in every chapter, the reader is afforded insight, where appropriate, into the physiological, psychological, and social underpinnings of diagnosis and treatments. Marcelle Morrison-Bogorad, Ph.D.
Introduction
My husband is most often kind, considerate and understanding. I love him! But he does not understand my circumstances— He wants me to drive a strange car—little does he understand the danger involved. The synapses in my brain are not up to making quick decisions, such as a speeding car trying to cut around me—or a need to push some crucial button on the dashboard—which takes a minute or two to find. In that minute the oncoming car has hit me (a synapse is the [point] where neurons in the brain touch and nerve impulses are transmitted from one to another). I am certainly sorry that you can’t always handle the way I am. I will try to stay out of your way and be as little trouble to you as possible. Let me know if you want something! I love you!
The woman who wrote the preceding words was trying to say that she should not drive a strange car and to tell why she should not. She handed the note to a man she did not recognize as her husband when she wrote the note. She did not know that the strange car was the new family car. After all, it was strange to her. She pointed out that she reacts slowly and would have difficulty locating a “crucial button” and might thereby be a danger to herself and others. She could spell the word “synapse” and knew what it meant, but at first she misspelled the simple word “point.” She felt sorry for her husband having to put up with her. She would try not to be a bother. Then, finally recognizing him, she writes that she loves him. We can see in this woman’s words the confusion and fear that she experiences, her attempts to come to grips with her illness, her difficulty in expressing herself, and her wish to maintain her loving relationship with her husband. This is the human experience of Alzheimer’s disease, and it is at the heart of the third edition of The Dementias: Diagnosis, Treatment, and Research. This book is designed to meet the needs of clinicians dealing with persons with dementing illness, to serve as an introduction to the neurobiology of dementing illnesses, and to serve as a resource for clinical investigators. The present edition has been revised, updated, and expanded to cover xvii
xviii
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
changes in the classification, management, treatment, and research in dementing illness. We have changed the book’s subtitle to indicate that we can now offer disease-modifying treatments for many dementing illnesses, including Alzheimer’s disease. Because of these advances, attempts are being made to identify those at risk for the development of these disorders and to develop prophylactic treatments. In the case of vascular dementia, aggressive treatment of hypertension and hyperlipidemia has reduced the incidence of stroke. Studies are under way to determine if treating persons at high risk for developing Alzheimer’s disease will reduce their risk. The change in emphasis to early detection is reflected in the change of title for Chapter 1 from “Diagnosis of Dementia” to “Clinical Diagnosis of Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness.” Since the publication of the last edition, formal criteria have been proposed for the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment, vascular dementia, dementia with Lewy bodies, and frontotemporal dementia. Progress has been made in developing behavioral interventions for behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. Many changes have come about in the pharmacological management of psychiatric symptoms, such as the virtual replacement of the use in elders of tricyclic antidepressants by the use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and the wide use of anticonvulsant and antimanic drugs for control of behavioral symptoms. Several drugs that augment cholinergic function modestly improve cognition, slow clinical deterioration, and reduce behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease patients. High-dosage vitamin E has been shown to slow the progression of Alzheimer’s disease. Trials of estrogen and prednisolone in persons with Alzheimer’s disease have unfortunately been unsuccessful thus far. Mutations of the presenilin genes on chromosomes 1 and 14 have been found that account for many of the early-onset familial cases of Alzheimer’s disease, and the EA4 allele of apolipoprotein E4 has been found to be an important risk factor for late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Nonsteroidal antiinflammatory drugs and statin drugs may have prophylactic effects in Alzheimer’s disease, with statins also having prophylactic effects in vascular dementia. Further strides have been made in elucidating the pathophysiology of Alzheimer’s disease. Accumulation of toxic >-amyloid protein in brain through overproduction or abnormal processing of amyloid precursor protein may result in amyloid plaques. Interest in the tau protein, the main constituent of neurofibrillary tangles, has undergone a resurgence since the discovery of familial and sporadic tauopathies. This book embodies a broadly based approach to the diagnosis and management of the dementias and conveys the spirit of optimism in contemporary dementia research. This volume is coedited by a geriatric psychiatrist (M.F.W.) and a behavioral neurologist (A.M.L.), reflecting the close
Introduction
xix
alliance between psychiatry and neurology in the diagnosis and treatment of dementing illness. There have also been changes in the authorship of several chapters. The section on neuroimaging has been expanded into an entire chapter because of the rapid accumulation of imaging information. Full-blown dementia, the result of many types of brain insult, is a diagnostic term referring to global impairment of higher cortical function sufficient to compromise social or work adaptation. Dementing illness is accompanied by emotional, behavioral, ideational, perceptual, and vegetative disturbances in addition to impaired cognitive function. Although these cognitive, emotional, behavioral, ideational, perceptual, and vegetative disturbances result from many different dementing illnesses, certain general principles guide management and treatment. Unlike many other illnesses, dementing illness attacks the personhood of its victims, robbing them of their ability to understand, communicate, fend for themselves, or tend to the needs of others. This loss of personhood is well described in patients’ own words in Cohen and Eisdorfer’s The Loss of Self: A Family Resource for the Care of Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders (New York, WW Norton, 2001) and in many other recent volumes. Persons with dementia have described it as a painful dismemberment, a drifting away, a sense of being lost, or a feeling of being enveloped by a hazy curtain. Ultimately, it is an alienation from self and others. Many individuals are unaware of their progressive loss of function and do not appear to struggle against it. Those who struggle against dementing illness—trying to remember what was once familiar, or trying to grasp new situations—experience enormous emotional pain. Yet, such pain may be overlooked by clinicians who focus primarily on the cognitive aspects of these diseases and can be aggravated by the clinicians addressing family members as though the person with dementia were not present. The perception of many who work with these patients is that the remnants of a person can often be detected through the distorting effects of perceptual and cognitive impairment. Such persons can often be reached, if not through spoken language, through tone of voice or physical contact. It is a unique experience to momentarily touch another—who is drifting out of contact with his or her world—and to be experienced as an anchor to reality. This experience, at once painful and gratifying, is the essence of the therapeutic relationship, maintaining both hope and human contact. Patients and their families are grateful to health care professionals and others who allow themselves to be “in touch” in this way, and they bond strongly to those who are willing to do so. Although patients want to be cured, they and their families also value health care providers as compassionate companions who provide comfort and support. It is our aim to help clinicians comfort, support, and treat individuals with dementing illnesses.
xx
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Working effectively with these patients, their families, and other caregivers requires knowledge derived from medicine; pharmacology; cognitive, behavioral, and linguistic assessment; psychodynamics; family interaction; and systems management to address the interaction between biological, psychological, and social aspects of dementing illness. Effective diagnosis and management call for collaboration between primary care physicians; psychiatrists; neurologists; psychologists; nurses; social workers; physical, occupational, and speech therapists; lawyers; nursing home administrators; and other persons concerned with various aspects of family life, rehabilitation, and extended care. The diagnostic process is ongoing. Many individuals present with problems of memory or other aspects of cognitive function, but without overall significant impairment. We know that many of these individuals will soon develop clinical Alzheimer’s disease, but at present, diagnosis can be made in these cases only by longitudinal follow-up. Also, an apparent progression of dementia may not be due to the illness originally diagnosed. Various systemic illnesses mimic the symptoms of dementing illness. Treating illnesses such as urinary tract infection, pneumonia, and congestive heart failure can help alleviate these aggravating factors. The treatment strategy for dementing illness depends on various factors: whether or not the illness is reversible, the individual’s life situation, and the availability of family and community resources. Complicated emotional, legal, and ethical decisions must often be faced at different points in the evolution of a dementing illness. Early on, important role changes occur between child and parent or spouse and spouse; later, a decision may be needed regarding nursing home placement. Legal issues include power of attorney and guardianship. Ethical issues may involve decisions on the inclusion of a loved one in antemortem and postmortem scientific research or feeding by nasogastric tube or gastrostomy. Health care professionals help to maintain patients’ and families’ hope while at the same time encouraging a realistic approach to each patient’s illness. Humane, effective care of persons with dementing illness is one of the important challenges facing the next few generations of health care providers. We hope this book conveys the positive attitude of those of us who deal day to day with dementing illness. The appreciation of families who care for loved ones with dementing illness is gratifying; their courage, strength, and ingenuity are inspiring.
Acknowledgments We are heavily indebted to the local and national Alzheimer’s Associations for providing information, support, and advocacy for our patients and their
Introduction
xxi
families and to the National Institute on Aging for funding and guiding the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center’s Alzheimer’s Disease Center (ADC) and its satellite clinics. We thank our present and former colleagues at the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center ADC, including Roger Rosenberg, Kyle Womack, Ron Tintner, Jim Hom, Charles White III, Eileen Bigio, Perrie Adams, Munro Cullum, Barbara Foster, Laura Lacritz, Alan Frol, Kathleen Saine, Samuel Speciale, Joan Reisch, Linda Hynan, Margaret Higgins, Ramón Diaz-Arrastia, Sandra Chapman,and Lawrence Honig. We also extend our gratitude to the staff of our ADC Clinical Core, led by Doris Svetlik, and including Jackie Rabb, Robbin Peck, Shelly Daniels, Kristin Martin-Cook, Kathleen Koch, Carol Moore, Mary Riojas, Loretta Pedyfoot, Dianne Phillips, Alice Cox, Barbara Remakel, Taylor Ninman, Kim Booker, and Kim Moser. The generous support of Marsha Ard and John Harbin has allowed Dr. Weiner time to become immersed in dementia research and teaching. Dr. Lipton was supported during a year of fellowship by funding from the State of Texas and the Department of Neurology at the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center. We also appreciate the help of Dr. Richard Barohn, former professor and interim chair of Neurology at the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center. Thanks also to Carol Laabs for organizing the correspondence, keeping track of chapters, and organizing the text for submission, and to American Psychiatric Publishing for their willingness to undertake a third edition. Jeanette Weiner and Lee Lipton continue to be patient and understanding. Myron F. Weiner, M.D. Anne M. Lipton, M.D., Ph.D.
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER
1
Clinical Diagnosis of Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness Myron F. Weiner, M.D.
Cognitive dysfunction and dementing illnesses are most common in elderly persons. Mild levels of cognitive impairment appear to be part of the normal aging process. More severe impairment in cognitive spheres such as memory or language may herald the onset of dementing illnesses. The overall incidence and prevalence of these illnesses are not known, because estimates concerning diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease are based on persons who meet criteria for dementia and thus miss the large number of patients with mild symptoms. A survey of memory function was performed among community-dwelling elderly persons in the United States as part of the 2000 census. Individuals were rated as having moderate to severe memory problems if they recalled 4 or fewer words from a list of 20 words on combined immediate and delayed recall. On the basis of this criterion, 4% of individuals between ages 65 and 69 years and 36% of those ages 85 years or older had moderate to severe memory problems (Federal Interagency Forum 2000). A study was performed in England using the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) (Folstein et al. 1975) followed by the Cambridge Examination for Mental Disorders of the Elderly (Roth et al. 1988). In this study, incidence rates for dementia were 2.3% for persons ages 75–79 years, 4.6% for persons ages 80–84 years, and 8.5% for persons ages 85–89 years—the incidence approximately doubles for every 5 years of age (Paykel et al. 1994). Based on a study using age-specific incidence rates summarized from several epidemiologic studies, United States mortality rates, and U.S. Census Bureau projections, the prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease in the United States was 2.32 million in 1997 (Brookmeyer et al. 1998). This prevalence is expected to quadruple by 2050. If Alzheimer’s disease and mixed dementia (Alzheimer’s disease plus vascular dementia) account for approximately 56% of dementia cases (Folstein et al. 1991), the total number of elderly persons with dementia in 1
2
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
1997 was more than 4 million. These figures are much less than those given in a federal government publication estimating that in the year 2000, there were approximately 35 million Americans ages 65 years or older with dementia (Federal Interagency Forum 2000). Whatever the figure, Alzheimer’s disease and dementing illness in general are clearly of considerable importance to public heath. A Canadian study suggests that dementing illness in old age has a very malignant course, with individuals having Alzheimer’s disease or vascular dementia surviving only an average of 3.3 years, but the study had important design flaws. It was a retrospective study; in many cases it employed as the starting point the date of the first medical visit for memory problems; and its participants had an average age at enrollment of nearly 85 years (Wolfson et al. 2001). On the other hand, it is clear that Alzheimer’s disease is now on a par with stroke as the third leading cause of death in the United States, accounting for 7.1% of all United States deaths in 1995 (Ewbank 1999). Of course, cognitive dysfunction and dementing disorders are not confined to old age. Cognitive dysfunction is a common consequence of head injury in youth and early adulthood, of alcoholism in middle age, and of acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS) at all ages. Dementing illness may also manifest at younger ages, for example, in hereditary illnesses such as Huntington’s disease or in the frontotemporal dementias. As research in dementing illnesses has progressed, three major developments have occurred for clinicians. There has been a shift from defining these diseases in terms of their full-blown, late manifestations toward earlier diagnosis, classification of subtypes, and identification of the underlying pathophysiological processes. The goal of diagnosis has become early recognition of dementing illnesses or their prodromes in the hope of treating them before they result in dementia. Thus, clinicians now speak of Alzheimer’s disease instead of the DSM-IV-TR category dementia of the Alzheimer’s type (American Psychiatric Association 2000). The second major shift has been the realization that diseases and conditions producing cognitive deficits that are not primarily memory-related can disrupt social or occupational function (Royall 2000). This applies especially to the group of disorders produced by frontotemporal disease processes (called frontotemporal dementias in the neurological literature) in which the first deficits may be in language or in executive function as manifested by impaired initiative or social judgment. The third shift has been the understanding that behavioral and emotional symptoms are part of the intrinsic manifestations of dementing illnesses, not just by-products of or reactions to cognitive impairment (Finkel et al. 1996). In this chapter I review and critique the relevant DSM-IV-TR diagnoses and set the stage for Chapter 3, which deals with detecting the illnesses underlying the DSM-IV-TR syndromes described in this chapter.
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
3
DSM-IV-TR diagnoses describe symptom clusters that meet a threshold of “clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning” (American Psychiatric Association 2000, p. 8). The diagnosis therefore indicates the minimal level of personal discomfort or dysfunction that is present. On the other hand, a person may have a dementing illness but does not meet DSM-IV-TR criteria for dementia. The advantage of threshold diagnoses over disease diagnoses is that they are less likely to yield false-positive diagnoses. On the other hand, threshold diagnoses lack the sensitivity to detect early disease and thus have the potential for delaying possible disease-modifying treatments. Despite these shortcomings, it is important to make diagnoses by criteria that are stable, are reproducible, and reflect the impact of diseases on the persons affected by them.
DSM-IV-TR Mental Disorders DSM-I through DSM-III contained the category of organic mental disorders—constellations of cognitive, behavioral, and emotional symptoms resulting from demonstrable changes in brain anatomy or physiology. Organic disorders were differentiated from functional disorders, in which no alteration in brain anatomy or physiology had been demonstrated or was thought to occur. It is now clear that any mental disorder, whether classified as organic or functional, results from altered brain activity, making distinctions between organic and functional perhaps superfluous and misleading. For these reasons, the American Psychiatric Association deleted the diagnostic category of organic mental disorders from DSM-IV and the most recent DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association 2000). In its place are the categories shown in Table 1–1. They are 1) delirium, dementia, and amnestic and other cognitive disorders; 2) mental disorders due to a general medical condition; and 3) substancerelated disorders. Although these disorders are diagnosed as separate entities, they are overlapping symptom complexes that often involve the same disease. For example, a disease beginning in the frontal lobes might be diagnosed initially as a personality change due to a general medical condition. As executive function and information processing become affected with disease progression, it would become diagnosable as a dementia. Nevertheless, diagnosis of DSM-IV-TR mental disorder subtype is important because of the differences in treatment and prognosis. In some cases, proper diagnosis is a life-or-death issue, such as differentiating barbiturate withdrawal or acute Wernicke’s syndrome from alcoholic dementia. There are many
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
4
TABLE 1–1.
DSM-IV-TR classification (partial listing)
Delirium, Dementia, and Amnestic and Other Cognitive Disorders DELIRIUM 293.0 Delirium Due to . . . [Indicate the General Medical Condition] —–.–
Substance Intoxication Delirium (refer to Substance-Related Disorders for substance-specific codes) —–.– Substance Withdrawal Delirium (refer to Substance-Related Disorders for substance-specific codes) —–.– Delirium Due to Multiple Etiologies (code each of the specific etiologies) 780.09 Delirium NOS DEMENTIA 294.xx Dementia of the Alzheimer’s Type, With Early Onset (also code 331.0 Alzheimer’s disease on Axis III) .10 Without Behavioral Disturbance .11 With Behavioral Disturbance 294.xx Dementia of the Alzheimer’s Type, With Late Onset (also code 331.0 Alzheimer’s disease on Axis III) .10 Without Behavioral Disturbance .11 With Behavioral Disturbance 290.xx Vascular Dementia .40 Uncomplicated .41 With Delirium .42 With Delusions .43 With Depressed Mood Specify if: With Behavioral Disturbance
Code presence or absence of a behavioral disturbance in the fifth digit for Dementia Due to a General Medical Condition: 0 = Without Behavioral Disturbance 1 = With Behavioral Disturbance
294.1x Dementia Due to HIV Disease (also code 042 HIV on Axis III) 294.1x Dementia Due to Head Trauma (also code 854.00 head injury on Axis III) 294.1x Dementia Due to Parkinson’s Disease (also code 332.0 Parkinson’s disease on Axis III) 294.1x Dementia Due to Huntington's Disease (also code 333.4 Huntington's disease on Axis III) 294.1x Dementia Due to Pick's Disease (also code 331.1 Pick's disease on Axis III) 294.1x Dementia Due to Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease (also code 046.1 CreutzfeldtJakob disease on Axis III) 294.1x Dementia Due to . . . [Indicate the General Medical Condition not listed above] (also code the general medical condition on Axis III) —–.–– Substance-Induced Persisting Dementia (refer to Substance-Related Disorders for substance-specific codes) —–.–– Dementia Due to Multiple Etiologies (code each of the specific etiologies) 294.8 Dementia NOS
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
TABLE 1–1.
5
DSM-IV-TR classification (partial listing) (continued)
AMNESTIC DISORDERS 294.0 Amnestic Disorder Due to . . . [Indicate the General Medical Condition] Specify if: Transient/Chronic
—–.– 294.8
Substance-Induced Persisting Amnestic Disorder (refer to SubstanceRelated Disorders for substance-specific codes) Amnestic Disorder NOS
OTHER COGNITIVE DISORDERS 294.9 Cognitive Disorder NOS Mental Disorders Due to a General Medical Condition Not Elsewhere Classified 293.89 Catatonic Disorder Due to . . . [Indicate the General Medical Condition] 310.1
Personality Change Due to . . . [Indicate the General Medical Condition] Specify type: Labile Type/Disinhibited Type/Aggressive Type/Apathetic Type/ Paranoid Type/Other Type/Combined Type/Unspecified Type
293.9
Mental Disorder NOS Due to . . . [Indicate the General Medical Condition]
Substance-Related Disorders The following specifiers apply to Substance Dependence as noted: a
With Physiological Dependence/Without Physiological Dependence b Early Full Remission/Early Partial Remission/ Sustained Full Remission/Sustained Partial Remission c In a Controlled Environment d On Agonist Therapy The following specifiers apply to Substance-Induced Disorders as noted: I
With Onset During Intoxication/WWith Onset During Withdrawal
ALCOHOL-RELATED DISORDERS Alcohol Use Disorders 303.90 Alcohol Dependencea,b,c 305.00 Alcohol Abuse Alcohol-Induced Disorders 303.00 Alcohol Intoxication 291.81 Alcohol Withdrawal Specify if: With Perceptual Disturbances
291.0 291.0 291.2 291.1
Alcohol Intoxication Delirium Alcohol Withdrawal Delirium Alcohol-Induced Persisting Dementia Alcohol-Induced Persisting Amnestic Disorder
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
6
TABLE 1–1. 291.x .5 .3 291.89 291.89 291.89 291.89 291.9
DSM-IV-TR classification (partial listing) (continued)
Alcohol-Induced Psychotic Disorder With DelusionsI,W With HallucinationsI,W Alcohol-Induced Mood DisorderI,W Alcohol-Induced Anxiety DisorderI,W Alcohol-Induced Sexual DysfunctionI Alcohol-Induced Sleep DisorderI,W Alcohol-Related Disorder NOS
AMPHETAMINE (OR AMPHETAMINE-LIKE)–RELATED DISORDERS Amphetamine Use Disorders 304.40 Amphetamine Dependencea,b,c 305.70 Amphetamine Abuse Amphetamine-Induced Disorders 292.89 Amphetamine Intoxication Specify if: With Perceptual Disturbances
292.0 292.81 292.xx .11 .12 292.84 292.89 292.89 292.89 292.9
Amphetamine Withdrawal Amphetamine Intoxication Delirium Amphetamine-Induced Psychotic Disorder With DelusionsI With HallucinationsI Amphetamine-Induced Mood DisorderI,W Amphetamine-Induced Anxiety DisorderI Amphetamine-Induced Sexual DysfunctionI Amphetamine-Induced Sleep DisorderI,W Amphetamine-Related Disorder NOS
CAFFEINE-RELATED DISORDERS Caffeine-Induced Disorders 305.90 Caffeine Intoxication 292.89 Caffeine-Induced Anxiety DisorderI 292.89 Caffeine-Induced Sleep DisorderI 292.9 Caffeine-Related Disorder NOS CANNABIS-RELATED DISORDERS Cannabis Use Disorders 304.30 Cannabis Dependencea,b,c 305.20 Cannabis Abuse Cannabis-Induced Disorders 292.89 Cannabis Intoxication Specify if: With Perceptual Disturbances
292.81 Cannabis Intoxication Delirium
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
TABLE 1–1.
DSM-IV-TR classification (partial listing) (continued)
292.xx Cannabis-Induced Psychotic Disorder .11 With DelusionsI .12 With HallucinationsI 292.89 Cannabis-Induced Anxiety DisorderI 292.9 Cannabis-Related Disorder NOS COCAINE-RELATED DISORDERS Cocaine Use Disorders 304.20 Cocaine Dependencea,b,c 305.60 Cocaine Abuse Cocaine-Induced Disorders 292.89 Cocaine Intoxication Specify if: With Perceptual Disturbances
292.0 292.81 292.xx .11 .12 292.84 292.89 292.89 292.89 292.9
Cocaine Withdrawal Cocaine Intoxication Delirium Cocaine-Induced Psychotic Disorder With DelusionsI With HallucinationsI Cocaine-Induced Mood DisorderI,W Cocaine-Induced Anxiety DisorderI,W Cocaine-Induced Sexual DysfunctionI Cocaine-Induced Sleep DisorderI,W Cocaine-Related Disorder NOS
HALLUCINOGEN-RELATED DISORDERS Hallucinogen Use Disorders 304.50 Hallucinogen Dependenceb,c 305.30 Hallucinogen Abuse Hallucinogen-Induced Disorders 292.89 Hallucinogen Intoxication 292.89 Hallucinogen Persisting Perception Disorder (Flashbacks) 292.81 Hallucinogen Intoxication Delirium 292.xx Hallucinogen-Induced Psychotic Disorder .11 With DelusionsI .12 With HallucinationsI 292.84 Hallucinogen-Induced Mood DisorderI 292.89 Hallucinogen-Induced Anxiety DisorderI 292.9 Hallucinogen-Related Disorder NOS INHALANT-RELATED DISORDERS Inhalant Use Disorders 304.60 Inhalant Dependenceb,c 305.90 Inhalant Abuse
7
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
8
TABLE 1–1.
DSM-IV-TR classification (partial listing) (continued)
Inhalant-Induced Disorders 292.89 Inhalant Intoxication 292.81 Inhalant Intoxication Delirium 292.82 Inhalant-Induced Persisting Dementia 292.xx Inhalant-Induced Psychotic Disorder .11 With DelusionsI .12 With HallucinationsI 292.84 Inhalant-Induced Mood DisorderI 292.89 Inhalant-Induced Anxiety DisorderI 292.9 Inhalant-Related Disorder NOS NICOTINE-RELATED DISORDERS Nicotine Use Disorder 305.1 Nicotine Dependencea,b Nicotine-Induced Disorder 292.0 Nicotine Withdrawal 292.9 Nicotine-Related Disorder NOS OPIOID-RELATED DISORDERS Opioid Use Disorders 304.00 Opioid Dependencea,b,c,d 305.50 Opioid Abuse Opioid-Induced Disorders 292.89 Opioid Intoxication Specify if: With Perceptual Disturbances
292.0 292.81 292.xx .11 .12 292.84 292.89 292.89 292.9
Opioid Withdrawal Opioid Intoxication Delirium Opioid-Induced Psychotic Disorder With DelusionsI With HallucinationsI Opioid-Induced Mood DisorderI Opioid-Induced Sexual DysfunctionI Opioid-Induced Sleep DisorderI,W Opioid-Related Disorder NOS
PHENCYCLIDINE (OR PHENCYCLIDINE-LIKE)–RELATED DISORDERS Phencyclidine Use Disorders 304.60 Phencyclidine Dependenceb,c 305.90 Phencyclidine Abuse
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
TABLE 1–1.
DSM-IV-TR classification (partial listing) (continued)
Phencyclidine-Induced Disorders 292.89 Phencyclidine Intoxication Specify if: With Perceptual Disturbances
292.81 292.xx .11 .12 292.84 292.89 292.9
Phencyclidine Intoxication Delirium Phencyclidine-Induced Psychotic Disorder With DelusionsI With HallucinationsI Phencyclidine-Induced Mood DisorderI Phencyclidine-Induced Anxiety DisorderI Phencyclidine-Related Disorder NOS
SEDATIVE-, HYPNOTIC-, OR ANXIOLYTIC-RELATED DISORDERS Sedative, Hypnotic, or Anxiolytic Use Disorders 304.10 Sedative, Hypnotic, or Anxiolytic Dependencea,b,c 305.40 Sedative, Hypnotic, or Anxiolytic Abuse Sedative-, Hypnotic-, or Anxiolytic-Induced Disorders 292.89 Sedative, Hypnotic, or Anxiolytic Intoxication 292.0 Sedative, Hypnotic, or Anxiolytic Withdrawal Specify if: With Perceptual Disturbances
292.81 292.81 292.82 292.83 292.xx .11 .12 292.84 292.89 292.89 292.89 292.9
Sedative, Hypnotic, or Anxiolytic Intoxication Delirium Sedative, Hypnotic, or Anxiolytic Withdrawal Delirium Sedative-, Hypnotic-, or Anxiolytic-Induced Persisting Sedative-, Hypnotic-, or Anxiolytic-Induced Persisting Amnestic Disorder Sedative-, Hypnotic-, or Anxiolytic-Induced Psychotic Disorder With DelusionsI,W With HallucinationsI,W Sedative-, Hypnotic-, or Anxiolytic-Induced Mood DisorderI,W Sedative-, Hypnotic-, or Anxiolytic-Induced Anxiety DisorderW Sedative-, Hypnotic-, or Anxiolytic-Induced Sexual DysfunctionI Sedative-, Hypnotic-, or Anxiolytic-Induced Sleep DisorderI,W Sedative-, Hypnotic-, or Anxiolytic-Related Disorder NOS
POLYSUBSTANCE-RELATED DISORDER 304.80 Polysubstance Dependencea,b,c,d OTHER (OR UNKNOWN) SUBSTANCE–RELATED DISORDERS Other (or Unknown) Substance Use Disorders 304.90 Other (or Unknown) Substance Dependencea,b,c,d 305.90 Other (or Unknown) Substance Abuse
9
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
10
TABLE 1–1.
DSM-IV-TR classification (partial listing) (continued)
Other (or Unknown) Substance–Induced Disorders 292.89 Other (or Unknown) Substance Intoxication Specify if: With Perceptual Disturbances
292.0
Other (or Unknown) Substance Withdrawal Specify if: With Perceptual Disturbances
292.81 292.82 292.83 292.xx .11 .12 292.84 292.89 292.89 292.89 292.9
Other (or Unknown) Substance–Induced Delirium Other (or Unknown) Substance–Induced Persisting Dementia Other (or Unknown) Substance–Induced Persisting Amnestic Disorder Other (or Unknown) Substance–Induced Psychotic Disorder With DelusionsI,W With HallucinationsI,W Other (or Unknown) Substance–Induced Mood DisorderI,W Other (or Unknown) Substance–Induced Anxiety DisorderI,W Other (or Unknown) Substance–Induced Sexual DysfunctionI Other (or Unknown) Substance–Induced Sleep DisorderI,W Other (or Unknown) Substance–Related Disorder NOS
instances in which several disorders affecting mental function coexist. Persons with cognitive impairment often become delirious. Major depression may coexist with dementia (Weiner et al. 1991, 1993). Persons with Down syndrome often develop Alzheimer’s disease (Crapper et al. 1975). Also, a dementing illness may be engrafted on a preexisting psychiatric disorder such as schizophrenia or bipolar disorder. A crucial diagnostic differentiation is between early pathological cognitive decline and normal aging. This is the question raised by many persons who detect changes in their cognitive functioning and is of paramount significance in early clinical intervention.
Normal Aging The cognitive changes of aging vary between individuals, but in general, young and old adults perform differently on problem-solving tasks. On concept-identification tasks, old persons use less efficient strategies, are less successful at finding solutions, make more errors, and are less likely to change their strategy when their responses are incorrect. Older adults also use less sophisticated strategies on classification and categorization tasks (Reese and Rodeheaver 1985). Older adults recall as well as younger adults the gist of material with which they are presented, but recall details less well (Ulatowska et al. 1998). Because they rely on their general world knowledge to supplement their memory, older adults are also more prone to errors in recall.
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
11
Older adults often complain of memory difficulties: 83% report forgetting names frequently; 60% report losing objects such as keys; and 57% report forgetting telephone numbers that were just checked (Bolla et al. 1991). Studies of memory function assessed by digit span show little change in immediate or primary memory with age (Drachman and Leavitt 1972). Encoding and retrieval from recent (secondary) memory appear to decline with normal aging, especially when the information cannot be placed in context and seems irrelevant or nonsensical (Schludermann et al. 1983), but vocabulary, general information, and recall of past historical or personal events (tertiary memory) remain relatively intact (Poon 1985). A cross-sectional study of cognitively normal individuals ranging from 62 to 100 years of age showed that learning ability decreased with advancing age (unrelated to education), but recall, adjusted for the amount of material initially learned, did not (Petersen et al. 1992). Thus, healthy elders’ memory is generally preserved for relevant, well-learned material, but the ability to process novel information declines. Slowing the presentation of new information helps normal elders; cueing helps them retrieve more effectively from recent memory (Derouesne and Lacomblez 2000). Neither memory aid is greatly helpful when a disease such as Alzheimer’s disease reaches the level of dementia. Thus, when asking for recall of three words—for example, apple, table, and penny—after a 5-minute period of distraction, mentioning fruit, furniture, and coins are associative memory aids that can help distinguish between normal aging changes and disease-related cognitive impairment.
Age-Associated Memory Impairment and Mild Cognitive Impairment Memory impairment is so troublesome for many persons over age 50 years that a specific syndrome of age-associated memory impairment (AAMI) has been proposed (Blackford and LaRue 1989; Crook et al. 1986), but it has not achieved wide acceptance. Criteria for AAMI begin with complaints of gradual memory loss in persons older than age 50. They include objective evidence of impairment on a standardized memory test (compared with the mean established for young adults), evidence of adequate intellectual function, and absence of dementia or any medical condition that could produce cognitive deterioration. AAMI is reported to have variable prevalence at different ages in elderly persons. A Finnish study showed a prevalence of approximately 40% in persons ages 60–70 years, with highest prevalence in the younger persons sampled (Koivisto et al. 1995). AAMI and subjective memory complaints in elders are seen by some as a stable condition (Hel-
12
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
kala et al. 1997; Wang et al. 2000). Others find increased risk for the development of dementing illness (Ritchie et al. 2000) and also report that such complaints are frequently associated with physical illness and depression. By contrast, objective evidence of impaired free recall and response to cueing has been found to be associated with higher risk of the development of a dementing illness (Grober et al. 2000). As treatments for Alzheimer’s disease have been developed, there has been an effort to identify as potential subjects for intervention trials individuals who are at high risk for developing Alzheimer’s disease or individuals who are affected but do not yet meet criteria for dementia. This has led to the development of the diagnostic entity of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (Petersen et al. 1997). Individuals with MCI have complaints of abnormal memory; have normal activities of daily living and normal general cognitive function; have abnormal memory function for age; and do not meet criteria for dementia. They differ from persons with AAMI in that their memory impairment is more severe (Bowen et al. 1997). When observed for up to 5 years, 10%–15% of individuals with MCI developed Alzheimer’s disease each year—far more than the expected 1%–2% per year expected in the general population. A comparison of 76 MCI patients with 234 healthy control subjects and 106 persons with mild Alzheimer’s disease showed that MCI patients differed cognitively from control subjects only in memory. They were similar in memory function to early Alzheimer’s disease patients but differed from them in that only one area of cognition (memory) was affected (Petersen et al. 1999). Therefore, it appears that these individuals are probably in the earliest stage of clinically detectable Alzheimer’s disease. This group of individuals is discussed further in Chapters 5 and 8. In my experience, the most sensitive tests for incipient Alzheimer’s disease include impaired concentration and impaired recent and remote memory. Assessment of remote memory is confounded by educational level when using general knowledge questions such as names of past presidents of the United States or historical events. Remote memory is more effectively tested by gathering information from knowledgeable informants that is relevant to the persons being examined, such as family events or the number, names, and ages of grandchildren. Another sensitive indicator is loss of the abstract attitude—detected by the similarities and proverbs part of the mental status examination. Proverb interpretation is highly culture bound and may yield false-positive results in persons from other cultures. Performance on the similarities subtest, although not culture bound, is related strongly to education and premorbid intelligence. Early in the course of the illness, Alzheimer’s disease patients may also demonstrate difficulty with verbal fluency (e.g., name all the animals you can think of in the next
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
13
minute), object naming (dysnomia) and drawing geometrical figures (constructional dyspraxia). There are no clinical criteria for the prodromal stages of non-Alzheimer’s disease dementing illnesses.
Dementia The term dementia has a long history. It is derived from the Latin de (out of)+mens (mind)+ia (state of); its literal meaning is a state of being out of or deprived of one’s mind. The poet Juvenal, in the first or second century, is alleged to have used dementia in reference to the mental decrepitude of old age (Lipowski 1980). The term was used through the nineteenth century to designate mental impairment of many kinds (Thomas 1889). Pinel (1806/1962) used the term to designate one of the five classes of mental derangement. Although it is described as the derangement that abolishes the thinking faculty, dementia appeared to refer to schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders (Pinel 1806/1962, p. 165). Esquirol (1845/1965) used dementia to describe mental disorders that were manifested by weakened sensibility, understanding, and will, with impaired recent memory, attention, reasoning, and abstracting ability. Prichard (1837) suggested that dementia might be primary or secondary to other disorders and described a four-stage natural history of dementia: 1) loss of recent memory with preservation of remote memory; 2) loss of reason, 3) loss of comprehension, and 4) loss of ability to care for vegetative functions. Kraepelin (1913) distinguished the so-called functional psychoses from the consequences of obvious brain damage, calling the former the insanities and the latter varieties of imbecility. The two categories of imbecility were acquired imbecility (dementia) and congenital (ordinary) imbecility. The dementia category included apoplectic dementia due to vascular disease, old age (or senility), and epilepsy. Dementia is a diagnosis of impaired mental function. Thus, the clinical history and the mental status examination are the most important tools in making the diagnosis. Dementia cannot be diagnosed on the basis of radiological, biochemical, genetic, or psychological tests alone. Such tests serve to confirm the presence of altered brain function and to help determine the cause of the dementia syndrome. As indicated previously, individuals with dementing illnesses may not meet formal criteria for dementia early on. Dementia is defined as a reduction or impairment of multiple cognitive abilities, including memory, sufficient to interfere with self-maintenance, work, or social relationships. This intellectual impairment occurs without clouding of consciousness and may or may not be permanent. The diagnosis is based on the history (usually supplied by informants other than the
14
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
patient) and clinical examination. At times, the history and clinical examination will suggest the nature of the dementing illness, as in the case of a person who has experienced multiple strokes and whose cognitive decline was related in time to the strokes. At other times, extensive interdisciplinary evaluation and laboratory procedures are required to determine the cause of dementia. On rare occasions, brain biopsy is needed for a definitive diagnosis. The diagnosis of dementia is complicated by the enormous variation between individuals. For example, many persons who have declined cognitively may still function at a level comparable to an average person their own age. Therefore, the clinician must compare a person’s present abilities with his or her own past abilities. However, this can usually be accomplished only by the retrospective accounts furnished by patients and their families—accounts that are subject to strong bias. For example, family members often minimize deficits by stating that a loved one who appears impaired on clinical examination was never interested in reading or in current events. Individual family members’ biases are minimized through the use of multiple informants. However, with the use of multiple informants and standardized instruments such as the Dementia Questionnaire (see Appendix A) and the Blessed Dementia Rating Scale (BDRS) (see Appendix D), accurate data can be obtained. The fundamental DSM-IV-TR criteria for dementia are presented in Table 1–2 and include the clinical means to elicit the diagnostic signs and symptoms. Table 1–3 indicates the criteria for diagnosing specific dementias. In DSM-IV-TR, the dementia syndrome is not regarded as either progressive or irreversible. There have been optimistic reports on the frequency of reversible conditions producing the dementia syndrome (Garcia et al. 1981; Marsden and Harrison 1972). Mild cognitive impairment, especially due to medications or metabolic disorders, is frequently reversible, but a full-blown dementia syndrome is rarely reversible. In a review of 32 studies of 2,889 cognitively impaired patients with a mean age of 72 years, Clairfield (1988) found clinical diagnoses of Alzheimer’s disease in 56.8%, vascular dementia in 13.3%, depression in 4.5%, alcohol-induced dementia in 4.2%, and drug use in 1.5% of the patients evaluated. Potentially reversible causes of dementia accounted for 13.2% of the cases, but not all cases of potentially reversible dementia were followed to see whether reversal actually occurred. In the 11 studies that provided follow-up, 8% of the dementias resolved partially, and only 3% resolved fully. Reversible causes of dementia included drug use (28%), depression (26%), and toxic-metabolic problems (15.5%). Katzman (1992) suggested that treatable dementia-producing conditions account for about 10.5% of cases and include neurosyphilis, fungal
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
TABLE 1–2.
15
General diagnostic criteria for dementia based on DSM-IV-TR criteria
A. The development of multiple cognitive deficits manifested by both (1) memory impairment (impaired ability to learn new information and to recall previously learned information) (a) Primary memory can be assessed by digit span forward and in reverse, with a discrepancy of three digits or more suggesting impairment. Secondary memory can be tested by asking the examinee to recall three words presented by the examiner after an interval of 5 minutes. Secondary memory can also be tested by presenting three objects without naming them, covering them up, and asking the examinee to name them 5 minutes later. Another test of short-term memory is to read a short paragraph aloud to the examinee and then ask the examinee to tell what he or she recalls. (b) Long-term or tertiary memory is tested by asking for personal information that can be validated by the accompanying person (date of birth, graduation from high school, marriage, etc.) and by asking facts of common knowledge compatible with the examinee's education and cultural background, including questions such as the name of the president of the United States, the immediate past presidents, the state capital, or the location of the U.S. Capitol. (2) one (or more) of the following cognitive disturbances: (a) aphasia (language disturbance) including, in addition to the classic aphasias, difficulty with word finding and confrontational naming. Word finding difficulty is evidenced in advanced dementia by empty speech devoid of nouns and verbs, with relative preservation of socially overlearned speech, such as “How are you?” Earlier, it can be demonstrated by asking the evaluee to name as many animals as possible in 1 minute. Dementia patients will typically name fewer than 10 animals and will often repeat names. They also have difficulty naming the parts of a watch (watchband, stem, back, crystal), making paraphasic errors (such as strap for band or lens for crystal), or describing functions (e.g., “it's how you set it” for watch stem) instead. (b) apraxia (inability to carry out motor activities despite intact motor function; e.g., strength and coordination), i.e., to draw the face of a clock and set the hands at 10 minutes after 11 (Sunderland et al. 1989). (c) agnosia (failure to recognize or identify objects despite intact sensory function) (d) disturbance in executive functioning (i.e., planning, organizing, sequencing, abstracting).
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
16
TABLE 1–2.
General diagnostic criteria for dementia based on DSM-IV-TR criteria (continued)
1. Impaired planning, organizing, and sequencing are indicated by an ability to deal with interpersonal, family, and employmentrelated issues and to describe logically how they might be dealt with. Changes in long-standing habits and personal hygiene may reflect executive dysfunction. The history is the best source of information about executive functioning, but it may also be evaluated by posing problems that individuals might encounter in daily life, such as an overdrawn bank account or a medical emergency. Executive functioning can be additionally assessed by asking examinees to perform serial tasks, such as going through the steps of mailing a letter (i.e., folding the paper, inserting it into an envelope, addressing the envelope, placing a stamp on it, and sealing it). 2. Impaired abstracting ability is evidenced by inability to abstractly categorize the similarity between objects such as a chair and a table, or a knife and a fork, or for highly educated persons, between a poem and a statue or praise and punishment. Impaired abstracting ability is also evidenced by inability to interpret abstractly common proverbs such as “don’t cry over spilled milk” or “the grass always looks greener on the other side of the street.” (See Appendix B for detailed mental status examination.) B. The cognitive deficits in Criteria A1 and A2 each cause significant impairment in social or occupational functioning and represent a significant decline from a previous level of functioning. C. The deficits do not occur exclusively during the course of delirium.
infections, tumor, alcohol abuse, subdural hematoma, normal-pressure hydrocephalus, and epilepsy. On the other hand, according to Katzman, reversible dementias, including drug toxicity, metabolic disorders, hyponatremia, vitamin B12 deficiency, hypothyroidism, and hypoglycemia account for only 4.7% of cases.
Delirium Delirium is a state of altered consciousness and cognition, usually of acute onset (hours or days) and of brief duration (days or weeks). Delirium is very common in general hospital patients, with estimates of prevalence ranging from 30% to 50% in patients age 70 years or older (Lipowski 1987). In a prospective study of 225 nonconfused individuals age 65 years or older who were undergoing repair of hip fracture or elective hip replacement surgery, DSM-IV-TR delirium was diagnosed in 20%. The incidence was 24% in the group with hip fractures and only 12% in those undergoing elective hip
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
TABLE 1–3.
17
DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for specific dementia syndromes
Dementia of the Alzheimer’s Type A. The development of multiple cognitive deficits manifested by both (1) memory impairment (impaired ability to learn new information and to recall previously learned information) (2) one (or more) of the following cognitive disturbances: (a) aphasia (language disturbance) (b) apraxia (impaired ability to carry out motor activities despite intact motor function) (c) agnosia (failure to recognize or identify objects despite intact sensory function) (d) disturbance in executive functioning (i.e., planning, organizing, sequencing, abstracting) B. The cognitive deficits in Criteria A1 and A2 each cause significant impairment in social or occupational functioning and represent a significant decline from a previous level of functioning. C. The course is characterized by gradual onset and continuing cognitive decline. D. The cognitive deficits in Criteria A1 and A2 are not due to any of the following: (1) central nervous system conditions that cause progressive deficits in memory and cognition (e.g., cerebrovascular disease, Parkinson’s disease, Huntington’s disease, subdural hematoma, normal-pressure hydrocephalus, brain tumor) (2) systemic conditions that are known to cause dementia (e.g., hypothyroidism, vitamin B12 or folic acid deficiency, niacin deficiency, hypercalcemia, neurosyphilis, HIV infection) (3) substance-induced conditions E. The deficits do not occur exclusively during the course of a delirium. F. The disturbance is not better accounted for by another Axis I disorder (e.g., major depressive disorder, schizophrenia). Vascular Dementia A.–B. See A and B under Dementia of the Alzheimer’s Type. C. Focal neurological signs and symptoms (e.g., exaggeration of deep tendon reflexes, extensor plantar response, pseudobulbar palsy, gait abnormalities, weakness of an extremity) or laboratory evidence indicative of cerebral vascular disease (e.g., multiple infarctions involving cortex and underlying white matter) that are judged to be etiologically related to the disturbance. D. The deficits do not occur exclusively during the course of a delirium. Dementia Due to Other General Medical Conditions A.–B. See A and B under Dementia of the Alzheimer’s Type.
18
TABLE 1–3.
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for specific dementia syndromes (continued)
C. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the disturbance is the direct physiological consequence of a general medical condition other than Alzheimer’s disease or cerebrovascular disease (e.g., HIV infection, traumatic brain injury, Parkinson’s disease, Huntington’s disease, Pick’s disease, Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, normalpressure hydrocephalus, hypothyroidism, brain tumor, or vitamin B12 deficiency). Substance-Induced Persisting Dementia A.–B. See A and B under Dementia of the Alzheimer’s Type. C. The deficits do not occur exclusively during the course of a delirium and persist beyond the usual duration of substance intoxication or withdrawal. D. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the deficits are etiologically related to the persisting effects of substance use (e.g., a drug of abuse, a medication). Dementia Due to Multiple Etiologies A.–B. See A and B under Dementia of the Alzheimer’s Type. C. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the disturbance has more than one etiology (e.g., head trauma plus chronic alcohol use, dementia of the Alzheimer’s type with the subsequent development of vascular dementia). D. The deficits do not occur exclusively during the course of a delirium. Dementia Not Otherwise Specified This category should be used to diagnose a dementia that does not meet criteria for any of the specific types described in this section. An example is a clinical presentation of dementia for which there is insufficient evidence to establish a specific etiology.
replacement; the former group probably had greater baseline cognitive impairment (Duppils and Wikblad 2000). The onset of delirium was postoperative in 96% of patients and generally resolved within 48 hours. Predisposing factors were older age, cognitive impairment, and preexisting brain disease. Impaired attention is the sine qua non of delirium, which is usually accompanied by fluctuating consciousness. Sleep-wake disturbances are common, and there is often reduced or increased psychomotor activity. In a series of 227 patients with DSM-IV-TR delirium, 27% experienced visual hallucinations, 12.4% auditory hallucinations, and 2.7% tactile hallucinations (Webster and Holroyd 2000). Delirium is usually characterized by generalized slow waves in the electroencephalogram (EEG); the course of delirium can be followed with serial EEGs (Engel and Romano 1959). The DSM-IV-TR criteria for delirium are presented in Table 1–4.
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
TABLE 1–4.
19
DSM-IV-TR criteria for delirium
Delirium Due to … [Indicate the General Medical Condition] A. Disturbance of consciousness (i.e., reduced clarity of awareness of the environment) with reduced ability to focus, sustain, or shift attention. B. Change in cognition (such as memory deficit, disorientation, language disturbance) or the development of a perceptual disturbance that is not better accounted for by a preexisting, established, or evolving dementia. C. The disturbance develops over a short period of time (usually hours to days) and tends to fluctuate during the course of the day. D. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the disturbance is caused by the direct physiological consequences of a general medical condition. Substance Intoxication Delirium A.–C. See A–C under Delirium Due to … [Indicate the General Medical Condition] D. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings of either (1) or (2): (1) the symptoms in Criteria A and B developed during substance intoxication (2) medication use is etiologically related to the disturbance Substance Withdrawal Delirium A.–C. See A–C under Delirium Due to … [Indicate the General Medical Condition] D. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the symptoms in Criteria A and B developed during, or shortly after, a withdrawal syndrome. Delirium Due to Multiple Etiologies A.–C. See A–C under Delirium Due to … [Indicate the General Medical Condition] D. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the delirium has more than one etiology (e.g., more than one etiological general medical condition, a general medical condition plus substance intoxication or medication side effect). Delirium Not Otherwise Specified Examples include 1. A clinical presentation of delirium that is suspected to be due to a general medical condition or substance use but for which there is insufficient evidence to establish specific etiology 2. Delirium due to causes not listed above (e.g., sensory deprivation)
Dementia and delirium frequently coexist, with dementia serving as a predisposing factor (Gustafson et al. 1988). In many individuals, the first sign of a dementing illness is postoperative delirium. Delirium differs from dementia in that delirium has a rapid onset and has a greater degree of personality disorganization and clouding of consciousness. Fluctuating cognitive ability occurs in dementia but not to the extent that it occurs in delirium. Dementia patients usually give their best cognitive performance
20
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
early in the day when they are not fatigued, under circumstances in which they do not feel challenged or anxious. Toward the end of the day, many cognitively impaired persons become transiently delirious, a phenomenon often referred to as sundowning. Although sundowning is often attributed to the waning of sensory cues, Volicer et al. (2001) have suggested that in Alzheimer’s disease, the mechanism of sundowning may be a disturbance of circadian rhythm related to a phase delay in body temperature. The diagnosis of dementia cannot be made in the presence of delirium; the patient must be clear of the acute disturbance.
Amnestic Disorder The primary manifestation of amnestic disorder is memory impairment. The DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for this uncommon disorder are presented in Table 1–5. The most common cause is thiamine deficiency associated with long-term alcohol abuse. Persons with Korsakoff’s syndrome (in DSM-IV-TR called substance-induced persisting amnestic disorder due to alcohol) differ from those with Alzheimer’s disease. The former may recall rules and principles for organizing information and have access to previously acquired knowledge with impairment of recent memory, whereas the latter may have little access to previously acquired information and may therefore have difficulty encoding ongoing events (Weingartner et al. 1983). Patients with amnestic disorder often meet criteria for dementia, with the exception of patients with brief amnestic episodes that occur with the short-acting benzodiazepines lorazepam and triazolam. Bilateral lesions of the hippocampus appear to impair recent memory without impairing remote memory (Scoville and Milner 1957; Zola-Morgan et al. 1986). The importance of considering amnestic disorders in differential diagnosis is that they are reversible in the case of minor tranquilizers and are partly reversible in thiamine deficiency–based Wernicke’s encephalopathy seen in alcoholic patients or in nutritionally deprived persons.
Psychotic Disorder Due to a General Medical Condition The DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for psychotic disorder due to a general medical condition are presented in Table 1–6. Hallucinations and delusions are the most common symptoms of this disorder. The following case, which is probably a combination of Alzheimer’s disease with other medical problems, is typical.
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
TABLE 1–5.
21
DSM-IV-TR criteria for amnestic disorder
Amnestic Disorder Due to... [Indicate the General Medical Condition] A. The development of memory impairment as manifested by impairment in the ability to learn new information or the inability to recall previously learned information. B. The memory disturbance causes significant impairment in social or occupational functioning and represents a significant decline from a previous level of functioning. C. The memory disturbance does not occur exclusively during the course of a delirium or a dementia. D. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the disturbance is the direct physiological consequence of a general medical condition (including physical trauma). Substance-Induced Persisting Amnestic Disorder A.–B. See A and B under Amnestic Disorder Due to … [Indicate the General Medical Condition] C. The memory disturbance does not occur exclusively during the course of a delirium or a dementia and persists beyond the usual duration of substance intoxication or withdrawal. D. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the memory disturbance is etiologically related to the persisting effects of substance use (e.g., a drug of abuse, a medication). Amnestic Disorder Not Otherwise Specified An example is a clinical presentation of amnesia for which there is insufficient evidence to establish a specific etiology (i.e., dissociative, substance induced, or due to a general medical condition).
Mrs. M. was an 84-year-old woman who had been delusional for 3 years. Her delusions seemingly began when her memory started to fail. She insisted that her husband was consorting with prostitutes and that he was trying to drive her crazy so that he could get rid of her. Mrs. M. had been jealous of her husband for nearly all of their 58-year marriage, beginning a year after the birth of their daughter, when he confessed a marital infidelity. He continued to be unfaithful, and she continued to accuse him of having affairs. In the 3 years prior to her evaluation, she developed increasing difficulty with her memory. Her emotions became increasingly labile, and at times she threatened her husband with physical harm. She was eating poorly and had reversed her sleep-wake cycle. She was hospitalized for evaluation, and physical examination revealed a cachectic woman with marked hearing impairment and marked visual impairment due to cataracts. Laboratory testing showed macrocytic anemia, a vitamin B12 level of 79 pg/mL, a folic acid level of 1.3 ng/mL, and low serum iron concentration. Mental status examination showed impaired concentration, impaired recent and remote memory, and a markedly reduced fund of information. Her MMSE score was 14. She became more tractable in a structured environment where she received an adequate diet, multiple
22
TABLE 1–6.
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
DSM-IV-TR criteria for mental disorders due to a general medical condition
Psychotic Disorder Due to...[Indicate the General Medical Condition] A. Prominent hallucinations or delusions. B. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the disturbance is the direct physiological consequence of a general medical condition. C. The disturbance is not better accounted for by another mental disorder. D. The disturbance does not occur exclusively during the course of a delirium. Mood Disorder Due to...[Indicate the General Medical Condition] A. A prominent and persistent disturbance in mood predominates in the clinical picture and is characterized by either (or both) of the following: (1) depressed mood or markedly diminished interest or pleasure in all, or almost all, activities (2) elevated, expansive, or irritable mood B. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the disturbance is the direct physiological consequence of a general medical condition. C. The disturbance is not better accounted for by another mental disorder (e.g., adjustment disorder with depressed mood in response to the stress of having a general medical condition). D. The disturbance does not occur exclusively during the course of a delirium. E. The symptoms cause clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning. Anxiety Disorder Due to...[Indicate the General Medical Condition] A. Prominent anxiety, panic attacks, or obsessions or compulsions predominate in the clinical picture. B. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the disturbance is the direct physiological consequence of a general medical condition. C. The disturbance is not better accounted for by another mental disorder (e.g., adjustment disorder with anxiety in which the stressor is a serious general medical condition). D.–E. See Criteria D and E under Mood Disorder Due to … [Indicate the General Medical Condition]. Catatonic Disorder Due to...[Indicate the General Medical Condition] A. The presence of catatonia as manifested by motoric immobility, excessive motor activity (that is apparently purposeless and not influenced by external stimuli), extreme negativism or mutism, peculiarities of voluntary movement, or echolalia or echopraxia. B. B. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the disturbance is the direct physiological consequence of a general medical condition.
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
TABLE 1–6.
23
DSM-IV-TR criteria for mental disorders due to a general medical condition (continued)
C. The disturbance is not better accounted for by another mental disorder (e.g., a manic episode). D. The disturbance does not occur exclusively during the course of a delirium. Personality Change Due to...[Indicate the General Medical Condition] A. A persistent personality disturbance that represents a change from the individual’s previous characteristic personality pattern. (In children, the disturbance involves a marked deviation from normal development or a significant change in the child’s usual behavior patterns lasting at least 1 year). B. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the disturbance is the direct physiological consequence of a general medical condition. C. The disturbance is not better accounted for by another mental disorder (including other mental disorders due to a general medical condition). D. The disturbance does not occur exclusively during the course of a delirium. E. The disturbance causes clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning. Mental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified Due to a General Medical Condition This residual category should be used for situations in which it has been established that the disturbance is caused by the direct physiological effects of a general medical condition, but the criteria are not met for a specific mental disorder due to a general medical condition (e.g., dissociative symptoms due to complex partial seizures).
vitamins with iron, and parenteral vitamin B12, but she was transferred to a nursing home because it was believed that her husband could not manage her care adequately from a physical standpoint.
Mood Disorder Due to a General Medical Condition Mood disorder due to a general medical condition and substance-induced mood disorder can be confused with dementia because of the many overlapping signs of both dementia and depression. The essential feature of this disorder is prominent and persistent mood alteration associated with a general medical condition. Carcinoma of the pancreas, viral illness, and stroke can cause depression. Hyperthyroidism and hypothyroidism and hyperadrenocorticism and hypoadrenocorticism can cause depression or mania. The DSM-IV-TR criteria for mood disorder due to a general medical condition are listed in Table 1–6. This disorder usually remits when the underlying cause is treated.
24
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Substance-Induced Mood Disorder Substance-induced mood disorder is characterized by prominent and persistent mood alteration associated with substance use. Depressive symptoms may be caused by drugs, including reserpine, methyldopa, >-blockers, and some hallucinogens. Exogenous steroids can cause depression or mania. This condition usually remits when the causative agent is withdrawn. The DSM-IV-TR criteria for substance-induced mood disorder are listed in Table 1–7. TABLE 1–7.
DSM-IV-TR criteria for substance-induced mood disorder
A. A prominent and persistent disturbance in mood predominates in the clinical picture and is characterized by either (or both) of the following: (1) depressed mood or markedly diminished interest or pleasure in all, or almost all, activities (2) elevated, expansive, or irritable mood B. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings of either (1) or (2): (1) the symptoms in Criterion A developed during, or within a month of, Substance Intoxication or Withdrawal (2) medication use is etiologically related to the disturbance C. The disturbance is not better accounted for by a Mood Disorder that is not substance induced. Evidence that the symptoms are better accounted for by a Mood Disorder that is not substance induced might include the following: the symptoms precede the onset of the substance use (or medication use); the symptoms persist for a substantial period of time (e.g., about a month) after the cessation of acute withdrawal or severe intoxication or are substantially in excess of what would be expected given the type or amount of the substance used or the duration of use; or there is other evidence that suggests the existence of an independent non-substance-induced Mood Disorder (e.g., a history of recurrent Major Depressive Episodes). D. The disturbance does not occur exclusively during the course of a delirium. E. The symptoms cause clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning.
Anxiety Disorder Due to a General Medical Condition Generalized anxiety or recurrent panic attacks are the chief characteristics of anxiety disorders due to a general medical condition, which characteristically impair concentration. Endocrine disorders such as hyperthyroidism and hypothyroidism, pheochromocytoma, hypercortisolism, and fasting
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
25
hypoglycemia are potential causative factors, along with a host of others. DSM-IV-TR criteria for anxiety disorder due to a general medical condition are listed in Table 1–6. Many persons with dementia become distressed or anxious when their cognitive and adaptive abilities are challenged. Often, the distress or anxiety is expressed through reiteration of physical complaints when the individual is intellectually challenged or emotionally stressed. Overt dysphoria is most common early in the course of a dementing illness. As dementing illnesses progress, anxiety often appears to diminish. This may occur because the ability to anticipate real or symbolic danger becomes impaired or because patients’ ability to communicate anxiety diminishes.
Personality Change Due to a General Medical Condition The greatest impact of a general medical condition may be exaggeration of preexisting personality traits or a change in personality instead of obvious intellectual impairment. There are many patterns, but emotional instability, recurrent outbursts of aggression or rage, impaired social judgment, apathy, suspiciousness, and paranoid ideation are frequent. Brain tumors, head trauma, multiple sclerosis, frontotemporal degenerative diseases, and strokes are common causes of personality changes. These symptoms may also occur as interictal phenomena in temporal lobe epilepsy. The DSMIV-TR criteria for personality change due to a general medical condition are presented in Table 1–6. Although many such cases do not meet criteria for dementia, their functional deficits may be as severe as those of persons diagnosed with dementia, as illustrated in the following case: Mr. M., a 69-year-old man, was referred for evaluation by his wife and son. They complained that he talked to himself, did “silly things,” failed to maintain his personal hygiene, and did not pay his bills on time. Mr. M. was a college graduate who had worked at a military installation until he contracted viral encephalitis at age 44 years. He was comatose for a week and experienced residual weakness and slowness of motion. His personality changed. He became highly emotional and negativistic. At age 66 years, he suffered a right-hemisphere stroke that mildly weakened the left side of his body and impaired his speech for a short time. On examination, Mr. M. was poorly groomed. His speech and language were normal, but he tended to engage in long diatribes. He laughed at inappropriate times during the interview. Difficulty with concentration was detected with serial subtraction. Recent and remote memory appeared intact. His construction ability was good. He was able to abstractly categorize the similarity between an egg and a seed but was unable to interpret simple
26
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
proverbs. He had good knowledge of current events but had difficulty when asked to think of an appropriate course of action for dealing with a medical emergency. Neuropsychological testing revealed a verbal IQ of 117, a performance IQ of 94, and a full-scale score of 108—a performance surpassing 70% of his age peers. There was great scatter in his verbal intellectual abilities from below average to superior. He had difficulty with tasks requiring attention to verbally presented material. His perceptual motor abilities were much poorer, ranging from mentally defective to average. He had difficulty with sequencing social stimuli and with understanding part-whole relationships. He performed poorly on complex verbal problem solving and had mild to moderate difficulty with short- and long-term verbal memory. Language, communication, and constructional abilities were intact.
Other Cognitive Disorders Cognitive disorder not otherwise specified (NOS) is a diagnosis for cognitive dysfunction presumed to be due to the direct physiological effect of a general medical condition that does not meet criteria for delirium, dementia, or amnestic disorder. Examples include impairment in cognitive functioning as evidenced by neuropsychological testing or quantified clinical assessment, accompanied by objective evidence of a systemic illness or central nervous system dysfunction; postconcussion disorder; and impairment in memory or attention with associated symptoms after a head trauma. Many cases of postoperative cognitive dysfunction (POCD) fall into this category. Older adults often complain of deficits in cognitive function following major noncardiac surgery. In one prospective study employing measures of memory and attention, including cognitive speed and speed of general information processing, deficits were present at 1 week but largely gone at 3 months after surgery. However, after 6 months, 29% continued to complain of cognitive deficits (Dijkstra et al. 1999). A prospective study of POCD in 1,218 persons age 60 or older undergoing major noncardiac surgery under general anesthesia showed cognitive dysfunction in 26% at 1 week after surgery and in 10% at 3 months compared with control rates of 3.4% and 2.8%, respectively (Biedler et al. 1999). Risk factors for early POCD were increasing age and duration of anesthesia, little education, a second operation, postoperative infection, and respiratory complications. Only age was a risk factor for long-term POCD.
Mental Retardation Mental retardation must be considered in the evaluation for dementing illness. Except when there are obvious stigmata of a syndrome ordinarily
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
27
associated with mental retardation (such as in Down syndrome), it is often impossible to distinguish clinically between mental retardation and dementing illness. The examiner must rely instead on the patient’s history by an outside informant. That history usually includes academic failure with early dropout from school, or graduation from a special education program. Job history usually indicates limited skills and limited comprehension. In many instances, educational history can be deceptive. Graduation from an ungraded school, for example, while requiring 10–12 years, may be the equivalent of only a third- or fourth-grade education. A study of an educationally and socially deprived group showed a correlation of intelligence with educational level (Weiner and Lovitt 1984). However, half the studied individuals with more than 12 years of education had full-scale IQs of less than 85; half of the individuals with fewer than 12 years of education had IQs less than 75. A low level of schooling or limited intelligence markedly impairs performance in the fund of information, abstract reasoning, and judgment aspects of the mental status examination. Two important distinguishing features between developmental disorders and dementing illness are vocabulary and fund of information, which tend to be less impaired than other aspects of the mental status examination in dementing illnesses that do not primarily affect language function. A significant problem for those dealing with mentally retarded persons is the issue of cognitive decline in Down syndrome patients. Virtually all Down syndrome patients who live past age 35 years demonstrate the microscopic pathology of Alzheimer’s disease (reviewed by Mann 1988; Wisniewski et al. 1978). In 57 Down syndrome patients over age 30 years, 5year follow-up showed severe cognitive deterioration in 28% (Oliver et al. 1998). The rate of cognitive impairment increased with age and degree of preexisting cognitive impairment. It is important in this population, as in all others, to seek remediable causes of functional decline such as metabolic abnormalities and psychiatric disorders and not to assume that all functional worsening in these patients is due to Alzheimer’s disease.
Depression Depression is the most important psychiatric disorder to be considered in the diagnostic evaluation of a person with cognitive impairment, either as a primary diagnosis or as a complication of an underlying disease. The prevalence of depression in Alzheimer’s disease is highly controversial, largely due to the substantial overlap of depressive symptoms with the symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease. Most of the nine DSM-IV-TR criteria for major depression are seen in Alzheimer’s disease, including decreased
28
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
interest in activities, weight loss, insomnia or hypersomnia, loss of energy, and diminished ability to think or concentrate. The most important difficulty is that criteria for major depression do not require depressed mood. Thus, Lyketsos and Olin (2002) report 50% prevalence of depressive disorders in Alzheimer’s disease. It seems reasonable that distinct criteria be established for the diagnosis of major depression in Alzheimer’s disease. Weiner and Sairam (2000) have proposed that five or more of the following nine criteria be present for at least 1 week and nearly every day: sadness most of the day, no apparent enjoyment when engaged by others in ordinarily pleasurable activities (or active refusal), little interest in food when presented by others and helped to engage in eating, increased irritability, psychomotor agitation or retardation, low energy, feelings of worthlessness or crying, function below expectation for the level of cognitive impairment, and expressions of a positive wish to die and suicide plans or attempts. In my experience in a dementia clinic, depressive syndromes are a common cause of cognitive impairment in persons without demonstrable brain pathology, but major depression is a rare complication of Alzheimer’s disease. Of 317 consecutive patients in our dementia clinic, 19 (6%) had little or no evidence of brain disease or damage. In that group, 8 (42%) were thought to have depression-related cognitive impairment. In the 192 patients diagnosed as having probable or possible Alzheimer’s disease, only 4 (2%) were thought to be depressed; of those, only 2 (1%) met DSM-III-R criteria for major depression (Weiner et al. 1991). The low prevalence of major depression seen in dementia clinics was later reconfirmed by Weiner and Doody (2002). By contrast, there is a 20%–25% prevalence of major depression in the first 2 years after stroke (Robinson and Forrester 1987), and depression is also frequent in Parkinson’s disease (Mayeux et al. 1981). Many depressed persons have cognitive impairment, although the severity of their impairment does not always correspond with the severity of their depressive symptoms. Much attention has been paid to the phenomenon of pseudodementia (now described as the dementia of depression), a term attributed by Bleuler (1924) to Wernicke in the 1880s and later resurrected by Kiloh (1961). Cognitive processes susceptible to depression are attention, perception, speed of cognitive response, problem solving, memory, and learning. Depressed persons appear to use weak or incomplete strategies to encode events to be remembered. If depressed patients are provided organization and structure, memory deficits disappear (Weingartner et al. 1981). The cognitive and motor tasks most impaired are those requiring sustained effort (Cohen et al. 1982). Depressed persons tend to show impaired recent memory without impaired long-term memory. Short-term memory deficits are correctable by successful treatment with
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
29
antidepressants (Sternberg and Jarvik 1976). MMSE scores improved in depressed elders after successful treatment with antidepressants or electroconvulsive therapy (Greenwald et al. 1989). In a 12-week trial comparing two antidepressant drugs, Butters et al. (2000) found depressed elders as a group showed minimal improvement on the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (Mattis 1988). Those who had cognitive impairment at baseline had improvement in the domains of conceptualization and initiation relative to those of depressed patients with normal cognition, but they still remained mildly impaired. The response of both depressive and cognitive symptoms to antidepressant treatment does not firmly establish elderly patients’ sole diagnosis as depression. Of 23 patients who had amelioration of cognitive symptoms with treatment of their depression, nearly half went on to develop a dementing illness (Alexopoulos et al. 1993). Although depression is widely presumed to be an important source of comorbidity in Alzheimer’s disease, one small series of depressed Alzheimer’s disease patients showed no difference in free recall, recognition of slowly and rapidly presented words, and digit span (Fahlander et al. 1999). DSM-IV-TR criteria for a major depressive episode are presented in Table 1–8. The differentiation between the cognitive impairment of depression and that due to degenerative or metabolic brain disorder is based on the following: 1. Onset of depressive symptoms preceding cognitive impairment 2. Sudden, fairly recent (weeks or months), and often identifiable onset of cognitive impairment, in terms of both time and emotionally important life events (loss of job or spouse) 3. Patients’ emphasizing inability to think, concentrate, and remember 4. Signs and symptoms of depression 5. Objective cognitive testing showing patients’ deficits to be less severe than their complaints, with performance improved by encouragement, cueing, and structure 6. Depressed patients more commonly giving “I don’t know” answers in contrast to making near misses, confabulating, or repeating (perseverating) answers 7. Normal EEG 8. Absence of any condition known to affect brain function Radiological evidence of mild generalized brain atrophy is not helpful in differentiating depression from dementing illness in elderly persons; nor is the dexamethasone suppression test. The dexamethasone suppression test is positive in 40%–70% of patients with melancholic depression (Rubin and Poland 1984), but it is also frequently positive in patients with
30
TABLE 1–8.
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
DSM-IV-TR criteria for major depressive episode
A. Five (or more) of the following symptoms have been present during the same 2-week period and represent a change from previous functioning; at least one of the symptoms is either (1) depressed mood or (2) loss of interest or pleasure. Note: Do not include symptoms that are clearly due to a general medical condition, or mood-incongruent delusions or hallucinations. (1) depressed mood most of the day, nearly every day, as indicated by either subjective report (e.g., feels sad or empty) or observation made by others (e.g., appears tearful). Note: In children and adolescents, can be irritable mood. (2) markedly diminished interest or pleasure in all, or almost all, activities most of the day, nearly every day (as indicated by either subjective account or observation made by others) (3) significant weight loss when not dieting or weight gain (e.g., a change of more than 5% of body weight in a month), or decrease or increase in appetite nearly every day. Note: In children, consider failure to make expected weight gains. (4) insomnia or hypersomnia nearly every day (5) psychomotor agitation or retardation nearly every day (observable by others, not merely subjective feelings of restlessness or being slowed down) (6) fatigue or loss of energy nearly every day (7) feelings of worthlessness or excessive or inappropriate guilt (which may be delusional) nearly every day (not merely self-reproach or guilt about being sick) (8) diminished ability to think or concentrate, or indecisiveness, nearly every day (either by subjective account or as observed by others) (9) recurrent thoughts of death (not just fear of dying), recurrent suicidal ideation without a specific plan, or a suicide attempt or a specific plan for committing suicide B. The symptoms do not meet criteria for a Mixed Episode C. The symptoms cause clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning. D. The symptoms are not due to the direct physiological effects of a substance (e.g., a drug of abuse, a medication) or a general medical condition (e.g., hypothyroidism). E. The symptoms are not better accounted for by Bereavement, i.e., after the loss of a loved one, the symptoms persist for longer than 2 months or are characterized by marked functional impairment, morbid preoccupation with worthlessness, suicidal ideation, psychotic symptoms, or psychomotor retardation.
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
31
Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia (Balldin et al. 1983; Greenwald et al. 1986; Jenike and Albert 1984). As indicated in Chapter 8, neuropsychological testing can help distinguish depression from degenerative or metabolic brain disorder and can also identify depression in cognitively impaired patients.
Other Psychiatric Disorders Ganser syndrome is subsumed in DSM-IV-TR under the heading of Dissociative Disorder NOS. In this syndrome, ludicrous approximate answers or responses are made to simple questions or commands, indicating that the questions are clearly understood and that deliberately incorrect responses are being given (Goldin and MacDonald 1955). When asked to add 2 and 2, the answer may be 5. The sum of 3 and 3 may be stated as 7. When asked to point upward, the patient may point down and then point up when asked to point down. The Ganser syndrome is frequently accompanied by complaints of auditory and visual hallucinations, circumscribed amnesia, and disorientation. Neuropsychological testing yields highly inconsistent performance consistent with malingering (Heron et al. 1991). These symptoms develop rapidly and usually occur in response to a severe environmental stress, such as facing trial or imprisonment. This syndrome is short lived and requires essentially no active treatment. Impaired cognition may also be malingered for various types of gain. The effects of trivial head injuries may be magnified to escape hard labor or to gain monetary compensation. Mental status examination and neuropsychological testing of the individual who is malingering impaired cognition show inconsistent deficits, with better performance on many items that call for high-level integration than on some items calling for lesser levels of cognitive function. For example, simple similarities will not be understood, whereas more complicated similarities will call forth an abstract response; or digit span, a simple test of attention, will be limited to three digits, whereas the patient can follow complicated directions to the restroom. Attempts have been made to develop a formal set of criteria for the detection of malingered cognitive malfunction (Slick et al. 1999). Schizophrenia and bipolar disorder are discussed in Chapter 5.
Language Disorders Language disorders due to brain damage (aphasias) are easily confused with dementia. The categorization of aphasias is based on which language functions (e.g., fluency, comprehension, repetition, and naming) are impaired.
32
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Global aphasia impairs all language functions and occurs with large lefthemisphere strokes. Anomic aphasia, by contrast, primarily affects word finding, may be related to lesions of the left angular or left posterior middle temporal gyrus, and is common in Alzheimer’s disease. Broca’s (anterior, nonfluent) aphasia impairs verbal fluency, repetition, and naming and results from lesions of the posterior inferior portion of the left (or dominant) frontal lobe (Benson 1985). In Broca’s aphasia, speech requires great effort, and the speaker omits word modifiers such as articles, prepositions, and conjunctions. For example, a person who wants to go to the bathroom might say, “Want...go...bath...room,” with great effort and great relief after having expressed himself or herself. These patients generally understand what is said to them and can obey commands but have difficulty with repetition, reading aloud, and writing. Although they have difficulty with naming, they are helped by prompting. Patients with Wernicke’s (posterior, fluent) aphasia have fluent paraphasic, neologistic speech with poor comprehension, repetition, and naming. The naming difficulty is not usually aided by prompting. Reading and writing are also impaired. The sentence “I want to go to the bathroom” might be rendered by a posterior aphasic patient as “I wish to go to the you-know bath place now soon,” with no awareness of the peculiarity of his or her speech. The brain damage in this syndrome is to the posterior superior portion of the first temporal gyrus of the dominant hemisphere. Other aphasia syndromes are transcortical motor (impaired fluency and naming with preserved repetition), transcortical sensory (impaired comprehension and naming with preserved repetition), conduction (impaired repetition and naming), and mixed transcortical aphasia—a combination of transcortical sensory and transcortical motor aphasia that leaves patients able only to echo speech (Cummings 1985). The history of aphasia patients will usually reveal an obvious brain insult, most often stroke or head trauma. Examination often demonstrates focal neurological deficits such as hemiparesis (especially in the Broca type), unilateral hyperreflexia, and visual field deficits. On the other hand, frontotemporal dementia may present with progressive aphasias such as primary progressive aphasia and semantic dementia (Kertesz and Munoz 1997; Mesulam 1982) that are at times familial (Morris et al. 1994). (For more detailed discussions of primary progressive aphasia, see Chapters 3 and 8.)
Clinical Techniques and Tools for Diagnosing Cognitive Dysfunction The clinical evaluation includes history taking and direct examination of patients. History and mental status examination establish the presence of
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
33
cognitive dysfunction; the neurological examination helps to determine its etiology. History taking involves the patient, a knowledgeable informant, and all pertinent medical information. Direct access to medical records is important because most lay informants do not know actual blood pressure values or the outcomes of various laboratory tests. In the case of very old nursing home residents, there may be little information available other than from the nursing home staff and records. In addition to eliciting information concerning patients’ cognitive abilities, evidence is sought of emotional or interpersonal contributions to the presenting symptoms. Patients’ emotional responses to their mental difficulties are evaluated, and an attempt is made to determine family strengths and weaknesses. Patients’ personality patterns are also considered. This information helps shape the plan of management, even if treatment of the underlying brain disorder is not possible.
History Taking Under ideal circumstances, medical records are gathered and reviewed in advance of examining the patient. My policy is to have a clinic staff member obtain a history from an outside informant over the telephone before the first visit. I also ask what medications are being taken. This information helps to focus the mental status examination. Trained interviewers may employ a dementia questionnaire such as the one modified from Breitner and Folstein (1984), shown in Appendix A. Personal and medical history are also elicited directly from the patient and accompanying friends or family members, if any, when I see them in person. I first interview the patient in the presence of the accompanying person so that more accurate information can be obtained. I allow time to interview patients alone. I also allow time to interview the patients’ accompanying friends or relatives alone because they often withhold information in the patients’ presence out of concern that they may humiliate or anger them. Typical information withheld concerns patients’ paranoid thinking, hallucinations, or incontinence. Having a friend or relative present during the psychiatric interview is a comfort to most cognitively impaired persons. Thus, history taking tends to be a three-way conversation rather than a formal interview. If patients object to an outside informant being in the room at the time of the examination, I honor their request but still interview the outside informant if one is available. In the flow of the conversation, many clues emerge concerning the relationship between patients and significant others, the impact of patients on their families, and the impact of others on the patients. Husbands often resent their wives’ diminished ability to maintain their household. Wives may
34
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
resent having to be responsible for their formerly dominant husbands. In many cases, there is tension between spouses because one does not believe that the other truly cannot learn, remember, or understand. For example, a man who knew his wife had Alzheimer’s disease chided her for reading romance novels instead of the more substantial reading she had done earlier in her life. Examining one spouse in the presence of the other can be helpful in breaching the intact spouse’s denial and in demonstrating how to deal with defects in the other’s ability to remember, plan, and cooperate.
Mental Status Examination The mental status examination can demonstrate the probable presence or absence of a mental disorder and may also suggest the nature of the underlying brain or systemic disease. (For a highly detailed mental status examination, see Strub and Black 2000.) The mental status examination contained in Appendix B is employed at my clinic. It can ordinarily be accomplished in the course of a 60-minute visit if historical and medical information have been gathered and reviewed in advance. Patients and their families are asked to bring all of the medications in the family medicine chest. After the patient history has been taken, a formal mental status examination is performed (with the patient’s consent) in the presence of any accompanying persons. During this time, tentative hypotheses are made as to how far patients can be pushed to perform and how to best support their coping and defense mechanisms while simultaneously obtaining the needed information. Hearing and vision should be assessed: hearing by whispering or using a tuning fork, and vision by having patients read a standardized vision chart or identify small objects. Mental status examination is performed with consideration for patients’ frustration tolerance and is also tailored to their level of cognitive performance. For example, when it becomes obvious that the patient is not oriented to year and month, I do not usually inquire about orientation to day and date. A patient who cannot perform well on the serial subtraction of 3s is not asked to do serial subtraction of 7s. When the patient is irritable or easily frustrated, I abbreviate each category of inquiry. All responses are treated as equally valid, whether correct or not, and the patient is praised for effort by saying “good” or “that’s fine” after a series of responses. Exceptions to this general approach are in formal testing of cognition for various studies, when completeness is important, or when I suspect that the patient is not making an effort to perform the task, and I therefore withhold praise until the patient has made adequate effort. Primary memory (attention) is tested by using digit span, forward and in reverse. Secondary memory (recent memory, new learning, encoding) is
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
35
tested by asking patients to recall three words in 5 minutes. This test can be performed with objects presented verbally (verbal memory) or objects shown to the patient without naming them (visual memory). Response to cueing is also important, because it helps to distinguish retrieval deficits from failure to encode. As indicated previously testing remote memory is more difficult. Patients with little formal education can be asked about current events that fall within their range of interest; this is done most effectively when an outside informant is asked about recent events in the patient’s life. A detailed examination of language function includes assessment of articulation, fluency, comprehension, repetition, naming, reading, and the ability to write sentences. Assessing language fluency includes delayed word finding, paraphasias, and neologisms. Word fluency (the ability to generate a list of words), a very sensitive indicator of cognitive impairment, can be tested by asking patients to name all the animals they can think of in 1 minute. The average score is 18±6 (Goodglass and Kaplan 1972). Persons with Alzheimer’s disease tend to name few animals and to repeat names they have already mentioned. Comprehension tests begin with graded tasks, asking patients to point to one, two, and three objects in the room, followed by asking simple logic questions, as indicated in Appendix B. Naming tests should include the parts of objects, such as the parts of a watch (stem, watchband, back or case, face, crystal or glass) or the parts of a shirt. Reading ability should be considered in the context of patients’ education. Writing ability is assessed by asking patients to write a dictated sentence and by then asking them to compose a sentence of their own. Calculating ability is tested with simple problems in addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division. Praxis is evaluated by asking patients to imitate an action performed by the examiner, to perform simple motor acts in response to the examiner’s request and to copy a set of simple geometric figures. For the figures, we employ intersecting rectangles, a Greek cross, and a cube (see Appendix B). Drawing the cube is used to detect constructional dyspraxia in mildly impaired, well-educated persons. It is our experience that cognitively intact persons of age 80 years or older cannot draw the cube well. Fund of information is assessed by using a standard set of questions ranging from simple to difficult and by evaluating the responses in relation to patients’ level of education and job performance. The patient’s ability to think abstractly can be assessed by using similarities and proverbs, but assessment of abstract reasoning by the use of proverbs and similarities requires consideration of patients’ education, cultural background, and native language. Judgment can be estimated by asking
36
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
patients questions on how they would manage certain life situations, such as “What would you do if you were in a church or theater and noticed a fire that nobody else saw?” or “What would you do if the electric company called and told you that the last check you wrote them was returned because of insufficient funds?” However, judgment is probably best assessed from a history by an informant other than the patient. Other tests of executive function than those included in Appendix B (ideomotor and constructional praxis, abstract reasoning, and judgment) are contained in the neurological examination (Appendix C) and include the Luria hand sequence, go–no go tasks, and tests for impersistence.
Characterizing Dementing Illnesses Dementing illnesses can be characterized as cortical or subcortical (see also Chapter 8), based on the association of their characteristics with cortical or subcortical pathology. Cortical dementias present as one of two overlapping groups: frontotemporal or temporoparietal. Frontal dementia can be due to Pick’s disease or anterior cerebral artery stroke. It occurs less frequently in Alzheimer’s disease. Frontal lobe degeneration presents in a number of ways. A common presentation is progressive personality change and breakdown in social conduct (Kumar and Gottlieb 1993). Other features are defective judgment, difficulty in focusing attention, apathy, disinhibition, silliness, echoing words, mirroring others’ behavior, unawareness of deficit, difficulty in following instructions (often manifested as motor dyspraxia), and often a slightly prancing gait. As an example of defective judgment, Neary (1990), for example, reported on a patient who insisted on repeatedly changing into fresh clothes but refused to bathe. Personality changes often antedate cognitive symptoms in frontal dementia; Miller et al. (1991) found social withdrawal and behavioral disinhibition to be the earliest symptoms. In primary progressive aphasia, initial symptoms include difficulty with verbal expression. Normal pressure hydrocephalus often presents with frontal signs, such as complete indifference to urinary incontinence. Temporoparietal dementia, the most common presentation of Alzheimer’s disease, is accompanied by naming difficulties and constructional dyspraxia with relative preservation of personality. Persons with cortical dementias may be aware or unaware of their deficits. Subcortical dementias, with primary pathology in the thalami, basal ganglia, rostral brainstem, and their frontal projections, overlap in symptomatology with frontal dementias but also usually involve speech and motor abnormalities. The most prominent symptoms include overall slowing of movement and cognitive processing and deficits in social judgment and
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
37
mood change. Causes of subcortical dementia include cerebrovascular disease, Parkinson’s disease, Huntington’s disease, Wilson’s disease, and progressive supranuclear palsy (Cummings 1990). The following case histories provide examples of each of these syndromes:
Frontotemporal Dementia This patient was a 77-year-old married man with 6 years of education, whose problems began at age 73. In addition to difficulty with memory, attention, and problem solving and word finding, he experienced personality changes. He made inappropriate advances toward women, asking them if he could feel their breasts. He tended to eat excessively and shoveled food into his mouth. He became incontinent of urine, reporting auditory hallucinations telling him that he did not need to use the toilet. When seen during his third year of illness, he had been experiencing gait difficulty for about 2 months and was largely wheelchair bound. Mental status examination revealed mild disorientation in time, impaired attention, concentration, mildly impaired verbal fluency, and memory deficits that responded to cueing. His constructional praxis was intact, except for tremor, and he had impaired abstract reasoning. Neurological examination revealed an unsteady, shuffling gait and both resting and intention tremor. He had atrophy of his interosseus muscles, generalized weakness and reduced deep tendon reflexes. He had no frontal release signs. Neuropsychological testing showed borderline to average intelligence. He had an MMSE score of 21, showed marked perseveration, and had difficulty with placing the numbers on a clock. His immediate recall of structured verbal material was average. Although he forgot 50% of the material he initially learned after a brief delay, he benefited from semantic cueing. Delayed recall of simple geometrical figures was mildly to moderately impaired. His difficulties were primarily in the areas of memory and executive function. A magnetic resonance imaging scan of the brain showed mild cerebral atrophy, more severe frontally. Over the next year he became unable to walk and was admitted to a nursing home. He developed a deep venous thrombosis and had surgical intervention with insertion of an inferior vena cava filter. Subsequently, he developed sepsis. He died approximately 5 years after the onset of his illness. At postmortem examination, this patient’s brain weighed 1,240 grams. There was moderate hydrocephalus ex vacuo, severe atrophy of the frontal lobes, and mild atrophy of the temporal and parietal lobes. There were small numbers of plaques and neurofibrillary tangles but not sufficient for a diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. There was also depopulation of the substantia nigra without Lewy bodies. The presence of vacuolation in the upper layers of the cortex, intraneuronal inclusions staining positive for ubiquitin, and swollen axons in frontal lobe neurons led to a diagnosis of frontal lobe dementia with ubiquitinated inclusions (also called motor neuron inclusion dementia).
38
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Temporoparietal Dementia A 60-year-old woman with 12 years of education had a history of slowly progressive impairment of memory over 3 years. She was having difficulty balancing her checkbook, was forgetting to keep appointments, and could not remember information communicated to her a few moments earlier. She had stopped sewing and cooking, but she was still driving and continued to do gardening. She had experienced depression since the onset of her memory symptoms and had some remission of her depressive symptoms with antidepressant medication. When the patient was evaluated during her third year of illness, the physical neurological examination was entirely within normal limits. Psychiatric examination revealed no significant depression. Neuropsychological testing showed a verbal IQ of 91, performance IQ of 92, and a full-scale IQ of 91. She had very poor abstract reasoning and mild impairment of verbal fluency. On complex verbal list learning tasks she had severe impairment. On tests of contextual verbal memory, her immediate recall score was in the first percentile. She remembered none of the information presented after a 30-minute delay. An EEG and a magnetic resonance imaging scan of the brain showed no abnormality. A regional cerebral blood flow examination showed a mild reduction of blood flow in the posterior superior parietal areas and mild frontal flow reduction. Seven years after the onset of symptoms, she had become completely disoriented and was unable to discriminate between relatives and strangers. She had developed anxiety, sadness, restlessness, and fearfulness, and she spent most of her waking hours pacing about the house and wandering outside if allowed to. She also became irritable and uncooperative. Her spontaneous speech steadily decreased. She often talked or mumbled to herself. She became incontinent of urine and feces and needed to be assisted with all activities of daily living. Various antipsychotic and antianxiety agents were prescribed with little effect. In her seventh year of illness, she stopped eating and drinking, regressed to a fetal position and died a few months later. At postmortem study, this patient’s brain weighed 935 grams. There was moderate hydrocephalus ex vacuo and mild frontal temporal and parietal atrophy as well as pallor of the locus coeruleus. She had numerous neuritic plaques and intraneuronal neurofibrillary tangles in the neocortex and hippocampus. Her final diagnosis was Alzheimer’s disease.
Subcortical Dementia The patient was a 60-year-old married woman with 13 years of education. During the first year of her illness, her husband noted that she could no longer balance the books for their family-owned business. During that year, her husband and daughter noticed a marked personality change; she had become quieter and did not want to socialize as much. She had difficulty maintaining her train of thought. The patient complained of weakness and nervousness. Her weakness affected her lower extremities to the point that she had difficulty arising out of a chair.
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
39
Over the next 2 years she had progressively greater difficulty with balance, judging distance, and falling. The patient’s daughter was afraid to let her mother care for her children because she was afraid her mother would fall and drop a child. The patient had difficulty judging distances and had on several occasions scraped the sides of her car in the garage. Her husband noted that she tended to leave doors open, and he reported minimal change in her ability to put on makeup and dress herself appropriately. She had also been stopped for speeding while driving, which was totally out of character for her. When seen for comprehensive evaluation 3 years after the onset of her symptoms, she had difficulty dressing, feeding herself, and controlling her bowel and bladder functions. She tended to flop rather than to sit down, and she was unable to raise the volume of her voice. She had slightly decreased upward gaze. Her handwriting was noticeably micrographic. Her deep tendon reflexes were generally increased and she had reflex spread in the lower extremities. She had difficulty with rapidly alternating movement and had unsteady gait and severe impairment of postural reflexes. On mental status examination she was alert and oriented to year, month, date, and season. Her MMSE score was 25. Neuropsychological testing revealed a verbal IQ of 81, performance IQ of 91, and full-scale IQ of 85. She had difficulty with attention, concentration, and flexibility of thought. She had moderate to severe impairment of both short- and long-term verbal memory and figural memory. However, she demonstrated no language or communication dysfunction. A magnetic resonance imaging scan of the brain showed moderate cortical atrophy. A regional cerebral blood flow study showed reduced flow in the left frontal region and an area of superficially reduced flow in the right frontal region. Six years after the onset of her symptoms, the patient was bed bound and needed to have pills crushed for her to swallow. She died in a nursing facility 10 years after the onset of her illness. At postmortem examination, the patient’s brain weighed 1,050 grams. There was mild hydrocephalus and pallor of the substantia nigra and locus coeruleus. There was atrophy of the brainstem nuclei, deep cerebellar nuclei, and subthalamic nuclei. Microscopic examination of the brain revealed neuron loss, atrophy, gliosis, and intraneuronal neurofibrillary tangles in subthalamic nucleus, globus pallidus, substantia nigra, periaqueductal gray matter, tectum, medullary olivary nuclei, and cerebellar dentate nucleus. The final diagnosis was progressive supranuclear palsy.
Quantifying Aspects of Dementing Illness Clinicians generally rely on impressionistic data to make diagnoses and to follow the course of an illness and its treatment. They also employ objective measures, such as pulse, temperature, blood pressure, hematocrit, and blood urea nitrogen concentration. Growing awareness of the potential reversibility of some dementias and the potential for treatment of dementing
40
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
illness and dementia-related symptoms has led to the development of instruments to quantify the phenomena associated with dementing illness, including overall functional ability; cognition; and behavioral, emotional, perceptual, and ideational symptoms. Described below are some commonly employed scales. In addition, I have included recently developed research scales for quantifying behavioral and emotional symptoms and quality of life.
Measures of Global Function The BDRS (Blessed et al. 1968) (see Appendix D) is a 17-point activities of daily living scale that includes a wide range of behaviors. It is administered to a knowledgeable informant—usually a person who spends 20 or more hours per week with the patient. This scale takes approximately 10 minutes to administer and is a useful complement to testing patients directly, a process that often does not expose many of the behavioral concomitants of dementia, such as lack of sphincter control or difficulty in eating or dressing. Its high degree of subjectivity and the use of an untrained observer make it less reliable than the MMSE and the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS), but it encompasses a broader range of function and correlates well with MMSE and ADAS scores in Alzheimer’s disease patients. The Washington University Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Scale (Hughes et al. 1982) (see Appendix E) was designed for the assessment of Alzheimer’s disease outpatients. It grades on a scale ranging from 0 (normal) to 3 (severe) and is based on a structured interview that takes 30–40 minutes and involves interviewing an informant as well as the patient. Two additional stages have been added to the CDR Scale (Morris 1993): stages 4 (profound) and 5 (terminal). In stage 4, speech is usually unintelligible or irrelevant, and patients are unable to follow simple instructions or understand commands. Patients occasionally recognize the spouse or caregiver, use fingers more than utensils, and require much assistance. They are able to walk a few steps with help (but usually are chair bound), often make purposeless movements, and are frequently incontinent. In stage 5, patients are unresponsive and neither comprehend communication nor recognize others. Patients are bedridden and unable to sit or stand. They need to be fed, often by nasogastric tube, and may have difficulty swallowing.
Measures of Cognitive Function The MMSE (Folstein et al. 1975), administered directly to the patient, is probably the most widely used brief cognitive assessment tool. It requires 10–15 minutes to administer and samples orientation, attention, concentra-
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
41
tion, recent memory, naming, repetition, comprehension, ideomotor praxis, constructional praxis, and the ability to construct a sentence. A perfect score is 30 points. The MMSE is confounded by premorbid intelligence—higher intelligence making for better performance and lower intelligence for worse. The originators indicate that a score of 23 or below by someone with a high school education is suggestive of dementia. A cutoff score of 18 or below is suggested for those with an eighth grade education or less. A population-based study showed an inverse relationship between test score and education. The median score was 29 for unscreened individuals with at least 9 years of schooling, 26 for those with 5–8 years of schooling, and 22 for those with 0–4 years of education. The same study also showed an inverse relationship between age and test score, with a median of 29 for those ages 18–24 years and a median of 25 for those ages 80 years and older (Crum et al. 1993). Although the MMSE is not a sensitive test, its brevity and the minimal training required in its administration make it especially useful as a screening test for cognitive impairment and for following the course of delirium in medical and surgical patients. This instrument is protected by copyright and must be ordered from Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc., P.O. Box 998, Odessa, FL 33556 (1-800-331-8378). The ADAS (Rosen et al. 1984) (see Appendix F) is a research scale designed to be sensitive to small changes in Alzheimer’s disease patients. Used to assess the effectiveness of cognition-enhancing drugs, the scale is based on the symptoms most commonly seen in 31 Alzheimer’s disease cases confirmed by brain biopsy (Mohs et al. 1983). The scale consists of 21 items, half of which are cognitive and half of which are noncognitive. Items are rated on a 6-point scale ranging from 0 (no impairment) to 5 (severe impairment). This test requires approximately 45 minutes and an outside informant, and needs to be administered by a person trained specifically in its use. The cognitive subtest items correlate well with other measures of clinical progression in Alzheimer’s disease. The noncognitive items such as depression, appetite, and pacing do not correlate well with dementia progression, but they do assess the major behavioral symptoms seen in Alzheimer’s disease. The cognitive portion of the ADAS has been used extensively in trials of cognitive enhancers for Alzheimer’s disease. The ADAS administration kit has not been standardized, and the test is too cumbersome for routine clinical use. It was not designed to be used as a diagnostic instrument.
Measures of Behavioral and Emotional Symptoms The Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) (Cummings et al. 1994) (see Appendix G) was designed to assess the entire range of psychopathology in cognitively impaired individuals. The newest version has 10 probe ques-
42
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
tions concerning dysphoria, euphoria, anxiety, agitation, aberrant motor behavior, apathy, irritability, disinhibition, delusions, hallucinations, nighttime behavior disturbances, and appetite and eating abnormalities (Cummings 1997). It also includes a measure of caregiver distress (Kaufer et al. 1998). If the response to the probe question is positive, further questions are asked. If the response is negative, the examiner proceeds to the next domain. A nursing home version has been also been created, but the authors caution that it is best administered by trained research personnel (Wood et al. 2000). Because of its design, the instrument can be administered in 5–10 minutes by an experienced technician or clinician. It is a good general screening tool and can also be used to follow the course of treatment. The instrument is available from Jeffrey Cummings, M.D., 710 Westwood Plaza, Suite 2238, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1769. The Agitated Behavior in Dementia (ABID) Scale (Logsdon et al. 1999b) (see Appendix H) is a 16-item instrument administered to caregivers. It has a 2-week window of observation. The scale evaluates the frequency of and caregiver reaction to common agitated behaviors in community-dwelling persons with Alzheimer’s disease. Derived from clinical experience and items from several other scales, it evaluates the behaviors rated as most problematic in persons with mild to moderate dementia that can be observed and described objectively. Administration time is about 15 minutes. This instrument is also useful for tracking response to treatment.
Measures of Quality of Life The impact of treatments on patients’ quality of life is as important as the impact of treatment on patients’ symptoms or the diseases that produce these symptoms. Because quality of life is subjective, it can be measured directly only in persons with relatively mild cognitive impairment. Persons with severe cognitive impairment are often unable to perceive or report on quality-of-life issues. The scales described below, although still in development, appear to have the most promise for clinical trials. The Quality of Life in Alzheimer’s Disease (QOL-AD) Scale (Logsdon et al. 1999a) (see Appendix I) is a 13-item instrument that can be administered directly to persons with Alzheimer’s disease or other forms of cognitive impairment. When administered directly, it is reliable for persons with MMSE scores between 10 and 28. The scale can also be given as a selfcompleted questionnaire to a caregiver surrogate. Each item is rated on a 4-point scale from poor to excellent. The patient and caregiver scales may be scored independently or may be combined by multiplying the patient score by 2, adding the caregiver score, and dividing by 3, thus giving more
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
43
weight to the patient report. The scale has a 1-week window of observation. Administration time is about 15 minutes. Its sensitivity to interventions is not yet known. The Quality of Life in Dementia (QUALID) Scale (Weiner et al. 2000) (see Appendix J) is an 11-item scale with a 1-week window of observation designed to rate quality of life in persons with advanced dementing illness. Items consist of observable behaviors such as smiling, crying, and apparent enjoyment of interaction with others. It is administered to family or professional caregivers. Administration time is about 5 minutes. Pilot data suggest that the scale is sensitive to change and can be used in treatment studies.
Summary Cognitive impairment is detected from history and mental status examination. As a result of increasing precision in diagnosis, clinicians are able to better determine the underlying brain or systemic pathology in persons with cognitive impairment. Along with greater precision in diagnosis has come greater awareness of the diversity of symptoms associated with these illnesses, including behavioral, emotional, vegetative, ideational, and perceptual disturbances. Many instruments have been developed to quantify the cognitive, behavioral, and psychological aspects of dementing illness. These instruments can provide valid and reliable measures of the effects of interventions with patients and can thereby aid in developing effective treatments.
References Alexopoulos GS, Meyers BS, Young RC: The course of geriatric depression with “reversible dementia”: a controlled study. Am J Psychiatry 150:1693–1699, 1993 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 2000 Balldin J, Gottfries CG, Karlsson I, et al: Dexamethasone suppression test and serum prolactin in dementia disorders. Br J Psychiatry 143:277–281, 1983 Benson D: Aphasia, in Clinical Neuropsychology. Edited by Heilman KM, Valenstein E. New York, Oxford University Press, 1985, pp 17–48 Biedler A, Juckenhofel S, Larsen R, et al: Postoperative Storungen der kognitiven Leistungfahigkeit bei altern Patienten. Die Ergebnisse der International Study of Postoperative Cognitive Dysfunction: (ISPOCD 1). Anaesthesist 48:884– 895, 1999
44
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Blackford RC, LaRue A: Criteria for diagnosing age associated memory impairment: proposed improvements from the field. Dev Neuropsychol 5:298–300, 1989 Blessed G, Tomlinson BE, Roth M: The association between quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral gray matter of elderly subjects. Br J Psychiatry 114:797–811, 1968 Bleuler E: Textbook of Psychiatry. New York, Macmillan, 1924 Bolla KI, Lindgren KN, Bonaccorsy C, et al: Memory complaints in older adults. Arch Neurol 48:61–64, 1991 Bowen J, Teri L, Kukull W, et al: Progression to dementia in patients with isolated memory loss. Lancet 349:763–765, 1997 Breitner JCS, Folstein MF: Familial Alzheimer dementia: a prevalent disorder with specific clinical features. Psychol Med 14:63–80, 1984 Brookmeyer R, Gray S, Kawas C: Projections of Alzheimer’s disease in the United States and the public health impact of delaying disease onset. Am J Public Health 88:1337–1342, 1998 Butters MA, Becker JT, Nebes RD, et al: Changes in cognitive functioning following treatment of late-life depression. Am J Psychiatry 157:1949–1954, 2000 Clairfield AM: The reversible dementias: do they reverse? Ann Intern Med 109: 476–486, 1988 Cohen RM, Weingartner H, Smallberg SA, et al: Effort and cognition in depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 39:593–597, 1982 Crapper DR, Dalton AJ, Skopitz M: Alzheimer degeneration in Down’s syndrome. Arch Neurol 33:618–623, 1975 Crook T, Bartus RT, Ferris SH, et al: Age-associated memory impairment. Proposed diagnostic criteria and measures of clinical change: report of a National Institute of Mental Health Work Group. Dev Neuropsychol 2:261–276, 1986 Crum RM, Anthony JC, Bassett SS, et al: Population-based norms for the MiniMental State Examination by age and educational level. JAMA 269:2386– 2391, 1993 Cummings JL: Clinical Neuropsychiatry. New York, Grune & Stratton, 1985 Cummings JL: Introduction, in Subcortical Dementia. Edited by Cummings JL. New York, Oxford University Press, 1990, pp 4–16 Cummings JL: The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: assessing psychopathology in dementia patients. Neurology 48 (5 suppl 6):S10–S16, 1997 Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray KF, et al: The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 44:2308– 2314, 1994 Derouesne C, Lacomblez L: [Memory complaints: epidemiology and diagnostic approach]. Presse Med 29:858–862, 2000 Dijkstra JB, Houx PJ, Jolles J: Cognition after major surgery in the elderly: test performance and complaints. Br J Anaesth 82:867–874, 1999 Drachman DA, Leavitt J: Memory impairment in the aged: storage versus retrieval deficit. J Exp Psychol 93:302–308, 1972 Duppils GS, Wikblad K: Acute confusional states in patients undergoing hip surgery: a prospective observation study. Gerontology 46:36–43, 2000
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
45
Engel GE, Romano J: Delirium, a syndrome of cerebral insufficiency. J Chron Dis 9:260–277, 1959 Esquirol JED: Mental Maladies: A Treatise on Insanity (1845). Translated by Hunt EK. New York, Hafner, 1965 Ewbank DC: Deaths attributable to Alzheimer’s disease in the United States. Am J Public Health 89:90–92, 1999 Fahlander K, Berger AK, Backman L, et al: Depression does not aggravate the episodic memory deficits associated with Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 13:532–538, 1999 Federal Interagency Forum on Aging Related Statistics: Older Americans 2000: Key Indicators of Well Being. Washington, DC, Government Printing Office, 2000 Finkel SI, Costa e Silva J, Cohen G, et al: Behavioral and psychological signs and symptoms of dementia: a consensus statement on current knowledge and implications for research and treatment. Int Psychogeriatr 8 (suppl 3):497–500, 1996 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189– 198, 1975 Folstein MF, Bassett SS, Anthony JC, et al: Dementia: case ascertainment in a community survey. J Gerontol 46:M132–M138, 1991 Garcia CA, Reding MJ, Blass JP: Overdiagnosis of dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 29:407–410, 1981 Goldin S, MacDonald JE: The Ganser state. J Ment Sci 101:267–280, 1955 Goodglass H, Kaplan E: The Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders. Philadelphia, PA, Lea & Febiger, 1972 Greenwald BS, Mathe AA, Mohs RC, et al: Cortisol and Alzheimer's disease, II: dexamethasone suppression, dementia severity, and affective symptoms. Am J Psychiatry 143:442–446, 1986 Greenwald BS, Kramer-Ginsberg E, Marin DB, et al: Dementia with coexistent major depression. Am J Psychiatry 146:1472–1478, 1989 Grober E, Lipton RB, Hall C, et al: Memory impairment on free and cued selective reminding predicts dementia. Neurology 54:827–832, 2000 Gustafson Y, Berggren D, Brannstrom B, et al: Acute confusional states in elderly patients treated for femoral neck fracture. J Am Geriatr Soc 36:525–530, 1988 Helkala E-L, Koivisto K, Hanninen T, et al: Stability of age-associated memory impairment during a longitudinal population-based study. J Am Geriatr Soc 45:120–122, 1997 Heron EA, Kritchevsky M, Delis DC: Neuropsychological presentation of Ganser symptoms. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 13:656–666, 1991 Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL: A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry 140:566–572, 1982 Jenike MA, Albert MS: The dexamethasone suppression test in patients with presenile and senile dementia of the Alzheimer's type. J Am Geriatr Soc 32:441– 444, 1984
46
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Katzman R: Diagnosis and management of dementia, in Principles of Geriatric Neurology. Edited by Katzman R, Rowe JW. Philadelphia, PA, FA Davis, 1992, pp 167–206 Kaufer DI, Cummings JL, Christine D, et al: Assessing the impact of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease: the Neuropsychiatric Inventory Caregiver Distress Scale. J Am Geriatr Soc 46:210–215, 1998 Kertesz A, Munoz DG: Primary progressive aphasia. Clin Neurosci 4:95–102, 1997 Kiloh LG: Pseudo-dementia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 37:336–351, 1961 Koivisto K, Reinikainen KJ, Hanninen T, et al: Prevalence of age-associated memory impairment in a randomly selected population from eastern Finland. Neurology 45:741–747, 1995 Kraepelin E: Lectures on Clinical Psychiatry, 2nd Edition. Translated by Johnstone T. New York, William Wood, 1913 Kumar A, Gottlieb G: Frontotemporal dementias: a new clinical syndrome? American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 1:95–108, 1993 Lipowski ZJ: Organic mental disorders: introduction and review of syndromes, in Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry/III, 3rd Edition, Vol 2. Edited by Kaplan HI, Freedman AM, Sadock BJ. Baltimore, MD, Williams & Wilkins, 1980, pp 1359–1391 Lipowski ZJ: Delirium (acute confusional states). JAMA 258:1789–1792, 1987 Logsdon RG, Gibbons LE, McCurry SM, et al: Quality of life in Alzheimer’s disease: patient and caregiver reports. J Ment Health Aging 5:21–32, 1999a Logsdon RG, Teri L, Weiner MF, et al: Assessment of agitation in Alzheimer’s disease: the Agitated Behavior in Dementia Scale. J Am Geriatr Soc 47:1354– 1358, 1999b Lyketsos C, Olin J: Depression in Alzheimer’s disease: overview and treatment. Biol Psychiatry 52:243–252, 2002 Mann DM: The pathological association between Down syndrome and Alzheimer disease. Mech Ageing Dev 31:213–255, 1988 Marsden CD, Harrison MJG: Outcome of investigation of patients with presenile dementia. Br Med J 2:229–252, 1972 Mattis S: Dementia Rating Scale. Odessa, FL, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1988 Mayeux R, Stern Y, Rosen J, et al: Depression, intellectual impairment and Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 31:645–650, 1981 Mesulam M-M: Slowly progressive aphasia without generalized dementia. Ann Neurol 11:592–598, 1982 Miller BL, Cummings JL, Villanueva-Meyer J, et al: Frontal lobe degeneration: clinical, neuropsychological, and SPECT characteristics. Neurology 41:1374–1382, 1991 Mohs RC, Rosen WG, Greenwald BS, et al: Neuropathologically validated scales for Alzheimer’s disease, in Assessment in Geriatric Psychopharmacology. Edited by Crook T, Ferris S, Bartus R. New Canaan, CT, Mark Powley, 1983 Morris JC: The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 43:2412–2414, 1993 Morris JC, Cole M, Banker BQ, et al: Hereditary dysphasic dementia and the PickAlzheimer spectrum. Ann Neurol 16:455–466, 1994
Cognitive Dysfunction and Dementing Illness
47
Neary D: Dementia of frontal lobe type. J Am Geriatr Soc 38:71–72, 1990 Oliver C, Crayton L, Holland A, et al: A four-year prospective study of age-related cognitive change in adults with Down’s syndrome. Psychol Med 28:1365– 1377, 1998 Paykel ES, Brayne C, Huppert FA, et al: Incidence of dementia in a population older than 75 years in the United Kingdom. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51:325–332, 1994 Petersen RC, Smith G, Kokmen E, et al: Memory function in normal aging. Neurology 42:396–401, 1992 Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, et al: Aging, memory, and mild cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr 9 (suppl 1):65–69, 1997 Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, et al: Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 56:303–308, 1999 Pinel P: A Treatise on Insanity (1806). New York, Hafner, 1962 Poon LW: Differences in human memory with aging: nature, causes and clinical implications, in Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, 2nd Edition. Edited by Birren JE, Schaie KW. New York, Van Nostrand Reinhold, 1985, pp 427–462 Prichard JAC: A Treatise on Insanity. Philadelphia, PA, Haswell, Barrington, & Haswell, 1837 Reese HW, Rodeheaver D: Problem solving and complex decision making, in Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, 2nd Edition. Edited by Birren JE, Schaie KW. New York, Van Nostrand Reinhold, 1985, pp 474–499 Ritchie K, Ledesrt B, Touchon J: Subclinical cognitive impairment: epidemiology and clinical characteristics. Compr Psychiatry 41 (2 suppl 1):61–65, 2000 Robinson RG, Forrester AW: Neuropsychiatric aspects of cerebrovascular disease, in The American Psychiatric Press Textbook of Neuropsychiatry. Edited by Hales RE, Yudofsky SC. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1987, pp 191–208 Rosen WG, Mohs RC, Davis KL: A new rating scale for Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 14:1356–1364, 1984 Roth M, Huppert FA, Tym E, et al. CAMDEX. The Cambridge Examination for Mental Disorders of the Elderly. New York, Cambridge University Press, 1988 Royall DR: Executive cognitive impairment: a novel perspective on dementia. Neuroepidemiology 19:293–299, 2000 Rubin RT, Poland RE: Variability in cortisol level assay methods. Arch Gen Psychiatry 41:724–725, 1984 Schludermann EH, Schludermann SM, Merryman PW, et al: Halstead’s studies in the neuropsychology of aging. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2:49–172, 1983 Scoville WB, Milner B: Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 20:11–21, 1957 Slick DJ, Sherman EM, Iverson GL: Diagnostic criteria for malingered neurocognitive dysfunction: proposed standards for clinical practice and research. Clin Neuropsychol 13:545–561, 1999 Sternberg DE, Jarvik ME: Memory functions in depression: improvement with antidepressant medication. Arch Gen Psychiatry 33:219–224, 1976
48
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Strub RL, Black FW: The Mental Status Examination in Neurology, 4th Edition. Philadelphia, PA, FA Davis, 2000 Thomas J: A Complete Pronouncing Medical Dictionary. Philadelphia, PA, JB Lippincott, 1889 Ulatowska HK, Chapman SB, Highley AP, et al: Discourse in healthy old-elderly adults: a longitudinal study. Aphasiology 12:619–633, 1998 Volicer L, Harper DG, Manning BC, et al: Sundowning and circadian rhythms in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 158:704–711, 2001 Wang PN, Wang SJ, Fuh JL, et al: Subjective memory complaint in relation to cognitive performance and depression: a longitudinal study of a rural Chinese population. J Am Geriatr Soc 48:295–299, 2000 Webster R, Holroyd S: Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in delirium. Psychosomatics 41:519–522, 2000 Weiner MF, Lovitt R: An examination of patients’ understanding of information from health care providers. Hosp Community Psychiatry 35:619–620, 1984 Weiner MF, Sairam R: The relationship of major depressive disorder to Alzheimer’s disease, in Physical Illness and Depression in Older Adults: A Handbook of Theory, Research, and Practice. Edited by Williamson GM, Shaffer DR, Parmelee PA. New York, Kluwer Academic/PlenumPublishers, 2000, pp 257– 276 Weiner MF, Bruhn M, Svetlik DS, et al: Experiences with depression in a dementia clinic. J Clin Psychiatry 52:234–238, 1991 Weiner MF, Edland SD, Luszczynska H: Prevalence and incidence of major depression in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 151:1006–1009, 1993 Weiner MF, Martin-Cook K, Svetlik DA, et al: The Quality of Life in Late-Stage Dementia (QUALID) Scale. Journal of the American Medical Directory Association 1:114–116, 2000 Weiner MF, Doody RS, Sairam R, et al: Prevalence and incidence of major depressive disorder in Alzheimer's disease: findings from two databases. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 13:8–12, 2002 Weingartner H, Cohen RM, Murphy DL, et al: Cognitive processes in depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 38:42–47, 1981 Weingartner H, Grafman J, Boutelle W, et al: Forms of memory failure. Science 221:380–382, 1983 Wisniewski K, Howe J, Williams DG: Precocious aging and dementia in patients with Down’s syndrome. Biol Psychiatry 13:619–627, 1978 Wolfson C, Wolfson DB, Asgharian M, et al: A reevaluation of the duration of survival after the onset of dementia. N Engl J Med 344:1111–1116, 2001 Wood S, Cummings JL, Hsu MA, et al: The use of The Neuropsychiatric Inventory in nursing home residents. Characterization and measurement. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 8:75–83, 2000 Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR, Amaral DG: Human amnesia and the medical temporal region: enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippocampus. J Neurosci 6:2950–2967, 1986
CHAPTER
2
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process Myron F. Weiner, M.D.
Although certain symptoms by definition constitute the core of the dementia syndrome, the clinical presentation of any dementing illness differs among individuals because these symptoms result from interacting social, psychological, physical, and biochemical forces. These symptoms fluctuate from hour to hour and from day to day, depending on patients’ emotional makeup, their interpersonal and physical environment, and changes in their general health and in the nature and progression of their underlying brain disease. As illustrated in Figure 2–1, the development of the full-blown dementia syndrome results when the threshold for adequate cognitive function—a threshold that varies from individual to individual and from time to time within any given individual— has been exceeded. Progression from normal functioning to mild cognitive impairment to dementia may be subtle and gradual, as in the case of Alzheimer’s disease, or progression to dementia may occur catastrophically, as in stroke, brain trauma, or fulminant Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease. A dementing illness may also manifest suddenly after a trivial brain insult, such as a mild episode of hypotension during spinal anesthesia for prostate surgery. In the latter case, we hypothesize that the minor brain insult reduces the brain’s overall integrative capacity to the point that the formerly subthreshold process manifests. The same phenomenon may result from psychological factors in persons whose brain function is already compromised. When experiencing high levels of anxiety or depression, such persons become more confused because they are unable to deal simultaneously with intense emotion and multiple environmental stimuli. Another frequent occurrence is the increased confusion of persons whose compromised brain function is not apparent until they attempt adapting to a new living situation, such as moving from one house to another. Still another is the unmasking of the cognitive impairment of one spouse on the death of the other. 49
50
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
FIGURE 2–1. A model of cognitive functioning. If the symptoms that develop in the course of dementing illness are viewed outside the constraints of dementia as defined in DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association 2000), it can be seen, using the same analogy as in Figure 2–1, that the threshold model can be applied to other aspects of dementing illness than information processing. The threshold model applies, for example, to behavioral disinhibition with resultant verbal and physical aggression toward others. Although this symptom is related in part to memory and other information-processing deficits, cognitively impaired persons with relatively mild memory deficits frequently lose their ability to refrain from grasping or striking out at others verbally or physically when frustrated or distressed. This is especially a problem when caregivers see them as psychologically clinging, illtempered, or vindictive and begin to react to cognitively impaired persons as manipulative or deliberately cruel. Consultation was requested for a 69-year-old man who had been admitted to a special care unit for disturbed dementia patients. The examiner greeted
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process
51
the patient, who was seated at a dining room table, and had a pleasant interaction concerning the patient’s former business and the types of merchandise he had sold. The patient acknowledged that he had difficulty with memory that was annoying, and also that he had difficulty with word finding. The examiner noted that the patient was seated in a wheelchair and asked the patient if he had difficulty walking. The patient said that he had mild difficulty walking due to his Parkinson’s disease. A few minutes later, the patient began attempting to stand and was told by an attendant to seat himself. This angered him. He pulled away from the table and wheeled himself away. When another attendant offered to tie his shoelaces, he began an angry, accusatory outburst, saying that he would decide if his laces needed to be tied and that if they did he would do it himself. The angry interaction between him and the staff members escalated as he again began to stand and they attempted to reseat him. He finally stood up and was able to walk by pushing the wheelchair. When the patient was reapproached by the examiner another angry outburst ensued, and the patient said he would never speak to the examiner again. The staff had a particularly difficult time with this man because his memory was far less impaired than that of others on the unit; they had difficulty understanding that his impairment was largely in the realm of judgment and emotional control.
Personality Function in Dementia Although it is not independent of the brain, the mind can be thought of as having a dynamic structure of its own. There are many dynamic schemas to explain the functioning of the mind. I use the Freudian schema, which conceives of mental functioning as a balance of the mind’s adaptive, coping, and defensive functions (the ego), the pressure of the sexual and aggressive drives (the id), and internal standards of behavior (conscience or superego). In brain-damaged persons, adaptive or coping ability is reduced, which may result in the development of emotional symptoms, perceptual or ideational disturbances, or problem behaviors. The nature of the symptoms and behaviors is partly determined by a preexisting personality structure and the new relationship between individuals and their sense of what they ought to be (their ego ideal). For some persons, their ego ideal may be the ability to nurture others or to competently maintain a household; for others, it may be the ability to function at a paying job. Thus, in early dementing illness, loss of ability to function in ways that are significant to the individual causes greater distress than does a person’s global loss of function. For example, a man with early Alzheimer’s disease who prided himself on his organizational ability became enormously distressed when he was unable to gather income tax information for his accountant and literally spent days and nights repeatedly making lists of his assets that he was unable to organize.
52
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Coping Ability The concept of adaptive or coping ability is important for understanding much of the phenomenology of dementing illness. To cope adequately with the demands of everyday living, a person must have sufficient ability to appropriately suppress, repress, or sublimate the demands of drives and conscience or to find adequate substitutes for meeting those demands. When a person lacks the ability to repress, suppress, sublimate, or substitute, the classic psychological defense mechanisms delineated by Anna Freud (1946) come into play. Coping ability is related only in part to cognitive integrity. It is also related to the biological integrity of the person as a whole and to that person’s ability to make positive relationships with others, to control impulses, test reality, take psychological or physical action, and assume responsibility for actions taken. Biological integrity refers to the ability to receive and interpret stimuli, to initiate and perform motor behavior necessary for self-maintenance, and to have sufficient psychological and physical energy to attend to matters outside of maintaining the integrity of one’s physical being. Thus, a person who has just experienced a myocardial infarction and is having intense chest pain has little ability to deal effectively with other matters. The same is true of a person whose emphysema is so severe that breathing requires active concentration and deliberate effort. By contrast, a trivial distraction makes it impossible for a cognitively impaired person to function at a task requiring concentration.
Personality Structure Personality refers to an individual’s predominant patterns of coping mechanisms and defenses. Common patterns of personality structure are found in DSM-IV-TR in their most exaggerated form—as personality disorders. Personality disorders differ from the traits that constitute more normal personality structure in the personality-disordered individuals’ tendency to employ stereotyped patterns of dealing with the world. Thus, many persons enjoy self-display and being the center of attention, as do persons with a histrionic personality disorder. The person with a histrionic trait is still able to function when not the center of attention. The person with a histrionic personality disorder often seeks attention through physical symptoms or outrageous behavior when he or she is unable to secure it through ordinary self-display. Personality styles include obsessive-compulsive, paranoid, avoidant, antisocial, and schizoid. Premorbid personality has much to do with the ability to concentrate, to establish positive relationships with others, to test reality, and to assume
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process
53
responsibility. Dementing illness uncovers aspects of the personality that were partially suppressed when the individual was biologically intact. Individuals who were formerly able to suppress their irritability may begin to lash out. Persons with pronounced dependent needs may become even less willing to do for themselves. Those who relish their autonomy may strive in unrealistic ways to maintain that autonomy and refuse appropriate help. This is often seen in the case of widowed elders, independent for many years, who refuse the help of neighbors and the public health nurse in meeting their needs as their sight, hearing, cognitive function, and physical health fail. Through historical information provided by the patient and others, it is possible to determine which behaviors are temporary reactions and which are aspects of the individual’s personality and are largely unalterable (Weiner 1984). This information also gives cues for managing patients. Knowing, for example, that a particular patient has had a lifelong inability to share, it would not be expected that this patient could comfortably tolerate a nursing home setting that involved sharing a room. A person who has been dominant in relationships throughout life will find it difficult to be under the control of others.
Dementing Illness as a Psychological Process A dynamic psychological view of dementing illness makes it easy to understand the fluctuating cognitive and behavioral symptoms of persons with intact and impaired brain function. Emotionally healthy persons are kept alert and are challenged by small increments of anxiety. Intense anxiety, on the other hand, impairs concentration, learning, and behavioral control. The greater number of brain systems that are affected, the greater the detrimental effect of anxiety. Thus, small changes in a person’s level of emotional tension during the course of a day can produce large fluctuations in coping ability and behavioral control. The same is true of sensory input. Adequate sensory input helps to maintain orientation in time and space; overwhelming input in the form of too novel or too many stimuli also increases confusion and impairs self-control. Persons with dementing illness are frequently accused by family members and professional caregivers of exaggerating their degree of cognitive impairment because these individuals appear to remember emotionally charged events but seem to rapidly forget neutral information, such as the location of items in the kitchen. This, however, is not unique to persons with dementing illness. The same phenomenon occurs in cognitively intact persons (Heuer and Reisberg 1990); it appears to be mediated by the >-adrenergic system and can be blocked by the >-adrenergic receptor antagonist propranolol (Cahill et al. 1994)
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
54
The psychology of dementing illness involves all of the coping mechanisms and defense mechanisms outlined in Tables 2–1 and 2–2. Table 2–1 indicates positive adaptive or coping mechanisms. TABLE 2–1.
Coping mechanisms
Decision making Mobilization of will Assumption of responsibility Stimulus seeking Mastery Attribution of meaning Trust of oneself and important others Hope
TABLE 2–2.
Defense mechanisms
Neurotic
Immature
Psychotic
Repression Intellectualization Isolation Displacement Reaction formation Dissociation
Projection Fantasy Hypochondriasis Passive-aggressive behavior Acting out
Denial of external reality Distortion Delusional projection
Source.
Adapted from Vaillant 1977.
The capacity for positive adaptation involves the ability to make decisions and to follow through with appropriate activity. Making decisions and taking action both involve the assumption of responsibility: acknowledging one’s own actions as a product of one’s own wishes or fears. Psychologically healthy persons tend to seek out novel stimuli and attempt to master and attribute meaning to their environment. They learn when they can trust their own judgment and the judgment of others, and they maintain hope that they can positively influence the world about them and derive a sense of satisfaction from their activities. Five other coping mechanisms have been described by Vaillant (1977) based on a long-term study of healthy men. They are 1) sublimation, 2) suppression, 3) altruism, 4) anticipation, and 5) humor. Sublimation is converting physical and psychological drives into socially acceptable thoughts and actions. Suppression is waiting for the best moment to express a thought or to act. Altruism is doing for others without expectation of personal gain. Anticipation is planning constructive action. It differs from
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process
55
worry or rumination, which lead primarily to greater worry rather than constructive action. Humor is the ability to enjoy surprise and uncertainty and to surprise others in a pleasant way. Coping mechanisms begin to fail in cognitively impaired persons. Because decision making and follow-through become difficult, such persons often blame others instead of recognizing that they have lost decisionmaking ability and initiative. Instead of acknowledging an inability to remember, these persons blame others when they do not meet their responsibilities. They rationalize inappropriate behaviors; they avoid new stimuli. They find that comprehension and mastery of the environment become more difficult, leading to feelings of hopelessness and helplessness. Instead of sublimating, raw drives emerge, sometimes in the form of inappropriate aggression or inappropriate genital display. Lack of ability to suppress leads to impatience. Self-centeredness replaces altruism as impaired coping ability reduces the capacity for empathy. Anticipation gives way to impulsiveness or inertia. Laughter in reaction to novel stimuli is replaced by startle and upset. A positive strategy for dealing with reduced coping ability is acknowledging the deficit and consciously developing compensatory mechanisms such as making lists, avoiding unfamiliar places, asking questions, and depending on others to solve problems. Often, however, there is damage to the brain regions that enable self-awareness. Patients with Alzheimer’s disease, for example, are frequently unaware of their memory deficits (Sevush and Leve 1993). Persons with frontal lobe damage are unaware of their impaired judgment. Persons with Wernicke’s aphasia frequently do not grasp their inability to understand what others are trying to communicate to them. Without self-awareness, conscious compensatory mechanisms are less likely to be developed. Persons with dementing illness whose coping mechanisms and self-awareness are intact may experience normal mourning for the loss of their former self (Bahro et al. 1995). Many persons with mild cognitive impairment become self-critical and experience lowering of self-esteem resulting from an imbalance between their ego ideal, their performance standards, and their impaired coping ability. The person can no longer live up to an ego ideal that includes productive work. In addition, self-criticisms that would ordinarily be dismissed overwhelm the personality and flood it with guilt and self-recrimination. The defenses shown in Table 2–2 represent failures of positive coping ability. They are mechanisms used to deal with its inadequate capacity to perceive, integrate, and act effectively on stimuli that impinge on the organism from within and without. As originally formulated (Freud 1946), these mechanisms referred to defenses against awareness of unconscious wishes or conflict, but they can also defend against awareness of unpleasant
56
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
or frightening aspects of reality. The defense mechanisms listed in Table 2–2 are classified by Vaillant (1977) into three groups: neurotic, immature, and psychotic, based on the frequency with which they are associated with various mental disorders and stages of development. Many types of defense are adopted by persons with dementing illness, depending on their premorbid personality, their environment, and the rate at which their illness progresses. Rapidly progressive brain disorders may provoke more primitive defense mechanisms, such as denial, distortion, and delusional projection. Denial is found frequently in all forms of dementing illness. It is not the same as neglect, such as the syndrome of contralateral neglect that often accompanies lesions of the right (nondominant) hemisphere (Denny-Brown et al. 1954). The apparent denial by many cognitively impaired patients of their defects in memory and judgment is often due to losing the neuronal substrate for self-observation. In a study of individuals with Alzheimer’s disease, Derouesne et al. (1999) showed that persons with mild Alzheimer’s disease were aware of the existence of their deficits, but not the severity of deficit or their degree of incapacity. Decreased awareness correlated positively with apathy and frontal perfusion deficits detected with single photon emission computed tomography, whereas, as would be expected from the psychological standpoint, persons with greater awareness of deficit were more anxious. Defenses from Vaillant’s category of neurotic mechanisms are not prominent in dementing disorders. Some persons appear to accept their impairment, but then demonstrate by their behavior that they simultaneously do not recognize any impairment. Displacement is occasionally employed, as indicated in the following case of Alzheimer’s disease: A 67-year-old widow was being evaluated for symptoms of dementia that had progressed over a 4-year period. When asked if she had any difficulties, she replied that she had a painful ankle and showed her leather-braced ankle to the examiner. She said that since she had started wearing the brace, the arthritis in her ankle was considerably less troublesome. Later, the examiner began a formal inquiry into her cognitive state, asking questions to test her memory, concentration, and fund of information. When at a loss for an answer to a question, she would reply that her ankle was hurting and interfering with her concentration.
Other persons develop reaction formations against their impairment, putting in longer hours at work to compensate for their slowed thinking and impaired comprehension. On one occasion, it was necessary to hospitalize a man with early dementia who had become delirious from working long hours and losing sleep in an effort to keep up with his work. The catastrophic reactions (Goldstein 1942) that occasionally occur when cognitively impaired persons are faced with an intense emotional
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process
57
stimulus or unmanageable cognitive input may take the form of a confused state. The person suddenly becomes unable to recognize a familiar environment or undergoes a sudden emotional regression and begins calling for long-dead parents or other relatives as though they were still alive. This is illustrated in the following example: A 70-year-old man with Alzheimer’s disease became frightened when he was unable to recognize his wife and began calling out for his mother. He remained in this excited state until he was calmed by his son, whom he was able to recognize.
Catastrophic reactions are usually self-limited and are best contained by distraction or by removing or making understandable the aggravating environmental stimuli. Immature defense mechanisms are often employed by dementia patients. Projection in the form of blaming (as opposed to delusional projection) often occurs early in the course of a dementing illness. A wife will exclaim with irritation that her husband did not tell her the children were coming to visit. An impaired husband will rage at his wife for hiding the car keys that he misplaced. The potential use of fantasy as a defense mechanism is impaired because of attention and memory deficits, but somatic preoccupation is often encountered, with a host of physical complaints being offered to distract from the potential awareness of decreased cognitive ability. Rather than passive opposition (which involves the ability to inhibit an impulse), cognitively impaired persons often express direct aggression in the form of blaming. Acting out is also common, precisely because these individuals lack the ability to inhibit their behavior. Thus, inappropriate aggression or sexual display may sometimes occur. For example, a nursing home roommate is screamed at for intruding instead of being politely asked to leave, or open masturbation may occur in response to genital stimulation from tight clothes. Psychotic defense mechanisms are seen frequently in dementia patients. The denial of impairment is common, as was previously noted. Distortion is illustrated by a dementia patient thinking that he or she is in a prison rather than a nursing home. Delusional projection may occur when a dementia patient who is unaware of having a defective memory attributes an inability to keep up with house keys to others deliberately removing them. Persons with dementing illness occasionally develop a transient delusional system to support such contentions, as seen in the following example: A woman with severe cognitive impairment who was unable to comprehend her environment, dealt with the problem by refusing to leave her house to
58
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
venture into an unfamiliar environment. She responded to all visitors as intruders, calling them whores and attempting to drive them off in an effort to keep her environment simple and comprehensible.
Delusion formation also occurs without the element of projection, especially in persons who develop prosopagnosia, the inability to identify faces. A man with prosopagnosia became very concerned when he failed to recognize his wife while on a trip. He became agitated and exclaimed that he was being kidnapped. Such delusions are usually transient and tend to occur when the environment becomes too demanding. Regression, another prominent mechanism employed by cognitively impaired persons, is reverting to adaptive mechanisms appropriate to an earlier era of life, usually early childhood. Regression can take many forms. It may manifest as infantile clinging to loved ones, avoidance of strangers, tantrums, inappropriate or unnecessary dependence, or attempts to obtain special attention from others. When regression takes the form of physical clinging to other persons, dolls or stuffed animals can often be substituted for the constant presence of another person. This parallels the use of socalled transitional objects to serve the emotional needs of young children who also lack object constancy—the ability to recognize that an object no longer in reach or in view can continue to exist (Piaget 1954). Whether such cognitive regression occurs in dementing illness has not been formally tested, but clinical experience suggests that it probably does occur. And, as with a young child, reassurances that one will return are often insufficient. A young child becomes upset recalling the unpleasurable experience of loss; a cognitively impaired person becomes upset in some measure because of inability to remember the promise to return. The psychological mechanism of splitting, which is frequently evident in dementing illness, may be part of the regressive process. This mechanism, theorized by Klein (1957) to be characteristic of an early stage of personality development, involves the active separation of good from bad experiences, perceptions, and emotions linked to other persons. Thus, the frustrating aspects of an emotionally important person are split off from his or her caring aspects to form an entirely separate person: one is all good and the other is all bad, as seen in the following example: A woman with Alzheimer’s disease, was having increasing difficulty identifying her husband. One night, frustrated with her seemingly endless repetitiveness, her husband spoke to her with a harsh tone of voice. The woman reacted with alarm. She perceived him as having become two persons, both of whom were claiming to be her husband. Frightened and unable to determine who was her real husband, she fled the house to the home of a neighbor and had the neighbor call the police. The following day, she was still not certain which of the two men was her husband. She could not fathom
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process
59
how her real husband could be so unkind to her and resolved her psychological problem by splitting him in two.
The splitting process described in the preceding case may contribute to the development of Capgras’ syndrome.
Psychological, Behavioral, Ideational, and Perceptual Concomitants of Dementing Illness Certain frequent psychological and behavioral concomitants of dementing illness warrant individual discussion from a psychological viewpoint. They are 1) depression, 2) delirium, 3) anxiety, 4) behavioral dyscontrol, 5) sleep-wake disturbances, 6) suspiciousness and delusion formation, 7) hallucinations, and 8) apathy and withdrawal. The catastrophic response has already been discussed.
Depression Major depression occurs in some series in 10%–20% of Alzheimer’s disease patients (Wragg and Jeste 1988). In our own dementia clinic, major depression was present in only 1% of Alzheimer’s disease patients at initial evaluation (Weiner et al. 1991), and there were no new cases of major depression in 153 patients followed up for an average of 3 years (Weiner et al. 1994). However, our experience may not reflect the true prevalence of major depression in Alzheimer’s disease because ours is a tertiary care clinic. Presumably, most cases of major depression would have been diagnosed and treated elsewhere. On the other hand, symptoms usually associated with depression are frequent in Alzheimer’s disease (Cummings et al. 1995), with agitation, motor restlessness, sleep disturbance, and tearful episodes occurring in 25%–50% of patients in one series (Reisberg et al. 1987). Chen et al. (1999), who prospectively studied a large series of elders, found that a “depression cluster” of five or more DSM-III-R depression symptoms (including depressed mood) followed the onset of dementia (15.4%) and Alzheimer’s disease (17.6%) compared with nondemented subjects (3.2%). Among other dementia-associated disorders, depression occurs in Parkinson’s disease (Cummings 1992), Huntington’s disease, multiple sclerosis, epilepsy, brain trauma, and stroke (Starkstein and Robinson 1993). The prevalence of depression is high during the first 2 years after a stroke (Robinson and Price 1982). The relationship between depression and the size, location, or cognitive impairment of stroke is controversial (Robinson et al. 1983; Schwartz et al. 1993), but depression does appear to magnify the cognitive effects of the stroke.
60
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
There is evidence that depression occurring in old age may herald the onset of a dementing illness, whether the depressive episode is one of many lifelong depressive or manic-depressive episodes or whether it arises de novo in old age. In one study, 57% of depressed nondemented elderly patients developed frank dementia within 3 years (Reding et al. 1985). In a study of 14 patients still alive at 15- to 47-month follow-up, 43% had developed dementia (Bulbena and Berrios 1986). Dhingra and Rabins (1991) found that of 25 bipolar patients age 60 years or older followed up for 5–7 years, 32% had developed significant cognitive impairment. It has been suggested that episodes of mood disorder may do damage to the brain, possibly caused by the accompanying hypersecretion of cortisol (Altshuler 1993). More recent large prospective studies have not added strong confirmation to earlier studies. Speck et al. (1995), using a case-control design, compared 294 probable Alzheimer’s disease patients with 300 age- and sexmatched control subjects. Depression was defined in this study as depression for which medical/psychological consultation, medication, or hospitalization occurred, and excluded treatment for grief reactions. The odds ratio for Alzheimer’s disease was 1.8 (95% confidence interval = 0.9–3.5) after adjusting for age, gender, education, and type of informant. When stratified by year of onset, the association between depression and Alzheimer’s disease was present only in the group with depressive symptoms preceding cognitive impairment by 10 or more years (odds ratio of 2.0, 95% confidence interval = 0.9–4.6). In another prospective community-based study, 954 nondemented persons 65 years or older were followed every 2 years over 8 years (Chen et al. 1999). In this group, there was an association between Alzheimer’s disease and depression defined by the presence of five or more depressive symptoms, including depressed mood (odds ratio = 6.5, 95% confidence interval = 2.2–19.1), but depression did not confer significantly greater risk for the development of dementia or Alzheimer’s disease. Major depression appears to be a psychosomatic-somatopsychic disorder involving the interaction of genetically determined and environmentally induced vulnerabilities with intercurrent psychological and physiological factors. In a study of poststroke depression, for example, 20% of the depressed patients had a prestroke history of depression (Schwartz et al. 1993). Two studies (Pearlson et al. 1990; Strauss and Ogrocki 1996) found that depressed Alzheimer’s disease patients with no previous history of depression had significantly more first-degree relatives with depression than did nondepressed Alzheimer’s disease patients. In a prospective study, Heim et al. (2000) studied four groups of women: control subjects with no history of childhood abuse or psychiatric disorder, women with childhood abuse and no major depression, women with childhood abuse and depres-
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process
61
sion, and women with depression and no childhood abuse. Women with childhood abuse had increased adrenocorticotropic hormone and heart rate response to stress; this increased response was even greater in women with childhood abuse and depression or anxiety. Abnormalities of the norepinephrine and serotonin systems appear to be important in the development of depressive illness, but their dysfunction is likely due to their role in modulating and being modulated by other neurobiological systems (Ressler and Nemeroff 2000). There is considerable evidence for dysfunction of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in major depression, including increased basal cortisol levels, dexamethasone-mediated negative feedback resistance, increased cerebrospinal fluid levels of corticotropinreleasing factor, and blunted adrenocorticotropic hormone response to challenge with exogenous corticotropin-releasing factor (Plotsky et al. 1998). However, these seem to be state rather than trait markers. Depression may be triggered by physiologic or psychological factors or by both. At first, an amotivational state appears. This state may result from catecholamine dysregulation, neuroendocrine factors, or an exaggerated phylogenetically determined reaction to a situation experienced as an overwhelming threat to psychological or physical survival. This state was described as conservation withdrawal by Engel and Schmale (1972) and was later suggested by Weiner and Lovitt (1979) as a possible physiologic substrate of depression. The physiologic reaction, once established, becomes partially autonomous and unresponsive to changes in the precipitating psychological factors. Even so, it is useful to conceptualize depression psychodynamically. The utility of the psychodynamic conceptualization of depression is that it suggests a means for patients’ psychological management. Injury to or death of neurons depletes the capacity of the ego to deal effectively with the demands of the id, superego, ego ideal, and external reality. Subclinical brain damage also interferes with the ability to live up to one’s ego ideal and results in guilt and lowering of self-esteem. There is an increased danger of id impulses erupting or irrational superego demands increasing at the same time that the ego is having greater difficulty interpreting environmental stimuli. Given a biological vulnerability to depression, overt depression may manifest when triggered by stressors such as illness, brain damage, or aging. The most common of these stresses is loss, including loss of a loved one, loss of health, loss of meaningful occupation, and so on (Engel 1962). These losses may be real, but symbolic or threatened losses may be equally powerful precipitants of depression. Thus, the illness of a spouse (or other loved one) or a spouse’s threat to leave can be as potent a trigger as the death of a spouse, as illustrated in the following example:
62
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
An 82-year-old retired ironworker became increasingly withdrawn and apathetic after his daughter’s divorce and the near loss of a favorite grandson in a motorcycle accident. His sleep pattern was disturbed with initial, middle, and terminal insomnia. He reacted to his slowing of bowel function by refusing to eat, for fear that food would block his intestinal tract. The patient had no history of emotional disorder before experiencing a stroke some 10 years previously. This was his third episode of severe depression since that time. At the time of his first depressive episode, he developed the delusion that federal agents were pursuing him for tax evasion. He had previously responded well to electroconvulsive therapy; therefore, he was given five such treatments, because his depression was refractory to treatment with antidepressants and because of the immediate danger of inanition. His mood responded well to that treatment.
Depression may not be accompanied by guilt (Harrow and Amdur 1971); in my experience, depression in the elderly is less often accompanied by guilt and self-derogation than is depression in younger adults. It more commonly appears as apathy and listlessness out of proportion to that person’s physical state. When pathological ideation is present, it may involve delusions of persecution for misdeeds, as indicated in the previous example. In the case of a 92-year-old nursing home patient with cognitive impairment, delusions centered on her being possessed by the devil and the nursing home staff planning to get rid of her because she was too demanding.
Suicidal Behavior and Suicide Suicidal behavior and completed suicide in persons with dementing illness may result from depression or from a combination of factors including depression (Rubio et al. 2001). Such factors may include other physical illness, social isolation, diminishing financial reserves, diminishing quality of life, and concern over burdening others. Depression-related suicidal ideation usually diminishes when the depression is treated successfully. There is not a great danger of suicide in cognitively impaired patients who are unaware of their deficits and who deny or do not comprehend their prognosis. On the other hand, persons with progressive dementing illnesses who are painfully aware of their cognitive deficits, their growing dependence on others, and the potential outcome of their disease may choose to end their own lives. They may make successful suicide attempts without being significantly depressed (Rohde et al. 1995). There is a strong association between Huntington’s disease and suicide. In a review of 831 Huntington’s disease patients from the National Huntington’s Disease Research Roster, 5.7% of deaths among affected persons resulted from suicide; 27.6% of patients had attempted suicide at least once (Farrer 1986). A review of death certificates for 395 Danish subjects with
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process
63
Huntington’s disease and 282 unaffected siblings showed that suicide accounted for 5.6% of deaths among those affected with Huntington’s disease compared with 2.7% in the general Danish population (Sorensen and Fenger 1992). Risk factors were identified in 9 persons affected with Huntington’s disease. Of these, 8 were male; 6 were single or divorced. Duration of illness was 1 to 14 years. The largest risk factor in the Huntington’s disease patients was having no children. Other factors were living alone, other suicides in the family, contact with other Huntington’s disease patients, and depression (Lipe et al. 1993). The availability of genetic testing for Huntington’s disease raises the issue of risk for those certain to develop the disease but who are not yet affected. Ethical aspects of such testing is discussed in Chapter 10. A survey of Huntington’s disease predictive centers worldwide showed that a total of 44 persons (0.97%) in a cohort of 4,527 test participants had catastrophic events: 5 suicides, 21 suicide attempts, and 18 psychiatric hospitalizations (Almqvist et al. 1999). All persons who committed suicide were symptomatic, whereas only half of the persons attempting suicide and 44% of those with psychiatric hospitalization were symptomatic. Increased risk in this series was associated with unemployment and history of psychiatric illness within 5 years of testing. Lawson et al. (1996) suggest that all persons entering into predictive testing with some evidence of depression warrant special vigilance and that counseling and support should be available for all participants in such testing.
Delirium From the psychological standpoint, delirium is the result of rapidly developing impairment of coping or adaptive abilities, manifesting when the individual becomes acutely unable to understand the environment and experiences overload from internal or external stimuli. The cardinal features of delirium include reduced clarity of awareness of the environment; reduced ability to focus, sustain, and shift attention; and cognitive deficits, including memory disturbance and disorientation (see also Table 1–4). These symptoms develop acutely and often fluctuate in severity during the day, becoming most pronounced at night. There is often a prodromal stage of disturbed sleep, bad dreams, and increasing anxiety (Lipowski 1987). Delirium can occur in a person whose nervous system is intact if the systemic illness is severe and has a rapid onset. An example is the sudden onset of pneumococcal pneumonia accompanied by high fever. More often, there is an antecedent vulnerability of the central nervous system, whether due to central nervous system immaturity or damage. Delirium that occurs in response to trivial insults is virtually pathognomonic of an underlying impairment of brain function. In the medical setting, the development of delirium
64
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
in elders is frequent; it often predicts poor functional outcome. For example, among 54 consecutive elderly patients treated surgically for femoral neck fracture, the incidence of postoperative delirium was 27.8% (Edlund et al. 1999). Dementia and prolonged waiting time for surgery were the two principal risk factors. A significantly greater percentage of persons who became delirious were unable to return to their previous dwelling or were either dead, wheelchair bound, or bedridden 6 months after surgery. From the Freudian perspective, persons who experience a sudden increase in brain damage become unable to adequately repress the contents of the unconscious part of the mind and to interpret environmental stimuli. Fragmented, repressed memories may come pouring forth, and persons in the environment become confused with important personages from the past. Therefore, it is easy to understand how a person can appear intact when admitted to a hospital in the company of a familiar loved one but can become delirious in the recovery room with no familiar person, no orienting cues, and sensorium clouded by anesthetic and analgesic medication. Despite the ease of a psychological explanation, each case of delirium warrants medical investigation for conditions such as an undetected stroke (Dunne et al. 1986). The following case is an illustration of delirium: A 67-year-old man, had sought help for memory problems of 1–2 years’ duration. He was treated for presumed normal-pressure hydrocephalus with a ventriculoperitoneal shunt. To his disappointment (and that of his family), no change occurred in his cognitive state. In addition, he was very disturbed by a flare-up of his daughter’s chronic illness. As a result, he became increasingly depressed. He was seen by a psychiatrist who elicited a history of weight loss, appetite loss, decreased energy, terminal insomnia, and crying spells. His mood was depressed, he had psychomotor retardation, and he cried during the interview. His concentration, attention, and recall were all markedly impaired. He saw himself as only a shadow of his former self. The psychiatrist prescribed 40 mg of imipramine per day in divided doses in addition to the 50 mg of diphenhydramine and 15 mg of flurazepam that he was already taking at bedtime to help him sleep. Because there was only slight amelioration of symptoms after 2 weeks, the imipramine dose was increased to 50 mg daily. Two weeks later, the psychiatrist received a call from the patient’s internist, who said that the patient had become psychotic. When interviewed at the hospital to which he had been admitted by the internist, the patient was disoriented to time and space. He thought that his wife was trying to poison him. He insisted that he was in a brothel and that the nurses were trying to solicit him.
In this case, the patient was experiencing a delirium brought on by the three medications he was taking, two of which had strong anticholinergic effects. When his medications were discontinued, his delirium cleared, his mood normalized, and there was no further need for the antidepressant medication.
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process
65
Anxiety Anxiety is often experienced in early dementing illness. Many individuals report a fear of losing their mental capacities and a sense of helplessness to understand and deal with their environment. If the personality is totally overwhelmed by new or complex environmental demands, a catastrophic response or a delirium may occur. As individuals become more compromised cognitively, the conscious experience of anxiety may no longer be communicated because of impaired language. Patients may also lose insight into their dementing illness. Agitation and restlessness supervene as alarm responses and as preparation for fighting against or flight from an incomprehensible environment. Teri et al. (1999) found a 70% prevalence of anxiety symptoms in community-dwelling Alzheimer’s disease patients. Chemerinski et al. (1998) found that 5% of 398 persons with probable Alzheimer’s disease met criteria for generalized anxiety disorder during the 4 weeks before undergoing a standardized psychiatric examination. An examination of 92 persons with vascular dementia showed that 72% had two or more anxiety symptoms, with higher prevalence (94%) in persons with severe dementia (Ballard et al. 2000).
Behavioral Dyscontrol Behavioral self-control is related to intactness of executive function: the ability to think abstractly and plan, initiate, sequence, monitor, and stop complex behavior. Although it is not localized to any portion of the brain, executive function is frequently related to the frontal lobes. Impairment of executive function reduces ability to learn from experience, to understand the impact of one’s behavior on others, or to regulate that behavior. Executive dysfunction can manifest as loss of initiative, impulsivity, emotional indifference, or labile mood. Impairment of self-observation manifests in poor personal hygiene and inappropriate interpersonal conduct. Persons with impaired executive function may indulge in inappropriate sexual activity or display; they may develop abnormal eating patterns such as hyperphagia or may readily eat from the plates of others in restaurants or institutions. One form of behavioral dyscontrol has been termed agitation.
Agitation The term agitation has been accepted into the informal nosology of dementia-related behavior disturbances. Agitation is an atheoretical descriptive term that includes behaviors originally observed in a cohort of
66
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
demented and nondemented nursing home residents (Cohen-Mansfield and Billig 1986). The definition given by these authors was “inappropriate verbal, vocal, or motor activity that is not explained by needs or confusion per se” (Cohen-Mansfield and Billig 1986, p. 712). In other words, the term did not apply to persons crying out because they were hungry, because their incontinence pads needed changing, or because they were ignored or abused. Behaviors included under this designation of agitation form the basis of the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (CMAI) (CohenMansfield 1986) and include such diverse behaviors as hitting, kicking, pacing, complaining, and screaming. Thus, the term agitation applies to vocal or physical behaviors whose origins or meanings are not understood. Because these behaviors are so heterogeneous, it seems unlikely that they constitute a syndrome, that they have a common etiology, or that they will respond as a group to specific behavioral, environmental, or psychopharmacologic interventions.
Sleep-Wake Disturbances Sleep-wake disturbances are common among persons with dementing illness and are an important factor in caregivers’ decisions to institutionalize them (Pollack and Perlick 1991). The most common sleep-wake disturbances are hypersomnia, insomnia, and reversal of the sleep-wake cycle. A study of demented and nondemented nursing home patients showed highly fragmented sleep, with patients averaging only 40 minutes of sleep per hour during the night (Ancoli-Israel et al. 1989). Comparing Alzheimer’s disease patients with age-matched control subjects, Bliwise et al. (1989) found no difference in the total amount of sleep. However, Alzheimer’s disease patients had poorer quality of sleep, as evidenced by lower mean sleep efficiency, lower percentages of stage 3 and 4 sleep, and a higher percentage of stage 1 sleep. Moe et al. (1995) found in Alzheimer’s disease patients that wakefulness during the night and greater rapid eye movement latency were associated with greater cognitive and functional impairment, suggesting that the neural substrate underlying these processes degenerate at comparable rates. Cognitively impaired persons who are not active frequently doze during the day because of boredom and are awake and restless at night because they have already obtained their required sleep during their naps. Elders’ sleep is often disturbed by pain, by the need to urinate, or by the need to take medications. Stimulants such as coffee, tea, or bronchodilators may contribute to the problem. Obstructive sleep apnea may occur, resulting in frequent sleep interruption, poor quality of sleep, and daytime hypersomnolence (Guilleminault et al. 1973).
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process
67
Hypersomnia Medications to control agitation or treat insomnia are highly sedative. Anticonvulsants and antihistamines are as well. Metabolic disturbances stemming from chronic lung disease, renal failure, or liver failure also cause somnolence. From the environmental standpoint, the chief cause of daytime sleepiness is a lack of engaging activities. Sleeping excessively is a way to deal with boredom and to evade the challenge of trying to understand the environment or to initiate behavior. Sometimes caregivers promote daytime sleeping to ease their burden. Unfortunately, this may promote nighttime wakefulness. A sleeping person is easier to manage than a person who is restless and demanding and does not understand how or why certain things must be done, such as urinating or defecating in the toilet.
Insomnia The inability of person with dementing illness to sleep the night through is a common complaint of caregivers. Family caregivers look forward to and need relief at night; nursing homes and other institutions are staffed lightly at night. Thus, both family and institutional caregivers bear an extra burden when their charges cannot sleep. Sleep is interrupted by anxiety, but the most common psychiatric syndrome leading to sleep loss is depression. Severely depressed persons have difficulty falling asleep and staying asleep, often waking 3–4 hours before their usual time and dreading the day ahead. Excessive stimulant intake in the form of caffeine in coffee, tea, or soft drinks or theophylline for bronchopulmonary disease adds to sleep difficulty. In addition, if there is little daytime physical activity, there is little physiological demand for rest.
Reversal of the Sleep-Wake Cycle Daytime napping and nighttime awakening may lead to reversal of the sleep-wake cycle. For elderly persons, getting to sleep and staying asleep are made difficult by painful arthritis and the need to urinate during the night, the latter being aggravated by diuretics to control blood pressure or to ameliorate heart failure. Sleep apnea, with its frequent interruptions of sleep, is also more common in elderly persons. More often than not, sleep difficulty is produced or aggravated by environmental factors, such as scheduling bedtime too early. When examined objectively, many individuals are found to be sleeping 6–8 hours, but their sleep time begins at 8 P.M. and ends at 2–4 A.M.—with attempts at getting up and beginning the day. In many cases, individuals get sufficient sleep from napping during the day.
68
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
In a preliminary study (Satlin et al. 1991), exposure of 10 late-stage Alzheimer’s disease patients to 2,000 lux of light from 7–9 P.M. for 1 week increased evening wakefulness and reduced nighttime activity, with return to former levels 1 week after discontinuance of exposure to bright light.
Suspiciousness and Delusion Formation Many persons attempt to compensate for their impairment with hyperalertness. In a hyperalert state, one becomes aware of many environmental stimuli that are not readily understood, so that the coping mechanism of hyperalertness leads to increasing confusion. Suspiciousness is a variant of the hyperalert or hypervigilant state in which stimuli are interpreted as dangerous. In a situation in which it is not possible to distinguish what is safe from what is dangerous, it makes sense to first view everything as dangerous. It is also frustrating and aggravating to find so much that cannot be understood in a world that one could formerly grasp. What better explanation than the explanation that attempts are being made to deceive? In this way, defective perception and integration can be denied, and it is no longer necessary to explain every event individually or to try to understand each confusing stimulus. The answer is that one is being deliberately deceived. For example, a man with dementia does not perceive himself as being unable to identify his wife. Instead, he regards the woman who says she is married to him as an impostor. A final important aspect of delusional thinking is that it enables action. Perceiving danger as external enables one to avoid it or to attack it, thus reducing the unpleasant feeling of helplessness that comes from being confused. Anxiety is dealt with in the same way. No longer able to recognize that anxiety results from inner tension, the person with dementia experiences fear and attributes it to the external environment. Common delusional beliefs include believing that others are in the house (phantom boarders), that one is not in one’s own home, that one’s spouse is an impostor (Capgras’ syndrome), and that deceased loved ones are still alive. The delusional beliefs of persons with dementing illness can often be temporarily reduced by gentle confrontation with reality, in contrast to such beliefs of persons with mental illnesses such as schizophrenia.
Confabulation and Related Phenomena When individuals with severe memory impairment, such as those with amnestic syndromes, are asked questions, they often supply elaborate, incorrect answers. When questioned about their place of residence, for example, individuals living at home with a spouse may indicate that they live in a
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process
69
hotel and may provide details about the hotel that are often based on earlier experiences in life. Confabulations do not have the emotional force of delusions, nor do confabulators cling to their stories. Other cognitively impaired persons speak of long-deceased persons as though they were still present without giving evidence that they have experienced hallucinations. Or they will look for children who long ago grew up and moved away. Common questions by these individuals are, “Where has mother gone?” or “Where are the children? It’s time for them to go to school.”
Delusional Syndromes A number of delusional syndromes have been reported in cognitively impaired persons. They include Capgras’ syndrome (Burns and Philpot 1987) and Cotard’s syndrome. In the former, there is the fixed belief that there are doubles of others, and there may be the belief that there are doubles of one’s self. Merriam et al. (1988) found Capgras-like symptoms to occur in 17% of Alzheimer’s disease patients. In Cotard’s syndrome, individuals believe that parts of their bodies are missing, that they do not exist, or that the world about them does not exist. In dealing with Cotard’s syndrome, the clinician must suspect major depression in addition to dementing illness. Alzheimer’s disease patients also often express the notion that an unseen person is living in the house; the so-called phantom boarder syndrome (Rubin et al. 1988). Patients who are unable to recognize themselves in a mirror may develop the delusion that another person is present. One of our patients became fearful of entering an automobile because he always saw the face of the same person in the window glass on entering a car.
Illusions Illusions occur commonly in cognitively impaired persons. An illusion is a mistaken perception: mistaking one thing for something else because of a real or imagined resemblance. For example, a man with dementia mistakes his wife for his mother. She resembles his mother in that she is roughly his mother’s age when she was last alive, and the notion of “mother” is nurturing and comforting. An illusion that becomes a fixed belief is a delusion.
Hallucinations Hallucinations are false perceptions; that is, they are reports of sensory impressions that cannot be consensually validated. Hallucinations are frequent in delirium and dementia. Delirious persons frequently envision animals crawling up the walls or feel ants crawling on their skin. Dementia
70
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
patients often report children or small adults running in and out of their homes or airplanes flying overhead. They frequently watch in fascination but often become excited and upset. Hallucinations can be explained psychologically in the same way as delusions. They are means to organize an otherwise incomprehensible environment and are often determined by personal needs or wishes, as exemplified by the frequent hallucination of loved ones who are no longer living. Hallucinations may also have a physiological component; that is, they may be stimulated by abnormal electrical discharges of the brain, as in partial complex seizures, which often have an olfactory aura of an unpleasant smell such as burning feathers or burning rubber. Just as it does with incomprehensible environmental stimuli, the mind can organize incomprehensible sensory stimuli from within the brain into perceptions that can be understood and reacted to (Weiner 1961).
The Charles Bonnet Syndrome Patients with Alzheimer’s disease frequently experience a type of visual hallucinatory state or hallucinosis that may or may not be disturbing to them. Often described as the Charles Bonnet syndrome, and often attributed to visual impairment, the state consists of seeing formed, complex, often stereotyped visual hallucinations. Patients often have the sense that they are not real. There are no accompanying delusions, and hallucinations are not present in other sensory modalities (Gold and Rabins 1989). One of our patients with Alzheimer’s disease saw people in his house with Xs for eyes. They were never the same people from day to day, and none was recognizable as anyone he knew. He was mildly puzzled by the presence of these people, would talk to them (they did not reply), and was able to tolerate their presence very well. Another Alzheimer’s disease patient saw individuals with peculiarly colored hair. They seemed to appear in groups. They did not speak. Some of them had only half-bodies that they propelled with their hands. This type of hallucinatory experience has been reported to occur commonly in the Lewy body variant of Alzheimer’s disease and in diffuse Lewy body disease.
Apathy and Withdrawal Apathy has become the subject of a large body of recent literature. The term is derived from the Greek a (without) + pathos (feeling or passion) (Thomas 1889). The term has been expanded to include the absence or lack of feeling, emotion, interest, concern, or motivation (Robinson and Starkstein 2000), but the motor aspect of apathy is properly termed abulia, from the Greek a (without) + boulon (will) (Thomas 1889). The latter term was
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process
71
reintroduced by Adams and Fisher (Fisher 1984) to describe the slowness and loss of spontaneity in cerebral disease ranging from its mild form, apathy, to the extreme form, akinetic mutism. Apathy may be a direct result of or a psychological reaction to brain injury. Apathy, as manifested by social withdrawal, is one of the earliest signs of Alzheimer’s disease, antedating the clinical diagnosis by 2 years or more (Jost and Grossberg 1996). Apathy is often associated with injury to frontal convexity and is a frequent concomitant of frontotemporal degenerative disorders; it also occurs after closed head injury (Kant et al. 1998) and in individuals who are seropositive for human immunodeficiency virus (Castellon et al. 1998). In a series of persons with traumatic brain injury, stroke, and hypoxic brain injury, apathy was associated with subcortical and right hemisphere damage (Andersson et al. 1999). Although apathy is frequently associated with depression, for example, in left hemisphere stroke (Marin et al. 1994), many depressed persons do not display apathy. Marin et al. (1991) showed that apathy as detected by the Apathy Evaluation Scale was distinguishable from anxiety and depression. Levy et al. (1998) used the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (Cummings et al. 1994) to evaluate the relationship between depression and apathy in patients with Alzheimer’s disease, frontotemporal dementia, Parkinson’s disease, Huntington’s disease, and progressive supranuclear palsy. Apathy did not correlate with depression in the combined sample. Although apathy was related to decreased cognitive function, depression was not. Apathy can also be a psychological reaction to an environment perceived as hostile, unrewarding, or incomprehensible. As a result of feeling overwhelmed, cognitively impaired patients first withdraw into an environment that they can comprehend. When that fails, they may become apathetic and withdraw emotionally as well as physically.
Summary Viewing the symptoms of dementing illness as the product of a dynamic interaction between biological and psychological forces enables clinicians to recognize that fluctuations in cognitive and behavioral/emotional symptoms in persons with dementing illnesses are the norm. However, when confronted with increased disorientation in a cognitively impaired person, clinicians must consider whether the increased confusion or behavioral/emotional symptoms result from progression of the underlying brain disease, metabolic factors, the onset of depression, change in the relationship between the patient and others, or change in the physical environment.
72
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
The clinician’s intervention will depend on which variable or group of variables predominates. Having made an adequate diagnosis of the disease underlying the presenting symptoms, clinicians perform appropriate examinations, order appropriate tests, and assay the effects of interpersonal and environmental changes when the clinical course appears out of line with what is known of the general progress of the illness. In some cases, cognitive function or behavioral dyscontrol will improve when cardiac function is improved, when a urinary tract infection is treated, when the patient is better hydrated, when the environment is simplified by maintaining the same routine every day, or when the person is kept in a familiar environment. In other cases, there will be no change or the patient’s status may actually worsen. The clinician will again investigate to determine whether there are adverse effects from medications or unexpected reactions to interpersonal or environmental changes. In all cases, the patient’s level of functioning is the result of a delicate balance between factors that become increasingly difficult to evaluate as dementing illnesses progress.
References Almqvist EW, Bloch M, Brinkman R, et al: A worldwide assessment of the frequency of suicide, suicide attempts, or psychiatric hospitalization after predictive testing for Huntington disease. Am J Hum Genet 64:1293–1304, 1999 Altshuler L: Bipolar disorder: are repeated episodes associated with neuroanatomic and cognitive changes? Biol Psychiatry 33:563–565, 1993 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 2000 Ancoli-Israel S, Parker L, Sinaee R, et al: Sleep fragmentation in patients from a nursing home. J Gerontol 44:M18–M21, 1989 Andersson S, Krogstad JM, Finset A: Apathy and depressed mood in acquired brain damage: relationship to lesion localization and psychophysiological reactivity. Psychol Med 29:447–456, 1999 Bahro M, Silber E, Sunderland T: How do patients with Alzheimer’s disease cope with their illness? A clinical experience report. J Am Geriatr Soc 43:41–46, 1995 Ballard C, Neill D, O’Brien J, et al: Anxiety, depression and psychosis in vascular dementia: prevalence and associations. J Affect Disord 59:97–106, 2000 Bliwise DL, Tinklenberg J, Yesavage JA, et al: REM latency in Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 25:320–328, 1989 Bulbena A, Berrios GE: Pseudodementia: facts and figures. Br J Psychiatry 148:87– 94, 1986 Burns A, Philpot M: Capgras’ syndrome in a patient with dementia. Br J Psychiatry 150:876–877, 1987
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process
73
Cahill L, Prins B, Weber M, et al: >-Adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events. Nature 371:702–704, 1994 Castellon SA, Hinkin CH, Wood S, et al: Apathy, depression, and cognitive performance in HIV-1 infection. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 10:320–329, 1998 Chemerinski E, Petracca G, Manes F, et al: Prevalence and correlates of anxiety in Alzheimer’s disease. Depress Anxiety 7:166–170, 1998 Chen P, Ganguli M, Mulsant BH, et al: The temporal relationship between depressive symptoms and dementia: a community-based prospective study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56:261–266, 1999 Cohen-Mansfield J: Agitated behaviors in the elderly, II: preliminary results in the cognitively deteriorated. J Am Geriatr Soc 34:722–727, 1986 Cohen-Mansfield J, Billig N: Agitated behaviors in the elderly, I: a conceptual review. J Am Geriatr Soc 34:711–721, 1986 Cummings JL: Depression and Parkinson’s disease: a review. Am J Psychiatry 149:443–454, 1992 Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray KF, et al: The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 44:2308– 2314, 1994 Cummings JL, Ross W, Absher J, et al: Depressive symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease: assessment and determinants. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 9:87–93 1995 Denny-Brown D, Meyer JS, Horenstein S: Amorphosynthesis from left parietal lesion. Arch Neurol 71:302–313, 1954 Derouesne C, Thibault S, Lagha-Pierucci S, et al: Decreased awareness of cognitive deficits in patients with mild dementia of the Alzheimer type. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 14:1019–1030, 1999 Dhingra U, Rabins PV: Mania in the elderly: a 5–7 year follow-up. J Am Geriatr Soc 39:581–583, 1991 Dunne JW, Leedman PJ, Edis RH: Inobvious stroke: a cause of delirium and dementia. Aust N Z J Med 16:771–778, 1986 Edlund A, Lundstrom M, Lundstrom G, et al: Clinical profile of delirium in patients treated for femoral neck fractures. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 10:325–329, 1999 Engel GE: Psychological Development in Health and Disease. Philadelphia, PA, WB Saunders, 1962 Engel GE, Schmale AH: Conservation withdrawal: a primary regulatory process of organismic homeostasis, in Physiology, Emotion and Psychosomatic Illness: CIBA Foundation Symposium No 8. Amsterdam, Elsevier–Excerpta Medica, 1972, pp 57–85 Farrer LA: Suicide and attempted suicide in Huntington disease: implications for preclinical testing of persons at risk. Am J Med Genet 24:305–311, 1986 Fisher CM: Abulia minor vs agitated behavior. Clin Neurosurg 31:9–31, 1984 Freud A: The Ego and the Mechanisms of Defense. New York, International Universities Press, 1946
74
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Gold K, Rabins PV: Isolated visual hallucinations and the Charles Bonnet syndrome: a review of the literature and presentation of six cases. Compr Psychiatry 30:90–98, 1989 Goldstein K: After Effects of Brain Injuries in War. New York, Grune & Stratton, 1942 Guilleminault C, Eldridge FL, Dement WC: Insomnia with sleep apnea: a new syndrome. Science 181:856–858, 1973 Harrow M, Amdur MJ: Guilt and depressive disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 25: 240–246, 1971 Heim C, Newport DJ, Heit S, et al: Pituitary-adrenal and autonomic responses to stress in women after sexual and physical abuse in childhood. JAMA 284:592– 597, 2000 Heuer F, Reisberg D. Vivid memories of emotional events: the accuracy of remembered minutiae. Mem Cognit 18:496–506, 1990 Jost BC, Grossberg GT: The evolution of psychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease: a naturalistic study. J Am Geriatr Soc 44:1078–1085, 1996 Kant R, Duffy JD, Pivovarnik A: Prevalence of apathy following head injury. Brain Inj 12:87–92, 1998 Klein M: Envy and Gratitude. London, Tavistock, 1957 Lawson K, Wiggins S, Green T, et al: Adverse psychological events occurring in the first year after predictive testing for Huntington’s disease. The Canadian Collaborative Study of Predictive Testing. J Med Genet 33:856–862, 1996 Levy ML, Cummings JL, Fairbanks LA, et al: Apathy is not depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 10:314–319, 1998 Lipe H, Schultz A, Bird TB: Risk factors for suicide in Huntington’s disease: a retrospective case controlled study. Am J Med Genet 48:231–233, 1993 Lipowski ZJ: Delirium (acute confusional states). JAMA 258:1789–1792, 1987 Marin RS, Biedrzycki RC, Firinciogullari S: Reliability and validity of the Apathy Evaluation Scale. Psychiatry Res 38:143–162, 1991 Marin RS, Biedrzycki RC, Firinciogullari S: Group differences in the relationship between apathy and depression. J Nerv Ment Dis 182:235–239, 1994 Merriam AE, Aronson MK, Gaston P, et al: The psychiatric symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 36:7–12, 1988 Moe KE, Vitiello MV, Larsen LH, et al: Symposium: cognitive processes and sleep disturbances: sleep/wake patterns in Alzheimer’s disease: relationships with cognition and function. J Sleep Res 4:15–20, 1995 Pearlson GD, Ross CA, Lohr WD, et al: Association between family history of affective disorder and the depressive syndrome of Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 147:452–456, 1990 Piaget J: The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York, Basic Books, 1954 Plotsky PM, Owens MJ, Nemeroff CB: Psychoneuroendocrinology of depression. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Psychiatr Clin North Am 21:293–307, 1998 Pollack CP, Perlick D: Sleep problems and institutionalization of the elderly. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 4:204–210, 1991
Dementing Illness as a Psychobiological Process
75
Reding M, Haycox J, Blass J: Depression in patients referred to a dementia clinic. Arch Neurol 42:894–896, 1985 Reisberg B, Borenstein J, Salob S, et al: Behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease: phenomenology and treatment. J Clin Psychiatry 48:9–15, 1987 Ressler KJ, Nemeroff CB: Role of serotonergic and noradrenergic systems in the pathophysiology of depression and anxiety disorders. Depress Anxiety 12 (suppl 1):2–19, 2000 Robinson RG, Price TR: Post-stroke depressive disorders: a follow-up study of 103 stroke outpatients. Stroke 13:635–641, 1982 Robinson RG, Starkstein SE: Neuropsychiatric aspects of cerebrovascular disorders, in Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry/VII, Seventh Edition. Edited by Sadock BJ, Sadock VA. Philadelphia, PA, Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2000, pp 242–253 Robinson RG, Starr LB, Kubos KL, et al: Mood changes in stroke patients: relationship to lesion location. Compr Psychiatry 24:555–566, 1983 Rohde K, Peskind ER, Raskind MA: Suicide in two patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 43:187–189, 1995 Rubin EH, Drevets WC, Burke WJ: The nature of psychotic symptoms in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1:16–20, 1988 Rubio A, Vertner AL, Stewart JM, et al: Suicide and Alzheimer’s pathology in the elderly: a case-control study. Biol Psychiatry 49:137–145, 2001 Satlin A, Teicher MH, Liberman HR, et al: Circadian locomotor activity rhythms in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychopharmacology 5:115–126, 1991 Schwartz JA, Speed NM, Brunberg JA, et al: Depression in stroke rehabilitation. Biol Psychiatry 33:694–699, 1993 Sevush S, Leve N: Denial of memory deficit in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 150:748–751, 1993 Sorensen SA, Fenger K: Causes of death in patients with Huntington’s disease and in unaffected first degree relatives. J Med Genet 29:911–914, 1992 Speck CE, Kukull WA, Brenner DE, et al: History of depression as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease. Epidemiology 6:366–369, 1995 Starkstein SE, Robinson RG (eds): Depression in Neurologic Disease. Baltimore, MD, Johns Hopkins University Press, 1993 Strauss ME, Ogrocki PK: Confirmation of an association between family history of affective disorder and the depressive syndrome in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 153:1340–1342, 1996 Teri L, Ferretti LE, Gibbons LE, et al: Anxiety of Alzheimer’s disease: prevalence and comorbidity. J Gerontol 54:M348–M352, 1999 Thomas J: A Complete Pronouncing Medical Dictionary. Philadelphia, PA, JB Lippincott, 1889 Vaillant GE: Adaptation to Life. Boston, MA, Little, Brown, 1977 Weiner MF: Hallucinations in children. Arch Gen Psychiatry 5:54–63, 1961 Weiner MF: Coping styles and behaviors of medical/surgical patients, in Manual of Psychiatric Consultation and Emergency Care. Edited by Guggenheim FG, Weiner MF. New York, Jason Aronson, 1984, pp 124–134
76
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Weiner MF, Lovitt R: Conservation-withdrawal versus depression. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 1:347–349, 1979 Weiner MF, Bruhn M, Svetlik DS, et al: Experiences with depression in a dementia clinic. J Clin Psychiatry 52:234–238, 1991 Weiner MF, Edland SD, Luszczynska H: Prevalence and incidence of major depression in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 151:1006–1009, 1994 Wragg RE, Jeste DV: Neuroleptics and alternative treatments: management of behavioral symptoms and psychosis in Alzheimer’s disease and related conditions. Psychiatr Clin North Am 11:195–213, 1988
CHAPTER
3
Medical Evaluation Myron F. Weiner, M.D. Anne M. Lipton, M.D., Ph.D. Ramó n Diaz-Arrastia, M.D., Ph.D.
Subjective complaints of cognitive impairment, the objective development of cognitive impairment, or the sudden worsening of cognitive impairment or behavior in a person diagnosed with dementia all indicate the need for clinical evaluation. The elements of the evaluation are summarized in Table 3–1. Additional studies used for selected patients are listed in Table 3–2. The extent of the evaluation depends on the physician’s assessment of the problem, the facilities available to undertake a diagnostic evaluation, and the cost-benefit ratios of the various diagnostic procedures available. The most important reason for undertaking a medical evaluation is the possibility of finding a reversible cause of cognitive impairment. Fully reversible dementia syndromes are rare, but an important goal of clinical assessment is to identify causes of cognitive dysfunction that are at least partially reversible. Reversible cognitive impairment is most often due to depression, medications, or hypothyroidism. A medical evaluation can also search for factors leading to excessive disability (Barry and Moskowitz 1988), in which functional impairment is exacerbated by infection, cardiac decompensation, or even environmental changes. Depression may worsen the cognitive impairment resulting from such conditions as head injury, acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS), and stroke. Barry and Moskowitz (1988) suggest that, in general, evaluation of cognitive impairment should be undertaken with the goal of improving patient well-being rather than simply identifying disease. On the other hand, the number of untreatable diseases is shrinking, and precise diagnosis is important in deciding which medications to prescribe or avoid. For example, anticholinergic drugs may aggravate cognitive impairment in patients with Alzheimer’s disease by increasing the preexisting cholinergic deficit. We find three main groups who present for cognitive status evaluation. A few (usually self-referred) individuals seek evaluation because of a family 77
78
TABLE 3–1.
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Medical evaluation of cognitive dysfunction
History and physical examination Neurological examination Mental status examination Attention and concentration, recent and remote memory, language, executive functioning, visuospatial skills Mini-Mental State Exam or comparable objective screening measure Assessment of mood Blood tests Complete blood count and differential Erythrocyte sedimentation rate Blood chemistries (electrolytes, urea nitrogen, creatinine, glucose, calcium, magnesium, liver function tests, hemoglobin A1c, lipid profile) Thyroid stimulating hormone Vitamin B12 level Serologic tests for syphilis (RPR and treponemal test) Computed tomographic or magnetic resonance imaging of the brain
history of Alzheimer’s disease or concern about the cognitive changes that normally begin in middle or advanced age. The second and largest group is persons brought in by concerned friends or family members. Virtually all of these individuals have dementing illnesses. The third group is composed of individuals with cognitive impairment whose cognition is worsening. The families of these individuals have often been told that the cognitive change they noted in their loved ones was due to advanced age or that they may have Alzheimer’s disease. These families seek second opinions for various reasons, including the wish to explore every possibility of uncovering a treatable cause for cognitive impairment or to find treatment for a dementing illness that had previously been described as untreatable. Welldiagnosed nursing home residents whose cognitive state changes suddenly are also within this group. Different medical evaluations may be indicated for a subjective complaint of cognitive impairment, for the initial evaluation of a person whose friends and family have noted signs of cognitive impairment, and for the follow-up evaluation of sudden cognitive deterioration in an adequately diagnosed dementia patient. A subjective complaint of cognitive impairment that is unsubstantiated by an outside informant nevertheless requires a medical and psychiatric history, a formal mental status examination, and a general physical examination, including neurological examination and routine screening laboratory tests. If the history and mental status examinations do not confirm cognitive impairment and there is no sign of brain
Medical Evaluation
TABLE 3–2.
Medical evaluation of cognitive dysfunction—additional measures
Neuropsychological testing Blood tests Antinuclear antibodies Ceruloplasmin DNA studies (prensenilin 1, CAG repeats, ataxia profile) Human immunodeficiency virus Homocysteine Methylmalonic acid Paraneoplastic antibodies (anti-Hu, anti-MaTa, anti-Ri, anti-Yo) Porphyrins Vasculitis workup (protein C and S activity, activated protein C resistance, anti-thrombin III, factor V Leiden, lupus anticoagulant, prothrombin 20201 A) Lumbar puncture Routine studies (cell count and differential, total protein, glucose, VDRL, Gram stain, and bacterial culture) Additional studies: A-> and tau protein concentrations Acid-fast bacillus stain and culture Cryptococcal antigen Cytology Fungal culture Immunoglobulin G index/synthesis rate Lyme titer or polymerase chain reaction (PCR) Oligoclonal bands Viral tests: titers and PCR (cytomegalovirus, herpes simplex, varicella zoster) 14-3-3 protein Whipple antibody by PCR Urine tests Urinalysis 24-hour urine copper 24-hour urine heavy metals 24-hour urine porphyrins Toxicology screen Electroencephalogram Cisternogram Arteriography (invasive or noninvasive) Single photon emission computed tomography Positron emission tomography
79
80
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
damage, psychiatric evaluation and neuropsychological testing may be indicated. The complaint of cognitive impairment, like any other medical complaint, may be a means to deal with emotional issues and concerns (Weiner 1969). This is illustrated in the following case: A 60-year-old woman complained of impaired memory and concentration. Her medical history indicated that she had been worked up for several physical complaints with no definite findings. Her husband reported that he had not observed difficulty with her cognitive functioning. A complete battery of blood tests and a urinalysis were performed and were negative or within normal limits. A complete neurological evaluation, including computed tomography of the head and an electroencephalogram, was negative. Neuropsychological testing showed no clinically meaningful impairment, but personality testing using the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory showed her to be highly anxious and highly preoccupied with physical symptoms. To the psychiatric examiner, she confided that her greatest problem was her husband’s physical abuse, which she related to their chronic marital conflict. This patient was assured that she did not have a brain disorder and was helped to draw her husband into brief marital counseling.
In addition, the syndrome of mild cognitive impairment is characterized by subjective memory decline in the absence of functional decline. Persons with mild cognitive impairment often have no detectable cognitive deficits on mental status testing by a physician, but testing of memory based on immediate and delayed recall of a paragraph-long narrative can reveal their deficit (Petersen et al. 1999).
Setting The medical evaluation of cognitively impaired persons is generally performed in an outpatient setting. Hospitalization is required only when behavioral symptoms make outpatient evaluation impossible or when it is suspected that an emergency medical or surgical procedure might be needed—such as the emergency treatment of lupus cerebritis with bolus intravenous steroids or evacuation of a rapidly expanding subdural hematoma. Otherwise, hospitalization may actually have transient deleterious effects, because some individuals become more confused in a hospital environment (Etienne et al. 1981). A comprehensive multidisciplinary evaluation requires 1–3 days, depending on the efficiency of scheduling, the patient’s tolerance, and the extensiveness of medical and neuropsychological testing. In this chapter, discussion is limited to the medical aspects of the evaluation. Neuroimaging is discussed in Chapter 4. Neuropsychological and language testing are discussed in Chapter 8.
Medical Evaluation
81
History The diagnostic process begins with history taking. An adequate evaluation also involves obtaining information from collateral sources. The evaluation process begins with gathering medical and psychological information. It is useful to obtain prior medical and psychological records in advance to avoid duplication of tests and loss of information due to informants’ lack of awareness or understanding of the diagnostic procedures that have already been employed or the medications that have been prescribed. In all instances, we try to include a person with close knowledge of the patient in the diagnostic process. We also request that patients or family members bring in all prescribed or unprescribed medications to which the patient has access. The history of the present illness should be gathered with regard to the patient’s and family’s chief current concern, the first cognitive and/or behavioral problem(s) noted, the course of the problem(s), and other problems that may have developed. In addition to the patient’s age and handedness, the level of education and current or prior occupation should be determined. A few patients report inability to learn or to recall the names of people and objects. Many do not report cognitive impairment but are brought for evaluation because others have noted lapses in memory or judgment and are concerned. Does the history specifically indicate difficulties characteristic of cognitive impairment? Have there been problems in cognitive domains of memory, language, visuospatial skills, and/or executive dysfunction? Have there been any changes in personality or behavior such as apathy, depression, agitation/irritability, or psychosis? A number of key questions are useful in delineating the history of the cognitive and/or behavioral problems: 1. Were the symptoms acute, subacute, or gradual in onset? An acute onset (within minutes or hours) suggests delirium rather than dementia and a large differential diagnosis that includes infectious, toxic/metabolic, vascular, traumatic, psychiatric, or multifactorial causes. When vascular, the symptoms may be due to ischemic stroke (secondary to embolization or atherosclerotic thromboembolic disease), hemorrhage (traumatic, hypertensive hemorrhage, aneurysmal, amyloid angiopathy, and within a tumor), or vasculitis (including systemic lupus, temporal arteritis, and central nervous system vasculitis). Intermittence of symptoms occurs with all disorders of brain function. Waxing and waning of symptoms over hours is typical of delirium. Dementing illnesses are also characterized by fluctuating symptoms, but the fluctuations are less dramatic and are often more prolonged. There is often
82
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
diurnal confusion that may or may not be associated with fatigue. Symptoms of all illnesses producing cognitive impairment tend to worsen in unfamiliar environments or when emotional and cognitive demands increase. In young adults developing schizophrenia, a catatonic episode may come on within hours. Malingered cognitive impairment and fugue states also develop suddenly (Lamb and Prigatano 2000), both usually occurring in the context of extreme environmental or interpersonal stress. Subacute onset (days to weeks) suggests infectious, toxic/metabolic, or neoplastic origin, whereas a gradual progressive decline over months to years is more typical of degenerative disorders. It is often difficult to date the onset of cognitive difficulties, however. A family may be unaware of any significant changes until their loved one is hospitalized for one reason or another. The combination of an unfamiliar environment and a medical illness or a surgical procedure often unmasks preexisting dementia and may also precipitate delirium in a person with cognitive impairment. Therefore, it is important to reevaluate the person for cognitive impairment once any delirium has resolved. Several questions can help determine the onset and progression of any symptoms of cognitive impairment. How is the person’s job performance? If he or she is retired, was retirement due in part to impaired performance? Has there been any change in the performance of usual activities (e.g., conversation and social interaction, housekeeping, meal preparation, operating appliances, making routine purchases, making change, writing checks, driving, dressing, grooming, and performance in hobbies and social activities). When was the first change or loss of interest in these activities? Did symptoms precede or follow any significant medical illness or surgery? 2. Are the symptoms progressive, diminishing, or stabilized? Lack of progression suggests a single insult, such as a vascular accident, trauma, or depression. Progression may be associated with infectious diseases (AIDS, Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease [CJD], neurosyphilis), trauma (subdural hematoma), metabolic (diabetes), and degenerative disorders. Degenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, Pick’s disease, and multisystem atrophy have a relatively smooth downward course. Vascular dementia generally has a stepwise progression. In the case of stepwise progression, sensory and motor changes accompany the cognitive change in vascular dementia and multiple sclerosis. Symptomatic improvement is associated with trauma, acute vascular disorders, and acute toxic and metabolic disorders. Fluctuations in cognitive dysfunction may occur in many types of dementing illness, including the degenerative disorders, but they do not include a return to baseline. This is frequently the case in dementia with Lewy bodies (McKeith et al.
Medical Evaluation
83
1996). In most dementing illnesses, cognitive impairment fluctuates, depending on the complexity of the environment, emotional strain, fatigue, general physical health, and time of day. Symptoms are frequently worse in the evenings due to fatigue and loss of orienting sensory cues. Such nighttime worsening is referred to as sundowning. Paroxysmal deterioration with relatively full interepisode recovery occurs in alcoholic persons with encephalopathy due to liver disease. Transient global amnesia is a syndrome of intermittent confusion of diverse etiology, probably due in many cases to ischemia of the medial temporal lobes. Partial complex seizures can cause intermittent behavioral disruption with cumulative chronic deterioration, but the history also reveals motor stereotypy and postictal sleepiness. 3. Has the person experienced the same or similar symptoms in the past? Past experience of short confusional episodes may suggest epilepsy or transient ischemic attacks. Periods of cognitive dysfunction lasting days or weeks may be related to emotional disorders, metabolic disorders such as porphyria, or diseases associated with Lewy bodies. Individuals who are malingering or in fugue states will often report similar previous episodes. Depressed persons may report similar episodes of cognitive impairment with past episodes of depression. 4. Is there a history of psychiatric disorder or severe environmental stress? An episode of depression, whether mild or severe, can markedly impair cognitive functioning (Boone et al. 1995). Persons with long-standing schizophrenia frequently develop severe cognitive impairment (Purohit et al. 1998). Cognitive impairment due to malingering occurs in individuals facing imprisonment (Allen et al. 2000), and fugue states may develop in individuals seeking to escape the consequences of acts such as bigamy. In Ganser syndrome, patients’ answers are one-off or near misses (nearly correct). 5. Are there behavioral or psychiatric symptoms? Frequently, the first symptoms noted in a dementing illness are loss of initiative and loss of interest in activities that were formerly pleasurable. Individuals with impaired frontal lobe function may show apathy and/or disinhibition. Suspiciousness and irritability may accompany early dementing illness, as may depression or elation and grandiosity. Depressive, psychotic, and obsessive-compulsive symptoms may herald the onset of Huntington’s disease (De Marchi and Mennella 2000). Visual hallucinations unaccompanied by explanatory delusions are frequent in Alzheimer’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Tactile hallucinations and illusions are common in delirium. Complex delusional systems are unusual in dementing illness.
84
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Auditory hallucinations in dementia tend to be of familiar others, whereas accusatory or threatening voices that speak through the radio or television are more characteristic of schizophrenia. 6. Are there associated neurological symptoms? Neurological symptoms often suggest specific dementia diagnoses. Such symptoms may include loss of consciousness, seizures, dyscoordination, gait and balance problems, movement disorders, weakness (generalized or localized), impairment of vision or hearing, and other cranial nerve dysfunction. Loss of consciousness may accompany severe head trauma, lupus cerebritis, and toxic-metabolic disorders. Seizures may point to a primary seizure disorder or to other conditions such as neoplasm, in which seizures are secondary. Gait apraxia and early urinary incontinence are associated with normalpressure hydrocephalus. Dysarthria and paralysis of gaze suggests progressive supranuclear palsy. Unilateral limb apraxia suggests corticobasal ganglionic degeneration (Schneider et al. 1996) or frontal dementia (Kertesz et al. 2001). Bradykinesia may indicate depression, early Parkinson’s disease, or another subcortical process. Dyscoordination and sensory and cranial nerve symptoms may indicate multiple sclerosis or progressive supranuclear palsy. Choreiform movements accompany Wilson’s and Huntington’s disease; myoclonic jerks accompany CJD and mid- to late-stage Alzheimer’s disease (Chen et al. 1991). Lateralized abnormalities of strength, tone, reflexes, or sensation suggest a possible vascular origin. Visual field deficits point to possible vascular or neoplastic disease, and unilateral hearing loss points to neoplasm. 7. Is there a personal or family history of a disease or disorder associated with cognitive decline? The presence of diabetes, hypertension, strokes, hypercholesterolemia, and signs of generalized atherosclerosis point to possible vascular dementia. Severe renal or hepatic disease may produce metabolic encephalopathy. Human immunodeficiency virus (HIV-1) seropositivity raises the possibility of direct effects of the virus on the brain (HIV-1–associated minor cognitive motor disorder or HIV-1– associated dementia [American Academy of Neurology 1991]) or an opportunistic brain infection. Huntington’s and Wilson’s diseases exemplify familial diseases associated with dementia. Alzheimer’s disease very rarely occurs as an autosomal dominant familial disease. Nearly half of frontal dementias may be hereditary (Chow et al. 1999; Knopman et al. 1990; Neary et al. 1988; Stevens et al. 1998). A variety of mutations (over 20) in the gene encoding tau, a microtubule-associated protein, have been associated with inherited frontotemporal dementia (see Genetic Markers section in this chapter).
Medical Evaluation
85
8. Does the person take any prescribed or unprescribed medications? Many medications have the ability to impair cognitive function, including benzodiazepine hypnotics and tranquilizers, barbiturates, anticonvulsants, propranolol, and cardiac glycosides. Episodes of confusion in persons with porphyria may be induced by various medications, including barbiturates and chlordiazepoxide (Sack 1990). In addition, patients and families should be specifically asked what sleep medications, over-the-counter medications (e.g., aspirin), vitamins, and other supplements (calcium, herbal—Gingko biloba, St. John’s Wort, etc.) the patient is taking, as this information is often not proffered. Patients and their families should be asked in advance of the patient’s appointment to bring in either the patient’s medication bottles or a detailed list including the name of each medication, its dose, and the number of times per day it is taken. 9. Is there a history of abuse or heavy intake of alcohol or other substances? History of alcohol abuse may point to the origin of an amnestic disorder or dementia. Prolonged heavy alcohol use without evidence of intoxication may rarely cause significant cognitive impairment, but we have not seen this in our clinic. On the other hand, repeated episodes of delirium tremens are significant. In teenagers and young adults, evidence of glue or paint sniffing is important. 10. Has there been exposure to environmental toxins? Arsenic, mercury, lead, organic solvents, and organophosphate insecticides can produce encephalopathies, usually accompanied by severe systemic symptoms. 11. Has there been exposure to HIV infection? Intravenous drug abuse and unprotected sexual contact are risk factors for infection with HIV in adults. Increased screening of blood and blood transfusions has virtually eliminated this form of HIV transmission. Highly active antiretroviral therapy (HAART) has postponed the occurrence of AIDS caused by HIV as well as associated symptoms, such as AIDS dementia. Even when the history suggests that a patient has a depressive disorder, is functioning normally, or is overreacting to the normal cognitive changes accompanying aging, a formal mental status examination should always be performed. When possible, information should be obtained separately from a knowledgeable informant. It is wise to obtain a baseline measure of cognitive performance such as the Mini-Mental State Exam (Folstein et al. 1975) or the more sensitive Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (Mattis 1988). Follow-up should also be arranged, with a revisit scheduled after a period of 1 year or sooner, depending on the findings of the initial evaluation and progression of any problems.
86
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Physical Examination A general physical examination is an important part of the medical evaluation. Diseases of many organ systems can lead to transient or progressive impairment of brain function or can contribute to excessive morbidity in dementing illness. Funduscopic examination may reveal optic atrophy in the case of multiple sclerosis or papilledema in the case of increased intracranial pressure such as tumor. Cardiovascular examination may demonstrate an irregular rhythm consistent with atrial fibrillation and increased risk for stroke. Carotid bruits may be an indicator of stenosis. Abdominal examination may reveal the presence of hepatomegaly. Examination of the skin and extremities may reveal signs of vasculitis such as petechiae.
Neurological Examination A detailed neurological examination is performed on every patient (see Appendix C).
Cranial Nerves Cranial nerve examination may include olfaction, but the relationship of olfactory deficits to dementing illness remains controversial. There is wide consensus that olfactory deficits occur in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease (Mesholam et al. 1998), but such deficits also occur in normal elders. Bacon et al. (1998) found changes in olfactory threshold in the year preceding change in diagnosis from nondemented to Alzheimer’s disease. On the other hand, a neuropathologically confirmed study showed no difference in olfactory discrimination between patients with Alzheimer’s disease and control subjects, but a significant difference between control subjects and subjects with cortical Lewy bodies (McShane et al. 2001). Anosmia of sudden onset may point to a significant head injury. It is important to test visual and auditory acuity, because sensory impairment may influence mental status testing. Pupillary abnormalities occur with neurosyphilis but may also result from cataract surgery. The typical Argyll Robertson pupil seen with neurosyphilis is small, irregular, and reactive to accommodation but not to light. Retinal examination may reveal damage from long-standing hypertension or diabetes. Impairment of gaze in progressive supranuclear palsy usually affects downward gaze first, then upward gaze, and, finally, horizontal gaze. Asymmetry of the facial muscles in the lower part of the face occurs with an upper motor neuron lesion such as a stroke or tumor. Weakness of
Medical Evaluation
87
muscles supplied by other cranial nerves (such as the tongue, sternocleidomastoids, and trapezia) and/or altered facial sensation may also suggest a stroke.
Motor System Patients are assessed for muscle bulk, tone, and strength as well as for any apraxia or abnormal involuntary movements, such as tremor, dyskinesia, or chorea. Increased resistance to passive movement (rigidity) is common as Alzheimer’s disease progresses. The occurrence of rigidity early in the course of a dementing illness may indicate a parkinsonian syndrome, especially when accompanied by tremor or other parkinsonian symptoms. Clonus may be demonstrable in patients with upper motor neuron damage due to a stroke or spinal damage. Myoclonus is a lightning-like jerk of a limb, limbs, or the entire body, that may be induced by testing reflexes (reflex myoclonus) or if the patient is suddenly startled (startle reflex), such as by a loud noise. Myoclonus in the setting of rapidly progressive dementia should raise the possibility of CJD, but myoclonus may occur later in the course of other dementias, such as Alzheimer’s disease and Lewy body disease.
Parkinsonism We prefer this term to the more nonspecific term extrapyramidal symptoms. Bradykinesia, resting tremor, rigidity, and postural instability are the cardinal signs of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. The presence of two or more, but not all, of these signs is suggestive of secondary parkinsonism, such as medication-induced Parkinson’s-plus syndromes or Lewy body disease. Other parkinsonian features that may be seen in Parkinson’s disease and these related syndromes include restriction of extraocular movements, masklike facies, hypophonia, dysarthria, dysphagia, collapsing movements, micrographia, difficulty arising from a sitting position, stooped posture, slow gait, turning en bloc, festinating gait, and decreased arm swing with walking.
Sensation Vibration sense in the lower extremities is frequently reduced in the elderly, but position sense is not. Sensory neuropathies, characterized by loss of vibratory and pinprick sensation in the periphery, and greatest distally, occur in individuals with hypothyroidism, significant alcohol use, diabetes, syphilis, and vitamin B12 deficiency. In the case of the tabes dorsalis syn-
88
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
drome of neurosyphilis, the dorsal columns are involved, and both vibratory and position sense are frequently impaired due to involvement of the dorsal columns.
Reflexes Deep tendon reflexes are generally reduced in sensory neuropathy. Increased deep tendon reflexes may accompany the sensory neuropathy of vitamin B12 deficiency. Asymmetric reflexes and the presence of a plantar extensor response (Babinski reflex) suggest upper motor neuron pathology. Frontal release signs, also called frontal reflexes or primitive reflexes (grasp, palmomental, rooting, snout, suck), may be seen even in healthy elderly persons. These signs are therefore relatively nonspecific, except when seen in younger adult patients and in the context of other frontal abnormalities on neurological or mental status examination.
Gait and Posture Gait tends to slow with aging, and tandem walking may be difficult for elders. Gait apraxia or “magnetic” gait, in which a patient has difficulty initiating steps, raises the possibility of normal-pressure hydrocephalus. Data from the neurological examination, mental status examination, and history enable the clinician to classify dementing illnesses into one of the types described in Chapter 1: frontotemporal, temporoparietal, or subcortical. The neurological examination facilitates the differentiation of dementing illness into these categories and the categories of cortical, subcortical, and mixed dementing illness. Frontotemporal disorders are often accompanied by cortical release signs such as the palmomental, suck, and snout reflexes; by difficulty with simple programmed motor activities such as the Luria hand sequence; language impairment; and disinhibition. Temporoparietal disorders, in addition to severe difficulty with recent memory, are often accompanied by difficulty with word finding and spatial orientation. Subcortical dementias may be accompanied by apathy, disinhibition, emotional lability, and depression. These illnesses also tend to have motor signs that may be pyramidal or extrapyramidal. The anatomic loci for cortical dementias are the neocortical association areas and hippocampus; the loci for subcortical dementias are the thalamus, basal ganglia, and rostral brainstem (Albert et al. 1974). Based on the foregoing criteria, the prototypical cortical dementias are Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia/Pick’s disease, but mild extrapyramidal signs, such as rigidity and bradykinesia, can occur in conjunction with Alzheimer’s disease. Typical subcortical dementias are Huntington’s disease,
Medical Evaluation
89
Wilson’s disease, AIDS dementia, and progressive supranuclear palsy. Vascular dementia, CJD, and trauma typically produce mixed cortical and subcortical signs.
Laboratory Studies Blood The Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology (1994) recommended routine blood tests, including serum electrolytes; glucose, blood urea nitrogen/creatinine, folate, and vitamin B12 concentrations; thyroid function; and syphilis serology. The only change in these recommendations in the most recent guidelines (Knopman et al. 2001) is that testing for syphilis be performed only if there is clinical suspicion of neurosyphilis. No changes were made in the other recommended tests because no further studies of their utility have been performed. In our clinic, we perform the following laboratory studies: complete blood count and differential, erythrocyte sedimentation rate, electrolytes, urea nitrogen, creatinine, blood glucose, hemoglobin A1c, calcium, magnesium, liver function tests, thyroid-stimulating hormone (TSH), vitamin B12 level, lipid profile, and serologic tests for syphilis (the rapid plasmin reagin [RPR] test and the microhemagglutinin assay for antibodies to Treponema pallidum [MHA-TP]). We have discontinued determining folic acid levels in blood because of the lack of evidence of its utility. We have not seen folate deficiency in more than 2,500 evaluations for dementia. We have not encountered a case of pernicious anemia, but we have treated all persons with serum vitamin B12 level below 200 pg/mL and have had no positive cognitive response. Others, however, have reported improvement in neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with low serum cobalamin levels in the absence of anemia (Lindenbaum et al. 1988). Eastley et al. (2000) reported on 1,432 persons seen in a dementia clinic, of whom 125 had low vitamin B12 concentrations. Of these, 65 were demented, and 22 had lesser degrees of cognitive impairment. After treatment of vitamin B12 deficiency, there was improved verbal fluency in the nondemented cognitively impaired group but no change in the demented group. Therefore, we feel justified in treating patients with serum vitamin B12 levels below 200 pg/mL. When levels of vitamin B12 are low normal (below 400 pg/mL), we also determine methylmalonic acid and homocysteine levels, because these tests may be evidence of functional vitamin B12 deficiency when vitamin B12 levels are still within the normal range. Patients with homocysteine levels above 15 mmol/L or with methyl-
90
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
malonic acid levels above 950 nmol/L are treated with cobalamin supplements. Vitamin B12 deficiency should be treated first weekly, then monthly, with parenteral B12 supplementation. Abnormalities in homocysteine metabolism may partly explain the association between folate and vitamin B12 levels and neurodegenerative diseases (Diaz-Arrastia 2000). Hyperhomocysteinemia has been associated with cerebrovascular disease, and high homocysteine levels have been retrospectively correlated with increased risk for developing vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease (Clarke et al. 1998; McCaddon et al. 1998). A recent analysis of prospectively collected data in Framingham, Massachusetts, indicates that persons with increased homocysteine levels have approximately a 2.5-fold increased risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease within 3–11 years (Seshadri et al. 2002). Further research on the role of homocysteine in dementia is ongoing, particularly on whether homocysteinelowering therapy can decrease the incidence of dementia or slow its rate of progression. We obtain a lipid profile because hypercholesterolemia is an independent and treatable risk factor for cerebrovascular disease. Moreover, so-called statins, hypolipidemic agents that act by inhibiting hepatic hydroxymethylglutaryl coenzyme A reductase, have been retrospectively associated with increased risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease (Jick et al. 2000; Wolozin et al. 2000). We continue to perform serologic test for syphilis because we are a tertiary care center and neurosyphilis is a treatable cause of dementia. Because tests for antibodies specific for treponemal antigens, such as the MHA-TP or the fluorescent treponemal antibody absorption (FTA-ABS) test, are highly specific for the diagnosis of syphilis, we do these in conjunction with the RPR test. We use TSH levels as our measure of thyroid function. We also perform routine liver function tests and use erythrocyte sedimentation rate as a screening test for systemic inflammatory disorders. Serum ceruloplasmin and 24-hour urinary copper are determined in possible cases of Wilson’s disease, as in a young person (age less than 40 years) presenting with cognitive impairment. It is also necessary to measure 24-hour urine copper output because serum ceruloplasmin may be low or normal in a person with Wilson’s disease. Unless specific evidence is present on clinical examination, we do not routinely perform tests to determine if toxic substances have been ingested. If, on the other hand, there is a suspicion of covert or unreported drug use or of exposure to toxins such as lead or mercury, appropriate toxicologic testing is performed. When patients are taking digitalis, anticonvulsants, antidepressants, lithium, or other drugs that produce confusion when the
Medical Evaluation
91
therapeutic blood level is exceeded, we determine the serum concentrations of these drugs.
Urine In the case of frail elders, a urinalysis is indicated routinely. In many instances, a urinary tract infection will cause confusion (usually delirium), but in others it may present only as a worsening of cognitive state. Urine studies (Table 3–2) may be indicated in individuals with suspected exposure to heavy metals, substance abuse, Wilson’s disease (see previously in this chapter), or porphyria.
Spinal Fluid Lumbar puncture is a relatively benign procedure that has produced virtually no complications in our hands, but in only one instance in more than 400 cases did our findings help in the differential diagnosis of patients evaluated for long-standing progressive cognitive impairment. Our experience is similar to that of Hammerstrom and Zimmer (1985), who reviewed the value of lumbar punctures in evaluating 80 dementia patients age 50 years or older. No diagnosis was made on the basis of the information derived from lumbar puncture in the 42 patients who underwent lumbar puncture. The only abnormalities found were 11 cases of nonspecific protein elevation and 1 case of increased cell count not due to bacterial infection. These researchers reviewed an additional series of 422 cases from other series and found 4 patients whose diagnoses could have been made by lumbar puncture. One patient had neurosyphilis, and the other 3 had postencephalitic parkinsonism. Nevertheless, lumbar puncture is often justified, especially in the evaluation of young and middle-aged patients. It is indicated specifically in rapidly progressive dementia, positive syphilis serology, and suspected central nervous system infection. It is also indicated in all cases of dementia occurring in persons with diagnosed or suspected AIDS and in persons who have had a blood transfusion. The detection of the 14–3–3 protein in cerebrospinal fluid is useful in confirming the diagnosis of CJD (Zerr et al. 1998), but a negative result does not exclude CJD. In the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease, measurements of b-amyloid1-42, tau, or AD7C-NTP levels in cerebrospinal fluid have not been shown to increase diagnostic specificity and specificity over that of the clinical diagnosis and are not recommended for routine diagnosis (Knopman et al. 2001).
92
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Special Diagnostic Procedures Genetic Markers There are no genetic markers recommended for routine diagnostic purposes (Knopman et al. 2001) or for asymptomatic persons concerned about the future development of dementing illness (Small et al. 1997). Genetic testing may be appropriate for some asymptomatic persons in selected cases of hereditary dementing illnesses such as Huntington’s disease, in which genetic testing can confirm a diagnosis of or detect presymptomatic disease (International Huntington Association 1994). A variety of mutations in the tau gene on chromosome 17 have been associated with familial frontotemporal dementias (Hutton et al. 1998; Pickering-Brown et al. 2002; Poorkaj et al. 1998; Spillantini et al. 1998), but a number of these illnesses have genetic mutations that have yet to be determined. Mutations in the amyloid precursor protein gene and the presenilin 1 and 2 genes can be detected in some cases of familial early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Rosenberg and Iannaccone 1995). Inheritance of the e4 allele of the cholesteroltransporting protein apolipoprotein E is associated with late-onset familial and sporadic Alzheimer’s disease, but it cannot be used as a diagnostic test or to predict the onset of Alzheimer’s disease. It is therefore not recommended for asymptomatic persons. However, in persons who meet clinical criteria for Alzheimer’s disease, the presence of an e4 allele increases the specificity of the diagnosis from 55% to 84% (Mayeux et al. 1998).
Neuroimaging Neuroimaging is discussed extensively in Chapter 4. Structural neuroimaging, brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), or computed tomography (CT) of the head, is done as part of our routine initial evaluation and is a guideline recommendation by the American Academy of Neurology (Knopman et al. 2001). Brain MRI is our structural neuroimaging test of choice. We generally perform brain MRI without contrast medium, but gadolinium is used in some patients, particularly if the history or examination raises a concern for neoplasm or infection. Functional neuroimaging, such as single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), is employed in diagnostic dilemmas and to confirm frontotemporal dementias. Cisternography is employed for suspected normal-pressure hydrocephalus.
Cisternogram Radionuclide cisternography is used to differentiate between communicating and noncommunicating hydrocephalus and helps establish the diagno-
Medical Evaluation
93
sis of normal-pressure hydrocephalus by demonstrating reflux into the ventricles and delayed pericerebral diffusion.
Other Diagnostic Procedures Angiography Intracerebral angiography should be performed for specific indications, such as diagnosis of aneurysms; vascular malformations; occluded arteries and veins; and mass lesions, such as hemorrhages, abscesses, and neoplasms. A radiopaque contrast medium injected percutaneously into a carotid, brachial, or femoral artery allows visualization of the entire circulation of the neck and brain. With this technique, there is a risk of embolization, and high concentrations of contrast medium may cause spasm and occlusion. MR and CT angiography, which produce computer-generated images of the major cervical and intracranial arteries, are much more benign and less expensive techniques, but they are somewhat less precise. These techniques involve the intravenous injection of a small amount of contrast material.
Brain Biopsy Brain biopsy is reserved for situations in which a treatable illness is suspected. We have reserved this procedure for suspected autoimmune cerebral vascular disease and infectious brain diseases not diagnosable by spinal fluid studies. We have requested brain biopsy four times in 14 years. All instances were cases of rapidly progressive dementia. In one case, the biopsy report was normal brain tissue; in the other three, it was Alzheimer’s disease. Brain biopsy is of limited use in that only small amounts of tissue can be sampled, and the brain areas most affected by diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease are not readily accessible. Brain biopsy is generally not advisable in cases of suspected CJD because of the high risk to the surgical team, the possibility of iatrogenic transmission to others, and the untreatable nature of this illness, unless the biopsy has some likelihood of revealing a treatable cause. (Even postmortem examination of potential CJD necessitates advance notice to the pathologists, as additional protective gear and special tissue preparation techniques are required to avoid inadvertent transmission.)
Carotid and Transcranial Sonography Evidence of generalized arteriosclerosis, carotid bruits, transient ischemic attacks, or stroke warrants sonographic investigation of the carotid and in-
94
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
tracranial vasculature. Patients with evidence of stroke on history, examination, or neuroimaging are referred for echocardiography to be evaluated for any cardiac source of emboli. Patients with evidence of large-vessel cerebrovascular disease are referred for cerebral angiography. Transcranial Doppler imaging may be performed to image the intracranial vasculature, including the circle of Willis (Sadik et al. 2001). In our experience, vascular stenosis does not often contribute to the development of dementia, but we tend to see few cases of vascular dementia. We have had one anecdotal report of progressive dementia with pronounced bilateral carotid narrowing that improved dramatically after bilateral endarterectomy (R. Rosenberg, personal communication, October 3, 1989).
Electroencephalography The electroencephalogram (EEG) is of limited utility in the evaluation of cognitive impairment. It has been argued that electroencephalographic evidence of an objective abnormality of cerebral function is useful in distinguishing between a degenerative disease and a psychiatric disorder, such as depression with cognitive impairment (depressive pseudodementia) (Brenner 1999). In practice, however, the sensitivity and specificity of electroencephalography is low. Electroencephalographic findings in Alzheimer’s disease include slowing of the posterior dominant rhythm, an increase in diffuse slow (theta or delta) activity, and generalized bursts of slow activity, but none of these findings are specific or sensitive for Alzheimer’s disease. Most persons with severe to moderate Alzheimer’s disease have such electroencephalographic abnormalities, reflecting the degree of impairment of cortical function. However, with mild impairment early in the illness, the EEG is often normal (Markand 1990). The majority of persons with Pick’s disease have normal EEGs (Stigsby 1988). However, these studies were based on very small numbers of patients, and one of us (A.M.L.) has seen a 39-year-old man with frontotemporal dementia with a low-voltage EEG. Huntington’s disease patients typically show low voltage (Robinson et al. 1994). This pattern is neither sensitive nor specific, and the utility of electroencephalography in making the diagnosis is limited, particularly in the early stages (Pedley and Miller 1983). The presence of triphasic waves is frequently an indication of delirium-producing toxic and metabolic disorders (Engel and Romano 1959). Focal slow waves may suggest destructive lesions such as strokes and tumors. Electroencephalography is far less sensitive and specific in diagnosing these conditions than is neuroimaging or other studies. Electroencephalography is useful in the diagnosis of patients with rapidly progressive cognitive deterioration in whom CJD is considered in
Medical Evaluation
95
the differential. In persons with CJD, the electroencephalographic pattern is distinctive. In the initial phase of the illness, electroencephalographic changes consist of a progressive disorganization of background rhythms and increased amounts of generalized slow (theta-delta) activity. As the disease progresses, the EEG is characterized by periodic, bilaterally synchronous, sharply contoured biphasic and triphasic waves, which appear at irregular intervals of one or two per second (Brenner 1999). These findings have a sensitivity of 67% and a specificity of 86% compared with neuropathologic findings when prospectively studied in a group of patients in whom CJD was clinically suspected (Steinhoff et al. 1996). Similar electroencephalographic findings have been reported in rare cases of lithium intoxication, baclofen encephalopathy, myoencephalopathy ragged red-fiber disease, and HIV encephalopathy (Brenner 1999). Electroencephalographic studies are most widely performed in the evaluation of cognitively impaired persons who may have epileptic seizures. The incidence of epilepsy increases significantly after age 65 (Hauser 1992), and several studies indicate that epilepsy complicates 5%–25% of cases of Alzheimer’s disease (Risse et al. 1990). The diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease or other dementia increases the risk of unprovoked seizures approximately sixfold (Hesdorffer et al. 1996), and it is estimated that by 10 years after diagnosis, 15% of Alzheimer’s disease patients will have experienced epileptic seizures (Hauser 1997). However, persons with dementing illness are also at high risk for other paroxysmal neurologic events that mimic, but are distinct from, epileptic seizures. These include transient ischemic attacks, syncopal episodes, and acute behavioral disturbances. Electroencephalography is often the best way to establish the diagnosis, because the presence of paroxysmal generalized or focal spikes, sharp waves, or sharp and slow wave complexes (interictal epileptiform discharges, or IEDs) are highly predictive of epileptic seizures. Although the specificity of IEDs for epileptic seizures is high (>95%), the sensitivity in elderly patients is poor (25%–35% for patients age 65 and older, compared with 50%–70% for younger persons (Drury and Beydoun 1998). A significant proportion of elderly persons admitted to epilepsy monitoring units for evaluation of presumptive seizures are found to have nonepileptic phenomena (Drury et al. 1999). Quantitative electroencephalography and long-latency cortical evoked potentials such as the P300 do not seem to add to clinical diagnostic specificity in the dementing illnesses, although they remain a subject for clinical research. Neither technique is mentioned in recently published practice parameters for the diagnosis of dementia (Knopman et al. 2001).
96
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
How Much Workup Is Enough? Opinions differ regarding what constitutes an adequate laboratory workup for dementing illness. The Canadian guidelines (Mohr et al. 1995) suggest that measurement of blood urea nitrogen, vitamin B12, and folic acid concentrations; serologic testing for syphilis; urinalysis; and erythrocyte sedimentation rate should be performed only if they are indicated by history or physical examination. These guidelines also suggest neuroimaging if patients are less than age 60 years, if there is use of anticoagulants or a history of bleeding disorder, recent head trauma, cancers that metastasize to brain, unexplained neurological symptoms, rapid progression of disease, dementia duration of less than 2 years, or urinary incontinence and gait disorder suggestive of normal-pressure hydrocephalus. As noted previously, the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology (Knopman et al. 2001) recommends routine blood tests, including serum electrolytes; glucose, blood urea nitrogen/creatinine, folate, and vitamin B12 concentrations; and thyroid function. Syphilis serology is recommended only if there is clinical suspicion of neurosyphilis. Structural neuroimaging with CT or MRI is now a guideline recommendation. In our clinic, the minimum medical evaluation includes patient history; physical and neurological examinations; mental status examination; brain MRI (preferably) or CT; and laboratory studies, including complete blood count and differential; erythrocyte sedimentation rate; electrolytes, urea nitrogen, creatinine, blood glucose, hemoglobin A1c, calcium, magnesium, TSH, and vitamin B12 concentrations; liver function tests; lipid profile; and serologic tests for syphilis (RPR and MHA-TP). Lumbar puncture is performed if an infectious, inflammatory, or autoimmune disorder is suspected. We use electroencephalography if epilepsy or CJD is suspected. We have typically used SPECT for atypical dementias, to confirm frontal dementias, and to differentiate dysphasic dementia (primary progressive aphasia) from aphasia due to cerebrovascular disease. Jobst et al. (1994) combined temporal lobe CT with SPECT in Alzheimer’s disease and reported a sensitivity of 90% and a specificity of 97%.
How Often Are Evaluations Indicated? Brief reevaluations should be done at least yearly, including an interim clinical history, a neurological examination, and mental status testing. Monitoring progression of the illness through history and examination and by noting the response to any treatment may help to confirm the original diagnosis or raise doubt if the typical clinical course and findings are not
Medical Evaluation
97
observed. An unusually fast progression may raise suspicion for an illness such as CJD. Marked improvement may suggest a reversible disease, even depression, responding to treatment or remission. New focal signs may point to a vascular or neoplastic component. More comprehensive evaluations are indicated when such new findings arise, particularly when a reversible component of cognitive impairment is suspected, as in the following example: A 74-year-old widow with a 4-year history of progressive dementia was diagnosed as having Alzheimer’s disease after a comprehensive evaluation including a head CT scan. Six months later, a positron emission tomographic scan showed findings typical of Alzheimer’s disease. Six months after that, she returned with severe gait problems, marked urinary incontinence, and increasing ventricular size accompanied by diffuse white matter changes on brain MRI. A diagnosis of normal-pressure hydrocephalus was made, and a ventriculoperitoneal shunt was inserted. Over the next 2 years, the woman showed marked improvement in her cognitive status (Friedland 1989).
When Is Reevaluation Longer Needed? Additional comprehensive evaluations are not indicated when an adequately diagnosed disease process is following its predicted course. Continued reevaluation of minor degrees of cognitive dysfunction is probably not indicated after 5 years of stability. On the other hand, it has been our experience that departures from the normal trajectory of a patient’s diagnosed dementing illness are often worthy of investigation, particularly in frail elders. A minimum evaluation in these cases includes medication check, physical examination, chest X ray, electrocardiogram, blood chemistries, and urinalysis. EEG, head CT, or serum drug levels may also be useful in some cases.
Summary Although the specific procedures that are used to determine etiology differ according to the type of cognitive impairment or dementing illness, the medical evaluation of cognitive impairment requires accurate history taking, mental status examination, physical and neurological evaluation, a relatively small number of blood tests, and neuroimaging. Illnesses that progress rapidly or that have an atypical course warrant more comprehensive investigation at a tertiary care center, where investigators have experience with a variety of dementing illnesses. As techniques and treatments evolve, methods such as functional neuroimaging may be used more frequently as a means for early detection and for monitoring the course of treatment.
98
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
References Albert ML, Feldman RG, Willis AL: The “subcortical dementia” of progressive supranuclear palsy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 37:121–130, 1974 Allen DF, Postel J, Berrios GE: The Ganser syndrome, in Memory Disorders in Psychiatric Practice, edited by Berrios GE, Hodges JR. Cambridge, England, Cambridge University Press, 2000, pp 443–455 American Academy of Neurology AIDS Task Force: Nomenclature and case definitions for neurologic manifestations of human immunodeficiency virus–type 1 (HIV-1) infection. Neurology 41:778–785, 1991 Bacon AW, Bondi MW, Salmon DP, et al: Very early changes in olfactory functioning due to Alzheimer’s disease and the role of apolipoprotein E in olfaction. Ann N Y Acad Sci 855:723–731, 1998 Barry PB, Moskowitz MA: The diagnosis of reversible dementia in the elderly. Arch Intern Med 148:1914–1918, 1988 Boone K, Lesser I, Miller B, et al: Cognitive functioning in older depressed outpatients: relationship of presence and severity of depression to neuropsychological test scores. Neuropsychology 9:390–398, 1995 Brenner RP: EEG and dementia, in Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields. Edited by Niedermeyer E, Lopes da Silva F. Philadelphia, PA, Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 1999, pp 349–359 Chen Y, Stern Y, Sano M, et al: Cumulative risks of developing extrapyramidal signs, psychosis, or myoclonus in the course of Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 48:1141–1143, 1991 Chow TW, Miller BL, Hayashi VN, et al: Inheritance of frontotemporal dementia. Arch Neurol 56:817–822, 1999 Clarke R, Smith AD, Jobst KA, et al: Folate, vitamin B12, and serum total homocysteine levels in confirmed Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 55:1449–1455, 1998 De Marchi N, Mennella R: Huntington’s disease and its association with psychopathology. Harv Rev Psychiatry 7:278–289, 2000 Diaz-Arrastia R: Homocysteine and neurologic disease. Arch Neurol 57:1422– 1428, 2000 Drury I, Beydoun A: Interictal epileptiform activity in elderly patients with epilepsy. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 156:369–373, 1998 Drury I, Selwa LM, Schuh LA, et al: Value of inpatient diagnostic CCTV-EEG monitoring in the elderly. Epilepsia 40:1100–1102, 1999 Eastley R, Wilcock GK, Bucks RS: Vitamin B12 deficiency in dementia and cognitive impairment: the effects of treatment on neuropsychological function. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 15:226–233, 2000 Engel GE, Romano J: Delirium, a syndrome of cerebral insufficiency. J Chronic Dis 9:260–277, 1959 Etienne PE, Dastoor D, Goldapple E, et al: Adverse effects of medical and psychiatric workup in six demented geriatric patients. Am J Psychiatry 138:520–521, 1981
Medical Evaluation
99
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189– 198, 1975 Friedland RP: “Normal”-pressure hydrocephalus and the saga of treatable dementias. JAMA 262:2577–2581, 1989 Hammerstrom DC, Zimmer B: The role of lumbar puncture in the evaluation of dementia: the University of Pittsburgh Study. J Am Geriatr Soc 33:397–400, 1985 Hauser WA: Seizure disorders: the changes with age. Epilepsia 33 (suppl 4):S6–S14, 1992 Hauser WA: Epidemiology of seizures and epilepsy in the elderly, in Seizures and Epilepsy in the Elderly. Edited by Rowan AJ, Ramsay RE. Boston, MA, Butterworth-Heinemann, 1997, pp 7–18 Hesdorffer DC, Hauser WA, Annegers JF, et al: Dementia and adult-onset unprovoked seizures. Neurology 46:727–730, 1996 Hutton M, Lendon CL, Rizzu P, et al: Association of missense and 5´-splice site mutations in tau with the inherited dementia FTDP-17. Nature 393:702–705, 1998 International Huntington Association (IHA) and the World Federation of Neurology (WFN) Research Group on Huntington’s Chorea: Guidelines for the molecular genetics predictive test in HD. Neurology 44:1533–1536, 1994 Jick H, Zonberg GL, Jick SS, et al: Statins and the risk of dementia. Lancet 356:1627–1631, 2000 Jobst KA, Hindley NJ, King E, et al: The diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: a question of image? J Clin Psychiatry 55 (suppl 11):22–31, 1994 Kertesz A, Martinez-Lage P, Davidson W, et al: The corticobasal degeneration syndrome overlaps progressive aphasia and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 55:1368–1375, 2001 Knopman DS, Mastri AR, Frey WH et al: Dementia lacking distinctive histologic features: a common non-Alzheimer degenerative dementia. Neurology 40: 251–256, 1990 Knopman DS, DeKosky ST, Cummings JL, et al: Practice parameter: diagnosis of dementia (an evidence-based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 56:1143–1153, 2001 Lamb DG, Prigatano GP: Malingering and feigned memory disorders, in Memory Disorders in Psychiatric Practice. Edited by Berrios GE, Hodges JR. Cambridge, England, Cambridge University Press, 2000, pp 456–478 Lindenbaum J, Healton EB, Savage DG, et al: Neuropsychiatric disorders caused by cobalamin deficiency in the absence of anemia or macrocytosis. N Engl J Med 318:1720–1728, 1988 Markand ON: Organic brain syndromes and dementias, in Current Practice of Clinical Electroencephalography. Edited by Daly DD, Pedley TA. New York, Raven, 1990, pp 401–423 Mattis S: Dementia Rating Scale. Odessa, FL, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1988
100
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Mayeux R, Saunders AM, Shea S, et al: Utility of the apolipoprotein E genotype in the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 338:506–511, 1998 McCaddon A, Davies G, Hudson P, et al: Total serum homocysteine in senile dementia of Alzheimer type. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 13:235–239, 1998 McKeith IG, Galasko D, Kosaka K, et al: Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathological diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): report of the consortium on DLB international workshop. Neurology 47:1113–1124, 1996 McShane RH, Nagy Z, Esiri MM, et al: Anosmia in dementia is associated with Lewy bodies rather than Alzheimer’s pathology. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 70:739–743, 2001 Mesholam RI, Moberg PJ Mahr RN, et al: Olfaction in neurodegenerative disease: a meta-analysis of olfactory functioning in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Arch Neurol 55:84–90, 1998 Mohr E, Feldman H, Gauthier S: Canadian guidelines for the development of antidementia therapies: a conceptual summary. Can J Neurol Sci 22:62–71, 1995 Neary D, Snowden JS, Northen B, et al: Dementia of frontal lobe type. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 51:353–361, 1988 Pedley TA, Miller JA: Clinical neurophysiology of aging and dementia, in The Dementias (Advances in Neurology Series, Vol 38). Edited by Mayeux R, Rosen WG. New York, Raven, 1983, pp 31–49 Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, et al: Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 56:303–308, 1999 Pickering-Brown SM, Richardson AM, Snowden JS, et al: Inherited frontotemporal dementia in nine British families associated with intronic mutations in the tau gene. Brain 25 (part 4):732–751, 2002 Poorkaj P, Bird TD, Wijsman E, et al: Tau is a candidate gene for chromosome 17 frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 43:815–825, 1998 Purohit DP, Perl DP, Haroutunian V, et al: Alzheimer disease and related neurodegenerative diseases in elderly patients with schizophrenia: a postmortem neuropathologic study of 100 cases. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:205–211, 1998 Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology: Practice parameter for diagnosis and evaluation of dementia (summary statement). Neurology 44:2203–2206, 1994 Risse SC, Lampe TH, Bird TD, et al: Myoclonus, seizures, and paratonia in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 4:217–225, 1990 Robinson DJ, Merskey H, Blume WT, et al: Electroencephalography as an aid in the exclusion of Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 51:280–284, 1994 Rosenberg RN, Iannaccone ST: The prevention of neurogenetic disease. Arch Neurol 52:356–362, 1995 Sack GH: Acute intermittent porphyria. JAMA 264:1290–1293, 1990 Sadik J, Riquier V, Koskas P, et al: Transcranial Doppler imaging: state of the art. J Radiol 82:821–831, 2001 Schneider JA, Watts RL, Gearing M, et al: Corticobasal degeneration: neuropathologic and clinical heterogeneity. Neurology 48:959–969, 1996
Medical Evaluation
101
Seshadri S, Beiser A, Selhub J, et al: Plasma homocysteine as a risk factor for dementia and Alzheimer's disease. N Engl J Med 346:476–483, 2002 Small GW, Rabins PV, Barry PP, et al: Diagnosis and treatment of Alzheimer disease and related disorders. JAMA 278:1363–1371, 1997 Spillantini MG, Murrell JR, Goedert M, et al: Mutation in the tau gene in familial multiple system tauopathy with presenile dementia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:7737–7741, 1998 Steinhoff BJ, Racker S, Herrendorf G, et al: Accuracy and reliability of periodic sharp wave complexes in Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease. Arch Neurol 53:162–166, 1996 Stevens M, van Duijn CM, Kamphorst W, et al: Familial aggregation in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 50:1541–1545, 1998 Stigsby B: Dementias (Alzheimer’s and Pick’s disease): dysfunctional and structural changes. Am J EEG Technol 28:83–97, 1998 Weiner MF: Beyond the presenting complaint. Psychosomatics 10:310–313, 1969 Wolozin B, Kellman W, Rousseau P: Decreased prevalence of Alzheimer disease associated with 3-hydroxy-3-methylglutaryl coenzyme A reductase inhibitors. Arch Neurol 57:1439–1443, 2000 Zerr I, Bodemer M, Gefeller O, et al: Detection of 14–3–3 protein in the cerebrospinal fluid supports the diagnosis of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease. Ann Neurol 43:32–40, 1998
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER
4
Neuroimaging Gene E. Alexander, Ph.D. Eric M. Reiman, M.D.
During the past three decades, advances in radiological and nuclear medicine technologies have led to the development of high-resolution structural and functional neuroimaging methods that allow for the evaluation of regional brain anatomy and neural activity in the living human brain. With these advances it has become possible to study with increasing sensitivity and specificity the effects of neurodegenerative diseases on the brain. Clinically, these techniques have been used in the differential diagnosis of dementing illness to exclude potentially treatable, spaceoccupying abnormalities (e.g., tumors or subdural hematomas), and they have begun to have a role in the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease and other progressive neurodegenerative disorders. Scientifically, these neuroimaging techniques have investigated the structural, physiological, and chemical effects of dementing illnesses. Neuroimaging research in Alzheimer’s disease has increased the understanding of the brain changes associated with the progressive cognitive and behavioral dysfunction characteristic of this debilitating illness. Furthermore, studies have illustrated the clinical potential for such neuroimaging methods by aiding the detection of Alzheimer’s disease before the onset of clinical symptoms. This provides a way to test the potential of prevention therapies to slow neurological disease progression without having to wait many years to determine whether or when treated individuals develop symptoms. Sensitive and specific markers that identify individuals early in the course of dementing illness offer the opportunity to implement treatments in the very early stages of disease when intervention may be most beneficial. Thus, the use and further development of such neuroimaging techniques hold great promise in aiding the clinical evaluation and treatment of dementing illnesses. In this chapter we review the major structural and functional neuroimaging methods that are currently used to aid the evaluation of dementing illnesses. These include computed tomography (CT), magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), positron emission tomography (PET), and single photon 103
104
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
emission computed tomography (SPECT). In addition, we discuss two neuroimaging techniques with potential clinical applications: functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS). These techniques have already provided important information on physiological and chemical processes in the brain.
Structural Neuroimaging Structural neuroimaging techniques are indicated in the diagnostic evaluation of cognitive impairment to help rule out the presence of potentially treatable conditions, including space-occupying lesions, such as brain tumors, stroke, subdural hematomas, abscesses, infections, and normalpressure hydrocephalus. In addition, structural brain imaging can provide supportive evidence for several forms of dementing illness, including vascular dementia, frontotemporal dementia, asymmetrical cortical degeneration syndromes, and evidence of prior traumatic brain injury. A recent review and consensus report by Knopman et al. (2001), as part of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology, has provided support for the use of either a non-contrast CT or an MRI scan as part of a routine initial diagnostic evaluation for dementia. Interestingly, the report also noted that one recent study showed that 5% of patients had a significant finding on structural imaging without having clinical features discovered on examination or by history that would have suggested the presence of such imaging abnormalities (Chui and Zhang 1997). In addition, a major area of research with structural neuroimaging has focused on applications for aiding early diagnosis and tracking progression in Alzheimer’s disease. Recent studies have emphasized the development and evaluation of rating scales and semi- and fully quantitative methods to analyze structural brain images to evaluate the potential of these techniques for use in making early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease, in identifying individuals at increased risk for developing Alzheimer’s disease, and in tracking disease progression. In this section we review the two major structural imaging techniques that are commonly used in the evaluation of dementia.
Computed Tomography CT provides a valuable method for the visualization of anatomic abnormalities of the skull and brain with high resolution, producing images that reflect tissue density differences with X-ray refraction. This technique relies on the use of a rotating radiation source that transmits a fan beam of X rays through the head. An array of scintillation detectors measure the attenua-
Neuroimaging
105
tion of the X rays as they pass through tissues of different densities, producing differential trajectories. A computer algorithm transforms the attenuation information into sections that reflect the density of the tissues. Generally, tissues with high radiation attenuation, such as bone, appear white, whereas cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), which has low attenuation, appears black. The brain has intermediate levels of attenuation and appears gray. With this technique, visualization of the cerebral gray and white matter and ventricles can permit identification of a wide range of cerebral abnormalities, including the presence of space-occupying lesions and shifts in midline brain structures, cerebral infarction and bleeding, and demyelinization of white matter. Generalized and focal areas of cortical and subcortical atrophy can be observed, and the main divisions between structures such as the basal ganglia and thalamus can be seen. Iodinated and nonionic contrast agents can also be administered intravenously with CT to enhance differences in tissue density, which can help reveal lesions associated with vascular abnormalities and the breakdown of the blood-brain barrier, such as with tumors, infections, and other inflammatory processes. CT has the advantage of being a relatively inexpensive and widely available technology that can produce images of the brain quickly. This can be especially important in detecting lesions that require acute surgical intervention, such as subarachnoid hemorrhages, and for restless patients who have difficulty maintaining their head position during the MRI scanning procedure. Another potentially important advantage of CT over MRI is the absence of adverse reactions to being in a closed space and the need for sedation in the 10% of individuals who may experience this problem. CT also has an advantage in detecting bone abnormalities and calcified tumors, as well as providing a safe structural procedure for patients with pacemakers and other ferromagnetic materials present. A major limitation of CT is the presence of beam-hardening artifacts that make it difficult to visualize brain tissue that is in proximity to bone, such as the posterior fossa. Other limitations of CT include its relatively poor contrast between gray and white matter, its relatively lower sensitivity for detecting white matter lesions, and the potential for allergic reactions to iodinated contrast agents. In a clinical evaluation, CT can provide evidence supportive of several forms of dementing illness. Lobar frontal and anterior temporal cortical atrophy can often be observed in the variants of frontotemporal dementia. Loss of tissue in the caudate nucleus may be visualized in Huntington’s disease. Generalized cortical atrophy and enlarged ventricles can be seen in Alzheimer’s disease, especially as the illness progresses, but the appearance of normal, age-related atrophy is also not unusual in Alzheimer’s disease. Enlarged ventricles can be observed in both obstructive and normalpressure hydrocephalus.
106
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Research efforts to test the potential of CT for aiding the early clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease have focused on studying its ability to provide sensitive and specific measures of brain atrophy in groups of Alzheimer’s disease patients. In addition to evaluating cortical atrophy and ventricular size (DeCarli et al. 1990), a major focus in structural imaging research in dementia has been the development and testing of methods to rate or measure atrophy in structures of the medial temporal lobes, such as the hippocampus, a structure that is affected early in Alzheimer’s disease (Braak and Braak 1996). By acquiring CT images oriented along the long axis of the medial temporal lobe, axial images of the medial temporal lobes and its structures (such as the hippocampus) can be qualitatively rated or quantitatively assessed with linear measurements of width. Some studies using such hippocampal atrophy measures have shown very good sensitivity and specificity for groups of Alzheimer’s disease patients with clinical diagnoses of dementia compared with healthy elderly persons (George et al. 1990). However, in a study using neuropathological diagnosis as the diagnostic standard, medial temporal lobe width measurements provided high sensitivity (95%) but low specificity (40%) for Alzheimer’s disease (Nagy et al. 1999). Given the wide variability in the range of sensitivity and specificity values reported across studies for such quantitative medial temporal lobe measures in CT (and MRI), their use for assisting clinical diagnosis in routine evaluations is not currently supported (Knopman et al. 2001).
Magnetic Resonance Imaging MRI is a high-resolution structural imaging technique that allows for the visualization of brain anatomy with a high degree of contrast between brain tissue types. The anatomical information provided by this imaging method is fundamentally based on the response of hydrogen protons in water to the perturbations created by an externally applied magnetic field. For clinical use, MRI applies strong magnetic fields created by powerful magnets that range from 0.5 to 3.0 T. In the presence of the magnetic field, the hydrogen protons in water precess in the same frequency along the axis of the magnetic field. Brief radiofrequency pulses are delivered that temporarily reorient these protons away from the axis induced by the magnet. After this radiofrequency pulse, the time that it takes for protons to relax back to their original energy state, in realignment with the axis of the magnetic field, is accompanied by the emission of radio waves characteristic of the particular tissue containing the protons. Radiofrequency receivers detect these radio waves, and three parameters are measured: two tissue-specific relaxation constants, known as T1 and T2, and the proton density. T1 relaxation reflects the rate at which the protons in water return to the axis of the mag-
Neuroimaging
107
netic field. T2 relaxation is a measure of the rate at which the protons decline to precess at the same frequency because of the inherent nonuniformities in the surrounding protons. MRI images that are T1 weighted are commonly used to visualize brain anatomy, with gray matter tissue appearing gray, white matter appearing white, and CSF appearing black. T2-weighted images are commonly used to detect the presence of brain pathology, because both CSF and brain lesions, which are often associated with increased water content, appear white. Proton density scans have intermediate image density. In addition, MRI can be performed after the administration of a paramagnetic contrast agent, which (like in CT) can enhance the ability to discern vascular abnormalities and the breakdown in the blood-brain barrier that is associated with the presence of tumor, infection, and other inflammatory conditions. MRI has a number of advantages over CT, including excellent contrast between gray matter, white matter, and CSF; a high level of sensitivity for white matter lesions with T2-weighted images; the ability to obtain volumetric images in any sectional orientation with high spatial resolution of contiguous slices; the availability of paramagnetic contrast agents that are nonallergenic; and the absence of ionizing radiation. Because bone is not visualized with MRI, brain tissue that is in close proximity to the skull can be studied equally well. Because gray matter, white matter, and CSF can be distinguished with MRI, cortical gyri and sulci can be seen in great detail, and even relatively small brain structures—including structures in the basal ganglia, thalamus, and limbic brain regions such as the hippocampus and amygdala—can be visualized quite well. In contrast to CT, MRI is a relatively expensive procedure. It cannot reveal skull abnormalities and calcified tumors, and it generally requires a longer scanning duration, making it prone to motion artifacts. It is potentially less useful when acute surgical intervention is required and with restless patients who have difficulty maintaining their head position for the duration of the imaging procedure. The MRI scanner can produce claustrophobic anxiety in some patients (although sedation may be helpful, and open MRI scanners are available for such patients). Another important contraindication to MRI is the presence of a pacemaker or certain ferromagnetic materials. With a few exceptions, MRI is generally considered the structural imaging method of choice for clinical and research studies of brain anatomy in the evaluation of dementing illness. In MRI, it is common to obtain either a T1-weighted or proton density image together with a T2-weighted image to enhance the ability to screen for brain abnormalities. T1-weighted images can be particularly helpful in detecting generalized and focal cortical atrophy. Narrowing of the cerebral cortex in Alzheimer’s disease can be observed as the illness progresses and
108
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
specific regional patterns of cortical atrophy can be discerned that may correlate with clinical findings. For example, persons with frontotemporal dementia can show prominent cortical atrophy in the prefrontal and anterior temporal brain regions (Miller and Gerhart 1999). In focal dementias, such as primary progressive aphasia, atrophy can appear unilaterally in frontal and temporal regions (Abe et al. 1997). Patients with normal-pressure hydrocephalus may have ventricles that appear disproportionately enlarged compared with cortical atrophy. The abnormalities underlying dementing illnesses that are associated with movement disorders can be revealed by MRI. Atrophy in the caudate nucleus can be observed in Huntington’s disease. Structural changes that are observed in multiple system atrophy, including cerebellar atrophy, may be helpful in distinguishing this disorder from other parkinsonian syndromes. T2-weighted images are especially helpful in detecting the presence of brain infarcts and other lesions due to cerebrovascular abnormalities and demyelinization. In the elderly, T2-weighted images can show areas of increased signal intensity in the white matter around the ventricles or in the deep white matter. This finding can be observed in healthy elders, but it has also been associated with the presence of cardiovascular risk factors such as hypertension, and it appears to occur to a greater extent in Alzheimer’s disease (DeCarli et al. 1996). Although the clinical and pathological significance of white matter hyperintensities are not fully understood, they can be associated with subtle cognitive impairment and are suspected to reflect cerebrovascular pathology in some patients, especially when the hyperintensities are extensive (DeCarli et al. 1995). Current interest in the application of research methods, such as diffusion-weighted tensor MRI, may help to discern the potential role that white matter hyperintensities and other white matter abnormalities can have in the development and progression of dementia. As with CT, MRI can identify space-occupying lesions and midline shifts in brain structures with brain tumors and can help to identify and track the effects of infection and other inflammatory conditions. The use of MRI with a paramagnetic (e.g., gadolinium-based) contrast agent increases the sensitivity for detecting neoplastic and inflammatory lesions. The gadolinium-based contrast agents cross damaged regions of the bloodbrain barrier like iodinated CT contrast media. In multiple sclerosis, gadolinium-based MRI can help distinguish areas of active inflammation within acute demyelinating plaques from the more chronic lesions and may assist in monitoring disease progression over time. Gadolinium-enhanced MRI can also assist in the detection of meningitis, encephalitis, and myelitis and can help distinguish the many types of lesions that can occur in acquired immune deficiency syndrome. Figure 4–1 shows a T1-weighted
O Neuroimaging FIGURE 4–1. Case example of a 53-year-old woman with a suprasellar craniopharyngioma compressing the hypothalamus and midbrain. The patient presented with severe headaches and memory loss, both of which improved after tumor resection. Gadolinium enhanced T1weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan shown in the sagittal, transverse, and coronal views (left to right). Source.
Figure courtesy of R.J. Caselli, M.D.
109
110
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
magnetic resonance image with gadolinium contrast of a suprasellar craniopharyngioma affecting the hypothalamus and midbrain in a patient who was experiencing headaches and memory loss, both of which improved after the tumor was resected. This case example illustrates how structural brain imaging can help to identify potentially treatable causes of memory impairment. Research in the use of MRI to characterize the early and progressive effects of Alzheimer’s disease on the brain have focused on the development of methods to quantify the changes in the size and shape of brain structures that appear particularly vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease neuropathology very early in the clinical course. In these studies, T1-weighted volumetric images are most often used to distinguish brain anatomy with high spatial resolution. One major approach has used manually traced structures in the medial temporal lobes—including the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, and amygdala—to compute the volumes of these brain structures defined by strict anatomical landmarks and decision rules to establish high interrater and intrarater reliabilities. Although this method is relatively labor intensive, requires neuroanatomical expertise, and requires ongoing qualitycontrol procedures to ensure rater reliability, differences between groups of Alzheimer’s disease patients and healthy elderly have been demonstrated using such volumetric measures, with the hippocampus being the brain structure most consistently studied (de Leon et al. 1989; Jack et al. 1992; Krasuski et al. 1998). It has been also shown that these volumetric measures decline with the clinical progression of dementia (Jack et al. 2000). Furthermore, nondemented individuals with increased risk for Alzheimer’s disease have also shown reduced medial temporal lobe volumes compared with healthy control subjects (Jack et al. 1999; Killiany et al. 2000; Krasuski et al. 2002), supporting the potential for using these methods in aiding early detection and tracking the progression of Alzheimer’s disease before the onset of symptoms. Shown in Figure 4–2 is an example of a T1-weighted MRI scan in an Alzheimer’s disease patient with observable atrophy of the medial temporal lobe compared with a scan of a healthy elderly individual. Another brain structure that has been studied with MRI in Alzheimer’s disease is the corpus callosum. Using manually traced area measurements from a midsagittal MRI section, researchers have shown that such measures are reduced in Alzheimer’s disease and that specific subregions of the corpus callosum (i.e., the rostrum and splenium) may be preferentially affected. They are correlated with reductions in cerebral metabolism with PET and neuropsychological performance, and they continue to decline with disease progression (Teipel et al. 1999, 2002). Although it is thought that medial temporal lobe structures such as the hippocampus and entorhi-
Neuroimaging
111
FIGURE 4–2. Examples of T1-weighted MRI scan slices acquired perpendicular to the sylvian fissure for a healthy comparison subject (HC) and an Alzheimer’s disease patient (AD) with mild dementia. The arrow highlights a portion of the left medial temporal lobe with observable atrophy in the vicinity of the hippocampus and amygdala in the Alzheimer’s disease patient. nal cortex are among the earliest affected in Alzheimer’s disease, atrophy of the corpus callosum may have a role in the progressive loss of corticocortical connections that occur as Alzheimer’s disease progresses (Hampel et al. 1998). In addition to investigating areas and volumes of specific brain structures, structural MRI images have been used to quantitate reductions of whole brain volume in sequentially acquired scans. The best-established method for characterizing whole brain atrophy involves the semiautomated computerized digital subtraction of sequential MRI scans to compute annual rates of decline (Fox et al. 1996). This method is highly reliable and effective in detecting differences between groups of Alzheimer’s disease patients and healthy elderly persons. Furthermore, annual decline rates of whole brain volume have distinguished individuals with increased risk for Alzheimer’s disease from healthy elders. These findings suggest that whole-brain MRI digital subtraction may be helpful in evaluating the effects of Alzheimer’s disease throughout the course of illness and may hold promise as an outcome measure in clinical trials (Fox et al. 2000). Promising methodological developments in the analysis of structural MRI data include the use of probabilistic brain maps to compute regional alterations in gray matter, white matter, CSF, and whole brain. This developing technique, known as voxel-based morphometry, uses fully automated linear and nonlinear warping computer algorithms to place brain scans into a common template space with stereotactic coordinates; to segment the image into gray matter, white matter, and CSF; and to compute differences in tissue concentration or degree of deformation to test group differences that
112
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
can reflect alterations in shape and size of specific brain structures, whole brain, or cortical atrophy (Fox et al. 2001; Good et al. 2001; Thompson et al. 2001). Although these methods are only beginning to be applied in Alzheimer’s disease and other neurodegenerative diseases, they hold great promise for enhancing the ability to detect the earliest effects of these diseases, to track their progression, and to evaluate treatments to diminish or delay the progression of neurodegenerative diseases (Reiman et al. 2001a).
Functional Neuroimaging Interest in the use of functional brain imaging in the evaluation of dementing illness has grown in recent years with the increasing availability of these imaging techniques and growing research suggesting that functional imaging methods may have a helpful role in aiding diagnosis. These methods may be especially helpful when patients present with unusual or confusing clinical symptoms. Functional neuroimaging techniques can potentially provide supportive evidence for several forms of dementia, including Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, frontotemporal dementia, and asymmetrical or focal cortical degeneration syndromes. Research in the clinical application of these functional neuroimaging methods has ranged from using simple rating scales with visual inspection to applying complex computerized algorithms that provide probabilistic brain maps of cerebral activity to test the ability of these neuroimaging methods in aiding early diagnosis, in tracking progression in Alzheimer’s disease, and evaluating individuals at risk for developing Alzheimer’s disease. In this section, we review the two most commonly used functional imaging methods in the evaluation of cognitive dysfunction, PET and SPECT. We also discuss the potential for developing clinical applications from two neuroimaging research methods that assess physiological and chemical processes in the brain, fMRI and MRS.
Positron Emission Tomography PET is a functional imaging technique that can provide information on physiological and chemical processes that occur in brain tissue and that may be useful in the evaluation of dementia. In PET, positron-emitting radiotracers are used to construct images of functional brain activity. These unstable radiotracer compounds have an excess of protons in their nuclei. Positrons with the same mass as an electron but with a positive charge are emitted and have the kinetic energy to travel a few millimeters within tissue. When it is nearly at rest, the positron interacts with an unbound electron, causing an annihilation event to occur. This results in two gamma ray photons of 511 KeV energy being emitted at 180 degrees from each other
Neuroimaging
113
(Horwitz 1990). In a PET scanner, the gamma photons are detected by rings of detectors surrounding the head. Pairs of these detectors face each other and are connected by an electronic coincidence circuit that detects the photons emitted at the moment when the positron meets an electron. The multiple pairs of coincidence circuits in the scanner measure the number of photon pairs originating from all angles. Through a computer algorithm, it is possible to determine both the amount and the regional location of activity throughout the brain. Because energy for brain function is produced through the oxidation of glucose and because neurons require energy in the form of adenosine triphosphate (ATP), measurements of the cerebral metabolic rates for glucose with PET provide an index of neural activity (Sokoloff 1982). Under most conditions, cerebral blood flow is closely associated with cerebral metabolism (Sokoloff 1981), allowing measures of blood flow to also act as an index of functional brain activity. Kety and Schmidt (1945) were the first to use a nitrous oxide technique to measure average brain blood flow as a quantitative, in vivo index of human brain function. Interest in evaluating brain function on a regional basis led to the development and use of radioactive tracers to measure blood flow with the use of external scintillation detectors placed around the head to quantify the regional clearance of freely diffusible radioactive gases, such as xenon-133 (Obrist et al. 1967). Although this method represented an advance over the Kety-Schmidt technique, this planar imaging technique was limited to assessing regional brain function over the cortical surface. The inability to measure regional activity from deeper, subcortical structures led to the further development of the tomographic imaging techniques of PET and SPECT. Advances in scanner design and the development of radiolabeled compounds to study various aspects of brain function allow us to measure regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) and the regional cerebral metabolic rates for glucose (rCMRglc) and oxygen (rCMRO2) throughout the human brain with relatively high spatial resolution (Jagust et al. 1993). The development of additional radiolabeled compounds allow for the in vivo assessment of other neurophysiologic processes, including uptake of precursors for neurotransmitter synthesis, neuroreceptor density and binding (Busatto and Pilowsky 1995), and (more recently) cellular signal transduction (Rapoport 2001). Furthermore, the ability to repeat scans over relatively short intervals due to the relatively shorter radioactive half-life of the cerebral blood flow tracer, H215O, has made it possible to assess regional brain activity in the same individual during the performance of different cognitive tasks or behavioral conditions and before and after pharmacological treatment or challenge.
114
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Because PET requires the use of a cyclotron to manufacture positronemitting tracers for local use, its availability has typically been limited to research medical centers. Availability of PET for use in clinical hospital settings has been increasing in recent years because 1) long-lived radioisotopes such as fluorodeoxyglucose can be distributed to other institutions or cities and 2) the number of scans has dramatically increased to accommodate reimbursable indications in the field of oncology. The cost of PET is on the order of about $2,000 per scan, higher than that of SPECT or MRI. Other limitations of this technology include its use of ionizing radiation and its relatively lower spatial and relatively poor temporal resolution compared with that of fMRI. The use of PET to aid clinical diagnosis of dementing illness is based on the distinctive patterns of cerebral metabolism and blood flow that are typically observed in different neurodegenerative diseases. Alzheimer’s disease is most frequently characterized by reductions of cerebral metabolism and blood flow in parietotemporal brain regions early in the disease course, as well as reductions in other association brain regions such as frontal and occipital association cortices as the disease progresses. Figure 4–3 shows a typical pattern of cerebral hypometabolism in a patient with Alzheimer’s disease compared with that of a healthy elderly individual. Although several disorders can be accompanied by cerebral dysfunction in these association brain regions at different stages of dementia severity, PET can provide valuable input to aid clinical diagnosis by confirming Alzheimer’s disease when a parietotemporal deficit is observed (Small and Leiter 1998). PET can also help characterize and follow the progression of Alzheimer’s disease during different stages of dementia severity. Examples of cerebral hypometabolism in Alzheimer’s disease are shown in Figure 4–4 for patients with mild, moderate, and severe dementia compared with those of a healthy elderly individual. In addition, PET can assist in characterizing the pattern of cerebral dysfunction in unusual or atypical cases of dementia. For example, patients with the visual variant of Alzheimer’s disease typically present with visual disturbances as their earliest clinical symptoms. It has been reported that such patients show preferentially greater cerebral metabolic reductions in occipital cortical brain regions compared with those of typical Alzheimer’s disease patients (Pietrini et al. 1996). Figure 4–3 shows an example of a PET scan in a patient with the visual variant of Alzheimer’s disease whose diagnosis was subsequently confirmed at autopsy. Patients with frontotemporal dementia, including Pick’s disease, typically demonstrate reductions in prefrontal and anterior temporal lobe cerebral metabolism and blood flow early in the clinical course (Ishii et al. 1998). Shown in Figure 4–3 is an example of the cerebral metabolic reductions seen with PET in a patient with a clinical diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia. In focal dementias, such as primary progressive aphasia, PET
Neuroimaging 115
FIGURE 4–3. Examples of positron emission tomographic scans measuring glucose metabolism in a healthy comparison subject (HC), a patient with typical Alzheimer’s disease (AD), a patient with the visual variant of Alzheimer’s disease (AD+VS), a patient with frontotemporal dementia (FTD), and a patient with primary progressive aphasia (PA). Higher values of the regional cerebral metabolic rate for glucose (CMRglc) in milligrams per 100 g per minute are indicated by the orange end of the colorimetric scale. Relative to the healthy comparison subject, the Alzheimer’s disease patient without visual symptoms shows the typical pattern of reductions in glucose metabolism in parietal, temporal, and frontal association areas, with preservation in primary sensory regions. The AD+VS patient shows prominent glucose hypometabolism in parietal and occipital areas, including primary visual cortex, along with preservation of metabolism in frontal and temporal regions. The FTD patient shows marked reductions in CMRglc in frontal and anterior temporal regions, and the PA patient shows hypometabolism in the left frontal and temporal cortex.
116
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
FIGURE 4–4. Transverse images at three different levels of brain positron emission tomographic scans in a healthy comparison (HC) subject and in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients with mild, moderate, and severe dementia (from top to bottom). Regional cerebral metabolic rate for glucose (CMRglc) values reported on the colorimetric scale are in milligrams per 100 g per minute. Reductions in CMRglc are present mostly in the association areas and become more severe as Alzheimer’s disease progresses. Primary neocortical areas are relatively spared, even in the late stages of the disease. Source.
Adapted from Grady and Rapoport 1992.
can reveal unilateral reductions in metabolism in frontal and temporal brain regions. Figure 4–3 illustrates cerebral metabolic findings with PET in a patient with progressive aphasia. Reductions in frontal blood flow and metabolism have been reported in other neurological diseases that can lead to dementia, such as progressive supranuclear palsy (Blin et al. 1990), as well as in disorders such as depression (Baxter et al. 1989). Symptomatic patients with Huntington’s disease show observable decrements in metabolism in the caudate nucleus (Kuhl et al. 1984). Vascular dementia can be characterized by widely distributed and varied patterns of cerebral dysfunction that can be observed in the cerebral cortex, cerebellum, and subcortical nuclei (Mielke and Heiss 1998; Yao et al. 1990). In Parkinson’s disease with dementia, cortical hypometabolism can be observed that may be most
Neuroimaging
117
pronounced in parietotemporal cortex and can appear similar to the pattern observed in Alzheimer’s disease (Kuhl et al. 1984; Schapiro et al. 1993). It has been reported that patients with Lewy body dementia show relatively greater cerebral metabolic reductions in occipital cortex (Minoshima et al. 2001). Global hypometabolism together with relatively greater regional decrements in lateral temporal, inferior parietal, and frontal opercular cortex has been reported in corticobasal ganglionic degeneration (Eidelberg et al. 1991). In multiple-system atrophy syndromes, PET can show pontocerebellar hypometabolism (Rosenthal et al. 1988). In a consensus statement, Knopman et al. (2001) cited a recent study with 22 patients that reported 93% sensitivity and 63% specificity for PET using autopsy-confirmed diagnoses (Hoffman et al. 2000). Furthermore, it was indicated that PET may be superior to SPECT in differentiating Alzheimer’s disease from vascular dementia regardless of patient differences in dementia severity (Mielke and Heiss 1998). On the basis of the reviewed studies, the statement indicated that PET shows promise as an adjunct test in the clinical evaluation of dementing illness, but that it is not currently recommended for routine use in the clinical evaluation of dementing illness. In a recent multicenter study by Silverman et al. (2001) to evaluate the ability of PET rCMRglc to detect dementing illness, 284 patients were followed up with longitudinal clinical evaluation of 2 years or more (n=146) or to autopsy (n=138). Using a simple rating scale and visual inspection of the PET scans, the pattern of cerebral metabolism with PET was 93% sensitive and 76% specific in detecting any progressive dementing illness and 93% sensitive and 73% specific for Alzheimer’s disease in persons undergoing evaluation for dementing illness. In addition, it was shown that an individual having a normal PET scan was unlikely to experience progressive cognitive impairment in the subsequent average of 3 years of clinical follow-up. Figure 4–5 shows the typical PET patterns forming six commonly occurring categories for the 284 patients in this study. This is the largest study to date with clinical follow-up and neuropathological confirmation showing the promise of PET in aiding the clinical diagnosis of dementing illness. Efforts to provide additional diagnostic utility for PET have included the development and application of analytical methods that produce computerized probabilistic brain maps (Friston 1995) of brain activity that can be compared to normative samples or characteristic patterns in disease groups (Minoshima et al. 1995; Signorini et al. 1999). With the use of a brainmapping program to compare a group of 14 Alzheimer’s disease patients with mild to moderate dementia to 34 healthy age-matched subjects, the characteristic pattern of reductions involving parietotemporal hypometabolism with additional reductions extending into occipital and frontal association brain regions was observed (Alexander et al. 2002). Figure 4–6 shows the sta-
118
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
FIGURE 4–5. Positron emission tomography (PET) patterns and occurrence of progressive disease in 284 patients undergoing evaluation for dementing illness. PET scans typical of each of six categories of cerebral metabolic patterns (designated N1, N2, N3, P1, P2, and P3) are shown. Images displaying N1, P1, and P3 patterns were acquired with a Siemens ECAT EXACT HR+ scanner; those displaying N2, N3, and P2 patterns were acquired with a Siemens ECAT 931 scanner. Image set in black background was acquired using a GE Signa 1.5 T MRI scanner, TR=2,100, TE=80. L indicates outcome established by longitudinal monitoring; H indicates diagnosis established by histopathologic examination. *Includes 3 cases of Alzheimer’s disease, 1 case of Alzheimer’s disease plus progressive supranuclear palsy, and 3 cases of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease. **Includes 86 cases of Alzheimer’s disease; 4 cases of Alzheimer’s disease plus Lewy bodies; 1 case of Alzheimer’s disease plus Parkinson’s disease; 1 case of Alzheimer’s disease plus cerebrovascular disease; 1 case of Alzheimer’s disease plus Lewy bodies and cerebrovascular disease; 7 cases of frontotemporal dementia; 6 cases of Lewy body disease; 3 cases of subcortical gliosis; 2 cases of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease; 1 case of progressive supranuclear palsy; and 1 case of Kuf’s lipofuscinosis. Source. Reprinted from Silverman DHS, Small GW, Chang CY, et al.: “Neuroimaging in Evaluation of Dementia: Regional Brain Metabolism and Long-Term Outcome.” Journal of the American Medical Association 286:2120–2127, 2001. Used with permission. Copyright © American Medical Association.
Neuroimaging
119
FIGURE 4–6. Reductions in regional cerebral metabolism (in milligrams per 100 g per minute) in Alzheimer’s disease patients (n=14) compared with healthy comparison subjects (n=34). Significant (P<0.001, uncorrected for multiple comparisons) voxels with reduced metabolism are shown in the orange color scale on lateral and medial projections for the right and left hemispheres. Prominent reductions in the Alzheimer’s disease group are observed in association cortex, including parietal, temporal, occipital, and frontal brain regions. Source. Reprinted from Alexander GE, Chen K, Pietrini P, et al.: “Longitudinal PET Evaluation of Cerebral Metabolic Decline in Dementia: A Potential Outcome Measure in Alzheimer’s Disease Treatment Studies.” American Journal of Psychiatry 159:738–745, 2002. Used with permission. Copyright 2002, American Psychiatric Association.
tistical probability map of the brain areas that were reduced in the Alzheimer’s disease patients compared with those of the healthy subjects in this study. Furthermore, when these Alzheimer’s disease patients were followed up after 1 year, PET revealed additional cerebral metabolic reductions in parietotemporal and frontal brain regions, supporting a progression of cerebral dysfunction with the progression of illness. Figure 4–7 shows the probability map of brain regions that declined over 1 year in the Alzheimer’s disease group. Importantly, it has been shown that cognitively intact individuals having one or more apolipoprotein E e4 (ApoEe4) alleles show decrements in cerebral metabolism and rates of decline with PET in brain regions that are typically reduced in Alzheimer’s disease (Reiman et al 1996, 2001b). Figure 4–8 shows areas of hypometabolism revealed by PET in a group of 11 cognitively normal individuals with two ApoEe4 alleles compared with 22 non-
120
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
FIGURE 4–7. Decline in regional cerebral metabolism (in milligrams per 100 g per minute) in Alzheimer’s disease patients (n=14) from baseline to 1-year follow-up. Significant (P<0.005, uncorrected for multiple comparisons) voxels with reduced metabolism are shown in the orange color scale on lateral and medial projections for the right and left hemispheres. Declines in cerebral metabolism over time are observed in parietal, temporal, and frontal association regions. Source. Reprinted from Alexander GE, Chen K, Pietrini P, et al.: “Longitudinal Evaluation of Cerebral Metabolic Decline in Dementia: A Potential Outcome Measure in Alzheimer’s Disease Treatment Studies.” American Journal of Psychiatry 159:738–745, 2002. Used with permission. Copyright 2002, American Psychiatric Association.
carriers of ApoEe4. Furthermore, the use of sophisticated multivariate statistical methods such as discriminant and principal-component-based analyses offers the potential to identify combinations of brain regions and patterns of subtle metabolic abnormalities that distinguish patient groups and can be applied to PET scans of individuals or can potentially aid in predicting individuals who are likely to develop dementia (Alexander and Moeller 1994; Azari et al. 1993; Pietrini et al. 1993). In addition to PET rCMRglc and rCBF studies performed in the baseline resting state, a number of studies have used cognitive tasks and sensory stimulation during rCBF or rCMRglc measurement to assess the functional response to brain activation in persons who have and who are at risk for developing dementing illness. Although resting-state studies provide valuable information on the regional distribution of the pathophysiological effects of disease, functional neuroimaging studies during a cognitive task
Neuroimaging
121
FIGURE 4–8. Regions of the brain with reduced regional cerebral metabolic rate for glucose (rCMRglc) in 11 e4 homozygotes and their relation to brain regions with reduced rCMRglc in 37 patients with probable Alzheimer’s disease. In this analysis, an automated algorithm was used to compare positron emission tomographic images of rCMRglc in 11 e4 homozygotes (mean age, 55 years) and 22 control subjects who did not carry the e4 allele and were who matched for sex, age, and level of education (mean age, 56 years). The three-dimensional surface-projection map of reductions in rCMRglc in the e4 homozygotes (z score³2.58; P£0.005) was superimposed on regions of hypometabolism in the probable Alzheimer’s disease patients. The purple areas are regions in which rCMRglc was significantly reduced only in the group with probable Alzheimer’s disease. The blue areas are regions in which rCMRglc was significantly reduced in both the e4 homozygotes and the probable Alzheimer’s disease patients, and the green areas are regions in which rCMRglc was significantly reduced only in the e4 homozygotes. In comparison with those of the control subjects, the e4 homozygotes had significantly reduced rCMRglc bilaterally in the same posterior cingulate (PC), parietal (Pa), temporal (Te), and prefrontal (PF1) regions as in the patients with probable Alzheimer’s disease, as well as in additional prefrontal (PF2) regions. Source. Reprinted from Reiman EM, Caselli RJ, Yun LS, et al.: “Preclinical Evidence of Alzheimer’s Disease in Persons Homozygous for the A4 Allele for Apolipoprotein E.” New England Journal of Medicine 334:752–758, 1996. Used with permission. Copyright © 1996 Massachusetts Medical Society. All rights reserved.
122
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
or exposure to “passive” sensory stimulation can directly assess, in vivo, how the disease process alters brain networks that are important for specific cognitive and behavioral functions. In general, these studies are performed by contrasting brain responses during a stimulation condition with a baseline or control condition to determine the brain regions that have increased or decreased activity from baseline during a specific cognitive or sensory experience. These studies have shown that PET can be used as a stress test for brain function that may elicit important information about the neural systems that become impaired as dementing illness progresses (Duara et al. 1992; Mentis et al. 1996; Pietrini et al. 2000) and in persons at increased risk for developing dementing illness (Pietrini et al. 1997). PET measurements of neurochemical processes have begun to be applied to the study of Alzheimer’s disease and other dementing illnesses. For example, radiotracer methods can be used to estimate the density and affinity of muscarinic and nicotinic receptors, synaptic concentrations of acetylcholine, and acetylcholinesterase activity (e.g., Kuhl et al. 1999). Given the vulnerability of the cholinergic system to the effects of Alzheimer’s disease, such radioligands may help characterize brain dysfunction as the disease progresses. In addition, PET (and SPECT) methods continue to be developed and tested for the characterization of the pathological features of Alzheimer’s disease, including plaques and tangles. Recently, dicyanodimethylamino-naphthalen-propene (DDNP) compounds were found to label plaques and tangles using fluorescence microscopic and in vitro autoradiographic studies of a postmortem Alzheimer’s disease brain. Using a fluorine-18 derivative of this compound (FDDNP) in a preliminary PET study, Alzheimer’s disease patients had greater radiotracer accumulation and slower radiotracer clearance than healthy control subjects in brain areas that included the hippocampal and temporal cortical regions (SoghiJadid et al. 2000). If a neuroimaging technique for the in vivo characterization of plaques proves to have adequate sensitivity and specificity, it could play an especially important role in the assessment of treatments designed to reduce plaque burden.
Single Photon Emission Computed Tomography SPECT imaging can provide useful information on physiological and chemical processes in the brain that may aid the clinical evaluation of cognitive dysfunction. In SPECT imaging, the radiopharmaceuticals use isotopes that emit a single gamma ray photon. This basic difference from PET (in which the radiotracers emit two photons) accounts for the major differences in instrumentation and capabilities for measuring functional brain activity between these tomographic imaging techniques.
Neuroimaging
123
SPECT imaging of the brain involves the administration of a radiotracer and the use of a tomographic scanner to measure the regional distribution of the radiotracer in the brain. In SPECT, as in PET, scintillation crystals are used to detect the radioactivity emitted from the tracers. Because only single photons are emitted and they travel in all directions, SPECT depends on the use of collimators to determine the point of origin of the emitted photons to localize regional brain activity. With the implementation of multiple-head SPECT cameras, sensitivity and spatial resolution have greatly improved in recent years (Links 1993). However, SPECT continues to have somewhat lower spatial resolution than PET and presents relatively greater difficulty in identifying deep brain structures (Small and Leiter 1998). The main radiopharmaceuticals currently used in SPECT to assess neural activity produce measures of rCBF, although agents to assess characteristics of neuroreceptor systems have also been developed (see, e.g., Eckelman et al. 1984; Kung et al. 1990). Commonly used tracers to measure rCBF in SPECT include [99mTc]hexamethylpropylene amine (HMPAO) and [99mTc]ethyl cysteinate dimer (ECD). In addition, the radiotracer [123I]isopropyl iodoamphetamine (IMP) was formerly widely used to measure rCBF with SPECT. Although it was employed initially in non-tomographic imaging, the use of xenon-133 has continued with SPECT. Given its relatively fast clearance, this latter tracer allows for multiple scans in the same SPECT session, but it offers relatively low spatial resolution (Newberg et al. 1995). Compared with the positron-emitting tracers employed in PET, the radioisotopes used in SPECT emit lower-energy photons (80–159 keV) that are generally more susceptible to attenuation as the photons pass through tissue. Therefore, brain structures such as the basal ganglia, which are farther from the detector surface, have poorer resolution than do cortical brain regions. This combined with the difficulties in modeling tracer behavior makes full regional quantitation of absolute rCBF in SPECT impractical. As is also often done with PET, regional brain counts are typically referenced to global counts or relatively preserved brain regions (e.g., cerebellum) to provide a semiquantitative measure of cerebral perfusion. Also, the radioactive half-lives of 6 hours for technetium-99m and 13 hours for iodine-123, as well as the relatively slow washout of the higher-resolution tracers, limit the ability to obtain multiple scans to assess rCBF under different test conditions during the same scanning session. The greater ease of production and the potential for longer-term storage of these radiotracers compared with those of positron-emitting agents, however, provide the advantage of eliminating the need for a local cyclotron.
124
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Despite the technical limitations of SPECT relative to PET, its wide availability and lower cost combined with the ability to detect regional reductions in cortical perfusion with a spatial resolution approaching that of PET has enhanced its role as a functional imaging method to aid the assessment of neurodegenerative disease. As with PET, regional patterns of cerebral perfusion in SPECT can provide important information that can help to support the clinical diagnosis of dementing illness. Using SPECT, numerous investigators have identified patterns characteristic of different forms of dementia (Bonte et al. 1986, 1990; Holman 1986; Jagust et al. 1987; Sharp et al. 1986; Van Heertum et al. 1993). For example, like PET, parietotemporal hypoperfusion is commonly observed in Alzheimer’s disease. Persons with frontotemporal dementia typically demonstrate reductions in prefrontal and anterior temporal lobe blood flow. Persons with Huntington’s disease can show reductions in the caudate nucleus. Vascular dementia can appear with varied patterns of cerebral dysfunction. When dementia is present in persons with Parkinson’s disease, the rCBF pattern is similar to that of Alzheimer’s disease. A study including 363 patients with dementia studied prospectively with rCBF SPECT suggested that SPECT may be helpful in distinguishing Alzheimer’s disease from vascular dementia and frontotemporal dementia (Talbot et al. 1998). However, SPECT appeared to be less useful in distinguishing Lewy body dementia from Alzheimer’s disease and vascular from frontotemporal dementia. Diagnoses in this study were based on clinical criteria; therefore, the possibility of misdiagnosis or comorbid neuropathological abnormalities in the sample cannot be ruled out. Knopman et al. (2001) noted that the sensitivity and specificity of SPECT for differentiating Alzheimer’s disease from non–Alzheimer’s disease dementia using visual inspection was not consistently better than clinical diagnosis alone and that PET appears to offer greater diagnostic accuracy than does SPECT. The potential for improving the sensitivity and specificity of SPECT for diagnosis has been suggested in studies that have used automated analysis methods (Bartenstein et al. 1997; Johnson et al. 1993). Using information from structural imaging may also enhance the ability of SPECT to aid in the clinical evaluation of cognitive impairment (Jobst et al. 1998).
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging The relatively new technology of fMRI has rapidly developed into an important neuroimaging method in the last 10 years. This technique can provide information on regional cerebral blood flow and volume in the
Neuroimaging
125
baseline resting state or during state-dependent regional brain changes in response to manipulation of cognitive or behavioral task conditions. In fMRI, high-field systems (typically 1.5 T or higher) are used with the addition of specialized hardware to allow for ultrafast imaging with either an echo planar platform or a spiral multishot imaging platform. This method is based on the use of endogenous or exogenous paramagnetic agents to produce measurable magnetic susceptibility contrast. The contrast between oxygenated and deoxygenated hemoglobin provides an important approach in fMRI to detect local neural activity in response to changes in cognitive or behavioral states. The use of blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) fMRI as an index of functional brain activity is based on the facts that deoxygenated hemoglobin is paramagnetic and that increases in local neuronal activity are associated with increases in blood flow, blood volume, and oxygen delivery to the region being activated. In fact, because oxygen delivery exceeds demand in the activated brain area, fMRI actually detects a decrease in venule deoxyhemoglobin. Although the BOLD method has great potential for evaluating state-dependent changes in local neural activity and how these changes are altered by aging and age-related neurodegenerative disease, it is not well suited for characterizing differences in baseline measurements of resting-state brain function. Cerebral blood volume can also be measured using exogenous paramagnetic media, such as gadolinium-related agents with echo planar imaging to dynamically track blood in small cerebral vessels. Measures of cerebral blood flow with fMRI have been developed in which arterial blood is magnetically tagged before arrival in the imaging plane—a method known as arterial spin labeling. These methods use echo planar imaging to track the flow of blood dynamically through small vessels. Major advantages of fMRI include its relatively high spatial and temporal resolution, the ability to acquire images quickly, and the absence of ionizing radiation, allowing for frequent repeated measurements. Limitations of fMRI include greater potential for confounding effects of head movement, potential for some individuals to experience anxiety in the MRI scanner, the practical limitations of working in a high magnetic field for the controlled presentation of complex cognitive and behavioral tasks, the potential for signal dropout and spatial distortions in certain brain regions near the sinuses (e.g., anterior temporal cortex), and limited ability to measure baseline state differences between subjects or scanning sessions. With further advances (e.g., in the development of perfusion MRI), some of these limitations may be addressed. Recent research efforts in perfusion MRI have enabled quantitative imaging of baseline cerebral blood flow with fMRI over an increasingly larger field of view in the brain and have demonstrated reductions in pari-
126
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
etotemporal cortices in Alzheimer’s disease patients (Sandson et al. 1996). In addition, BOLD fMRI studies have begun to demonstrate the neurophysiologic substrates of cognitive dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease using specific cognitive paradigms as behavioral probes to test for alterations in brain function in the context of disease (e.g., Buckner et al. 2000; Kato et al. 2001). The potential for using fMRI to directly test the effects of pharmacological treatments on brain function associated with specific cognitive processes has been demonstrated by Furey et al. (2000). In this study, the effect of enhancing the cholinergic neurotransmitter system with administration of the cholinesterase inhibitor physostigmine during the performance of a face working memory task was evaluated in healthy adults. Cholinergic neurotransmitter enhancement appeared to improve short-term memory performance by augmenting function in brain regions associated with the selectivity of perceptual processing during the encoding component of the task. Figure 4–9 shows representative brain regions that were activated during the face working memory task and were modulated by physostigmine compared with placebo. This finding provides initial support for the use of fMRI to help directly evaluate the effects of treatments designed to improve the function of neural systems that support cognitive processes and has important implications for the evaluation of potential treatments for Alzheimer’s disease and other forms of dementing illness.
FIGURE 4–9. Example of ventral and dorsal extrastriate visual areas that were activated in a working memory task and time series data from the two functional magnetic resonance imaging experimental sessions. Three axial slices from a single representative subject are shown with the voxels that showed a significant response to any component of the working memory task shown in color. Arrows indicate the locations of ventral occipital (A), ventral temporal (B), dorsal occipital (C), and intraparietal (D) regions. Source. Reprinted from Furey ML, Pietrini P, Haxby JV: “Cholinergic Enhancement and Increased Selectivity of Perceptual Processing During Working Memory.” Science 290: 2315–2319, 2000. Copyright 2000 American Association for the Advancement of Science.
Neuroimaging
127
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy MRS, a developing neuroimaging application that provides information on biochemical and physiological processes in brain tissue, has potential for clinical use in evaluating the effects of dementing illness. This magnetic resonance technique produces chemical spectra based on the extent to which the response of certain atoms (e.g., 31P, 1H) to perturbations in an externally applied magnetic field is affected by surrounding atoms. MRS is typically performed in homogeneous, high magnetic fields using MRI scanners (commonly 1.5 T or higher) with a surface coil. It uses radiofrequency pulses that selectively resonate to the particular chemical element being studied to determine relative tissue concentrations of specific compounds that contain that element. For example, 31P MRS provides information on the relative concentrations of phosphomonoesters and phosphodiesters that reflect precursors and degradation products of membrane phospholipids. ATP, adenosine diphosphate (ADP), and phosphocreatine are among the compounds involved in high-energy phosphate metabolism. In a study by Murphy et al. (1993), it was shown that the characteristic reductions of glucose metabolism seen with PET in Alzheimer’s disease was not related to rate limitations in glucose delivery, inherently abnormal glucose metabolism, or abnormal coupling between oxidation and phosphorylation. In 1H MRS, information is provided on the concentrations of hydrogen-containing compounds. These include N-acetylaspartate (NAA) (thought to be localized in neurons and a marker of neuronal integrity); myo-inositol (mI) (a membrane phospholipid precursor thought to have a role in cellular signal transduction and osmoregulation); choline-containing compounds (cho), some of which have a role in membrane phospholipid synthesis and degradation; and creatine, often used as a reference value in computing relative concentrations of the other metabolites. In a 1H MRS study by Huang et al. (2001), external standards placed in the MRI scanner to measure absolute brain concentrations of NAA, mI, cho, and creatine were used to compare groups of Alzheimer’s disease patients and healthy age-matched control subjects. Lower levels of NAA and higher levels of mI and cho were found in the Alzheimer’s disease patients. Furthermore, a statistical discriminant analysis using these 1H MRS findings combined with CSF volume measures distinguished the Alzheimer’s disease patients from control subjects with 100% correct classification. With the use of the same 1H MRS metabolite quantitation method, nondemented individuals with Down syndrome were found by Huang et al. (1999) to have higher levels of mI and cho than were age-matched comparison subjects, and the older Down syndrome group had higher levels of mI than did the young Down syndrome group. Examples of representative 1H MRS spectra from this study are shown in Figure 4–10. Because adults with
128
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
FIGURE 4–10. Representative 1H magnetic resonance spectra from Down syndrome and comparison subjects. Spectra are from a 36-yearold Down syndrome subject (top left panel), a 38-year-old comparison subject (bottom left panel), a 50-year-old Down syndrome subject (top right panel), and a 53-year-old comparison subject (bottom right panel). The spectra were all obtained from the same occipital voxel. The identified resonance peaks in the bottom left panel are N-acetylaspartate (NAA), 2.02 ppm; creatine (Cr), 3.03 ppm; cholinecontaining compounds (Cho), 3.23 ppm; and myo-inositol (mI), 3.56 ppm. The upward arrows indicate observable increases of mI and Cho peak amplitudes in Down syndrome subjects. Source. Reprinted from Huang W, Alexander GE, Daly EM, et al.: “Brain Myoinositol Is Elevated in the Predementia Phase of Alzheimer’s Disease in Down Syndrome Adults: A 1H MRS Study.” American Journal of Psychiatry 156:1879–1886, 1999. Used with permission. Copyright 1999, American Psychiatric Association.
Neuroimaging 129
130
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Down syndrome have a very high risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease pathology and dementia with increasing age (Holland and Oliver 1995), these findings suggest that 1H MRS has the potential to detect chemical abnormalities in persons with Alzheimer’s disease and in those with increased risk for Alzheimer’s disease before the onset of cognitive symptoms. As a noninvasive in vivo method that can obtain relatively unique information about brain chemistry, MRS has the potential to complement other neuroimaging techniques by adding to our understanding of disease mechanisms and potentially adding a neuroimaging method that can enhance the early detection and track the progression of Alzheimer’s disease and other dementing illnesses. MRS methods are currently limited by low sensitivity for the detection of compounds that occur in minute concentrations and by relatively poor spatial resolution. They are also provide limited ability to compare regional concentrations of metabolites throughout the brain. Further research is needed to discern the role of MRS in aiding differential diagnosis of dementia, early detection of individuals at risk for dementia, and tracking the progression of disease over time.
Summary and Future Directions Structural and functional neuroimaging techniques provide valuable tools in the evaluation of the anatomical and neurophysiologic effects of dementing illness that can aid the clinical diagnosis and assist in monitoring disease progression. Research using such neuroimaging methods has increased knowledge of the mechanisms of cognitive dysfunction and has aided efforts at early detection and differential diagnosis. These methods have the potential to help evaluate treatment effects in the brains of individuals with dementing illness and those at risk. New developments in the acquisition and analysis of neuroimaging data hold the promise of advancing the ability to characterize the brain structures, functional networks, and chemical processes that are altered by healthy aging and are progressively impaired by neurodegenerative disease. Such developments are likely to further enhance the role of both structural and functional neuroimaging methods in the evaluation of Alzheimer’s disease and other dementing illnesses. Neuroimaging techniques have the potential to act as surrogate markers that can identify the earliest effects of disease—even before the onset of clinical symptoms—to evaluate prevention therapies for Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias. This work, in part, will capitalize on the important prior research and new developments in identifying genetic factors that modify risk for dementing illnesses, such as ApoE genotype in Alzheimer’s disease. Current efforts to extend the use
Neuroimaging
131
of in vivo neuroimaging to evaluate brain changes in animal models of Alzheimer’s disease present a major opportunity to enhance the development of treatments and provide a potentially efficient, focused, and rational approach for screening promising candidate therapies before investing the substantial resources required for initial human clinical trials. Continued research in the application of analytical methods to evaluate longitudinal changes that occur with disease progression will be important to further current efforts to evaluate the use of structural and functional imaging as potential outcome measures in long-term clinical trials. This effort may be significantly enhanced by current work in developing new radiotracers that can allow direct imaging of pathological targets for treatment, such as >-amyloid and gliosis, with functional imaging (Cagnin et al. 2001; ShoghiJadid et al. 2000). Along with other clinical and experimental methods, neuroimaging has the potential to help clarify how brain abnormalities are related to specific aspects of behavioral dysfunction, individual differences in the clinical course, patient differences in response to treatment, and the development and progression of histopathology in neurodegenerative disease. The development of diagnostic algorithms that can combine functional neuroimaging results obtained at rest and during cognitive or sensory stimulation with findings from other imaging, clinical, genetic, or demographic data may ultimately offer the most powerful tool to aid the clinician in the early differential diagnosis of dementing illness.
References Abe K, Ukita H, Yanagihara T: Imaging in primary progressive aphasia. Neuroradiology 39:556–559, 1997 Alexander GE, Moeller JR: Application of the Scaled Subprofile Model to functional imaging in neuropsychiatric disorders: a principal component approach to modeling regional patterns of brain function in disease. Hum Brain Mapp 2:79–94, 1994 Alexander GE, Chen K, Pietrini P, et al: Longitudinal PET evaluation of cerebral metabolic decline in dementia: a potential outcome measure in Alzheimer’s disease treatment studies. Am J Psychiatry 159:738–745, 2002 Azari NP, Pettigrew KD, Schapiro MB, et al: Early detection of Alzheimer’s disease: a statistical approach using positron emission tomographic data. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 13:438–447, 1993 Bartenstein P, Minoshima S, Hirsch C, et al: Quantitative assessment of cerebral blood flow in patients with Alzheimer’s disease by SPECT. J Nucl Med 38: 1095–1101, 1997 Baxter LR, Schwartz JC, Phelps ME, et al: Reduction of prefrontal cortex glucose metabolism common to three types of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 46: 243–250, 1989
132
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Blin J, Baron JC, Dubois B, et al: Positron emission tomography study of progressive supranuclear palsy: brain hypometabolic pattern and clinicometabolic correlations. Arch Neurol 47:747–752, 1990 Bonte FJ, Devous MD, Stokely EM, et al: Testing vasoreactivity with SPECT study of regional cerebral blood flow in patients with Alzheimer or cerebrovascular disease. J Comput Assist Tomogr 10:579–583, 1986 Bonte FJ, Hom J, Tintner R, et al: Single photon tomography in Alzheimer’s disease and the dementias. Semin Nucl Med 20:342–352, 1990 Braak H, Braak E: Evolution of the neuropathology of Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurol Scand Suppl 165:3–12, 1996 Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, Sanders AL, et al: Functional brain imaging of young, nondemented, and demented older adults. J Cogn Neurosci 12 (suppl 2):24– 34, 2000 Busatto GF, Pilowsky LS: Neuroreceptor mapping with in-vivo imaging techniques: principles and applications. Br J Hosp Med 53:309–313, 1995 Cagnin A, Brooks DJ, Kennedy AM, et al: In-vivo measurement of activated microglia in dementia. Lancet 358:461–467, 2001 Chui H, Zhang Q: Evaluation of dementia: a systematic study of the usefulness of the American Academy of Neurology’s practice parameters. Neurology 49:925–935, 1997 DeCarli C, Kaye JA, Horwitz B: Critical analysis of the use of computer assisted transverse axial tomography to study human brain in aging and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neurology 40:872–883, 1990 DeCarli C, Murphy DGM, Tranh M, et al: The effect of white matter hyperintensity volume on brain structure, cognitive performance, and cerebral metabolism in 51 healthy adults. Neurology 45:2077–2084, 1995 DeCarli C, Grady CL, Clark CM, et al: Comparison of positron emission tomography, cognition, and brain volume in Alzheimer’s disease with and without severe white matter abnormalities. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 60:158–167, 1996 de Leon MJ, George AE, Stylopoulos LA, et al: Early marker for Alzheimer’s disease: the atrophic hippocampus. Lancet 2:672–673, 1989 Duara R, Barker WW, Chang J, et al: Viability of neocortical function shown in behavioral activation state PET studies in Alzheimer disease. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 12:927–934, 1992 Eckelman WC, Reba RC, Rzeszotarski WJ, et al: External imaging of cerebral muscarinic acetylcholine receptors. Science 223:291–293, 1984 Eidelberg D, Dhawan V, Moeller JR, et al: The metabolic landscape of corticobasal ganglionic degeneration: regional asymmetries studied with positron emission tomography. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 54:856–862, 1991 Fox NC, Freeborough PA, Rossor MN: Visualisation and quantification of rates of atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 348:94–97, 1996 Fox NC, Cousens S, Scahill R, et al: Using serial registered brain magnetic resonance imaging to measure disease progression in Alzheimer disease: power calculation and estimates of sample size to detect treatment effects. Arch Neurol 57:339–344, 2000
Neuroimaging
133
Fox NC, Crum WR, Scahill RI, et al: Imaging of onset and progression of Alzheimer’s disease with voxel-compression mapping of serial magnetic resonance images. Lancet 358:201–205, 2001 Friston KJ: Commentary and opinion, II: statistical parametric mapping: ontology and current issues. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 15:361–370, 1995 Furey ML, Pietrini P, Haxby JV: Cholinergic enhancement and increased selectivity of perceptual processing during working memory. Science 290:2315–2319, 2000 George AE, de Leon MJ, Stylopoulos LA, et al: CT diagnostic features of Alzheimer disease: importance of the choroidal/hippocampal fissure complex. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 11:101, 1990 Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, et al: A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 14:21–36, 2001 Grady CL, Rapoport SI: Cerebral metabolism in aging and dementia, in Handbook of Mental Health and Aging. Edited by Birren J, Sloane RB, Cohen G. San Diego, CA, Academic Press, 1992, pp 201–228 Hampel H, Teipel SJ, Alexander GE, et al: Corpus callosum atrophy is a possible indicator for cell type specific neuronal degeneration and cognitive impairment in Alzheimer disease: a magnetic resonance imaging analysis. Arch Neurol 55:193–198, 1998 Hoffman JM, Welsh-Bohmer KA, Hanson M, et al: FDG PET imaging in patients with pathologically verified dementia. J Nucl Med 41:1920–1928, 2000 Holland AJ, Oliver C: Down’s syndrome and the links with Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 39:111–114, 1995 Holman B: Perfusion and receptor SPECT in the dementias. J Nucl Med 27:855– 860, 1986 Horwitz B: Quantification and analysis of positron emission tomography metabolic data, in Positron Emission Tomography in Dementia. Edited by Duara R. New York, Wiley-Liss, 1990, pp 13–70 Huang W, Alexander GE, Daly EM, et al: Brain myo-inositol is elevated in the predementia phase of Alzheimer’s disease in Down syndrome adults: a 1H MRS study. Am J Psychiatry 156:1879–1886, 1999 Huang W, Alexander GE, Chang L, et al: Brain metabolite concentration and dementia severity in Alzheimer’s disease: a 1H MRS study. Neurology 57, 626– 632, 2001 Ishii K, Sakamoto S, Sasaki M, et al: Cerebral glucose metabolism in patients with frontotemporal dementia. J Nucl Med 39:1875–1878, 1998 Jack CR, Petersen RC, O’Brien P, et al: MR-based hippocampal volumetry in the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 42:183–188, 1992 Jack CR, Petersen RC, Xu YC, et al: Prediction of AD with MRI-based hippocampal volume in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 52:1397–1403, 1999 Jack CR, Petersen RC, Xu Y, et al: Rates of hippocampal atrophy correlate with change in clinical status in aging and AD. Neurology 55:484–489, 2000 Jagust WJ, Budinger TF, Reed BR: The diagnosis of dementia with single photon emission computed tomography. Arch Neurol 44:258–262, 1987
134
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Jagust WJ, Eberling JL, Richardson BC, et al: The cortical topography of temporal lobe hypometabolism in early Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Res 629:189–198, 1993 Jobst KA, Barnetson LPD, Shepstone BJ: Accurate prediction of histologically confirmed Alzheimer’s disease and the differential diagnosis of dementia: the use of NINCDS-ADRDA and DSM-III-R criteria, SPECT, x-ray CT, and apo E4 in medial temporal lobe dementias. Int Psychogeriatr 10:271–302, 1998 Johnson KA, Kijewski MF, Becker JA, et al: Quantitative brain SPECT in Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging. J Nucl Med 34:2044–2048, 1993 Kato T, Knopman D, Liu H: Dissociation of regional activation in mild AD during visual encoding: a functional MRI study. Neurology 57:812–816, 2001 Kety SS, Schmidt CF: The determination of cerebral blood flow in man by the use of nitrous oxide in low concentrations. Am J Physiol 143:53–66, 1945 Killiany RJ, Gomez-Isla T, Moss M, et al: Use of structural magnetic resonance imaging to predict who will get Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 47:430–439, 2000 Knopman DS, DeKosky ST, Cummings JL, et al: Practice parameter: diagnosis of dementia (an evidence-based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 56:1143–1153, 2001 Krasuski JS, Alexander GE, Daly E, et al: Volumes of medial temporal lobe structures in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment (and in healthy controls). Biol Psychiatry 43:60–68, 1998 Krasuski JS, Alexander GE, Horwitz B, et al: Relation of medial temporal lobe volumes to age and memory function in nondemented adults with Down’s syndrome: implications for the prodromal phase of Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 159:74–81, 2002 Kuhl DE, Metter EJ, Riege WH, et al: Patterns of cerebral glucose utilization in Parkinson’s disease and Huntington’s disease. Ann Neurol 15 (suppl): S119– S125, 1984 Kuhl DE, Koeppe RA, Minoshima S, et al: In vivo mapping of cerebral acetylcholinesterase activity in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 52:691–699, 1999 Kung HF, Ohmomo Y, Kung MP: Current and future radiopharmaceuticals for brain imaging with single photon emission computed tomography. Semin Nucl Med 20:290–302, 1990 Links JM: Multidetector single-photon emission tomography: are two (or three or four) heads really better than one? Eur J Nucl Med 20:440–447, 1993 Mentis MJ, Horwitz B, Grady CL, et al: Visual cortical dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease evaluated with a temporally graded “stress test” during PET. Am J Psychiatry 153:32–40, 1996 Mielke R, Heiss WD: Positron emission tomography for diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. J Neural Transm Suppl 53:237–250, 1998 Miller BL, Gerhart R: Neuroimaging in the diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 10:71–74, 1999
Neuroimaging
135
Minoshima S, Frey KA, Koeppe RA, et al: A diagnostic approach in Alzheimer’s disease using three-dimensional stereotactic surface projections of fluorine-18FDG PET. J Nucl Med 36:1238–1248, 1995 Minoshima S, Foster NL, Sima AA, et al: Alzheimer’s disease versus dementia with Lewy bodies: cerebral metabolic distinction with autopsy confirmation. Ann Neurol 50:358–365, 2001 Murphy DGM, Bottomley PA, Salerno JA, et al: An in-vivo study of phosphorus and glucose metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease using magnetic resonance spectroscopy and PET. Arch Gen Psychiatry 50:341–349, 1993 Nagy Z, Hindley NJ, Braak H, et al: Relationship between clinical and radiological diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer’s disease and the extent of neuropathology as reflected by “stages”: a prospective study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 10:109–114, 1999 Newberg AB, Alavi A, Payer F: Single photon emission computed tomography in Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders. Neuroimaging Clin N Am 5:103– 123, 1995 Obrist WD, Thompson HK, King CH, et al: Determination of regional cerebral blood flow by inhalation of 133-xenon. Circ Res 20:124–135, 1967 Pietrini P, Azari NP, Grady CL, et al: Pattern of cerebral metabolic interactions in a subject at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia 4:94–101, 1993 Pietrini P, Furey-Kurkjian ML, Graff-Radford N, et al: Regional cerebral glucose metabolism in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and prominent visual impairment. Am J Psychiatry 153:1261–1268, 1996 Pietrini P, Dani A, Furey ML, et al: PET-FDG stimulation reveals reduced brain metabolism in Down syndrome subjects at risk for Alzheimer disease prior to dementia. Am J Psychiatry 154:1063–1069, 1997 Pietrini P, Alexander GE, Furey ML, et al: Cerebral metabolic response to passive audiovisual stimulation in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and healthy volunteers assessed by PET. J Nucl Med 41:575–583, 2000 Rapoport SI: In vivo fatty acid incorporation into brain phospholipids in relation to plasma availability, signal transduction and membrane remodeling. J Mol Neurosci 16:243–261, 2001 Reiman EM, Caselli RJ, Yun LS, et al: Preclinical evidence of Alzheimer’s disease in persons homozygous for the A4 allele for apolipoprotein E. N Engl J Med 334:752–758, 1996 Reiman EM, Caselli RJ, Alexander GE, et al: Tracking the decline in cerebral glucose metabolism in persons and laboratory animals at genetic risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Clinical Neuroscience Research 1:194–206, 2001a Reiman EM, Caselli RJ, Chen K, et al: Declining brain activity in cognitively normal apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 heterozygotes: a foundation for using positron emission tomography to efficiently test treatments to prevent Alzheimer’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:3334–3339, 2001b Rosenthal G, Gilman S, Koeppe RA, et al: Motor dysfunction in olivopontocerebellar atrophy is related to cerebral metabolic rate studied with positron emission tomography. Ann Neurol 24:414–419, 1988
136
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Sandson TA, O’Connor M, Sperling RA, et al: Noninvasive perfusion MRI in Alzheimer’s disease: a preliminary report. Neurology 47:1339–1342, 1996 Schapiro MB, Pietrini P, Grady CL, et al Reductions in parietal and temporal cerebral metabolic rates for glucose are not specific for Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 56:859–864, 1993 Sharp P, Gemmell H, Cherryman G, et al: Application of iodine-123-labeled isopropylamphetamine imaging to the study of dementia. J Nucl Med 27:769– 774, 1986 Signorini M, Paulesu E, Friston K, et al: Rapid assessment of regional cerebral metabolic abnormalities in single subjects with quantitative and nonquantitative [18F]FDG PET: a clinical validation of statistical parametric mapping. Neuroimage 9:63–80, 1999 Silverman DHS, Small GW, Chang CY, et al: Neuroimaging in evaluation of dementia: regional brain metabolism and long-term outcome. JAMA 286:2120– 2127, 2001 Small GW, Leiter F: Neuroimaging for diagnosis of dementia. J Clin Psychiatry 59:4–7, 1998 Shoghi-Jadid K, Small GW, Agdeppa ED, et al: Localization of neurofibrillary tanlges and beta-amyloid plaques in the brains of living patients with Alzheimer’s disease. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 10:24–35, 2002 Sokoloff L: Relationships among local functional activity, energy metabolism, and blood flow in the central nervous system. Fed Proc 40:2311–2316, 1981 Sokoloff L: The radioactive deoxyglucose method, theory, procedure and application for the measurement of local cerebral glucose utilization in the central nervous system, in Advances in Neurochemistry, Vol 4. Edited by Agranoff BW, Aprison MH. New York, Plenum, 1982, pp 1–82 Talbot PR, Lloyd JJ, Snowden JS, et al: A clinical role for 99mTc-HMPAO SPECT in the investigation of dementia? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 64:306–313, 1998 Teipel SJ, Hampel H, Pietrini P, et al: Region specific corpus callosum atrophy correlates with regional pattern of cortical glucose metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 56:467–473, 1999 Teipel SJ, Bayer W, Alexander GE, et al: Progression of corpus callosum atrophy in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 59:243–248, 2002 Van Heertum RL, Miller SH, Mosesson RE: SPECT brain imaging in neurologic disease. Radiol Clin North Am 31:881–907, 1993 Yao H, Sadoshima S, Kuwabara Y, et al: Cerebral blood flow and oxygen metabolism in patients with vascular dementia of the Binswanger type. Stroke 21: 1694–1699, 1990
CHAPTER
5
Differential Diagnosis Anne M. Lipton, M.D., Ph.D. Myron F. Weiner, M.D.
In this chapter, we discuss the most common illnesses and conditions that produce severe cognitive impairment (the syndrome of mild cognitive impairment is discussed in Chapter 1). We do not include all possible etiologies of impaired brain function or all conditions that may worsen preexisting cognitive impairment or cause delirium. Severe cognitive impairment may have single or multiple causes that may be intrinsic to the central nervous system (CNS), related to systemic processes, or both. Some types of severe cognitive impairment are fully reversible; most are not. Nevertheless, many dementing illnesses are now treatable; timely and accurate diagnosis is therefore imperative. There is increasing evidence that the earlier treatment is started for Alzheimer’s disease, the better. Certainly, the ability to differentiate various dementing illnesses is enhanced by evaluating patients as early in the course of disease as possible. The presentations among various dementing illnesses are most distinctive as they begin. These distinctions blur as these illnesses progress toward global impairment. This chapter begins with a review of the neurodegenerative causes of cognitive impairment, including Alzheimer’s disease, and proceeds to a discussion of many other etiologies, all less common than Alzheimer’s disease. Probability is not destiny, however, and the differential diagnosis for each patient is dictated by individual history and circumstance and should be formulated correspondingly. Alzheimer’s Disease It had been assumed throughout history that aging itself is the cause of cognitive decline in elderly persons. According to Folsom (1886), “Senile dementia is simply an excess of the natural mental weakness of old age” (p. 174). That assumption was reaffirmed as recently as 1958, when Wech137
138
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
sler reported that “nearly all studies...have shown that most human abilities decline progressively after...ages 18 and 25” (p. 135). What was not attributable to normal aging was assigned to cerebrovascular arteriosclerosis and associated small strokes (Alvarez 1966). The former assumption was first challenged when Corsellis (1962) found a strong relationship between the severity of cerebral degenerative change and clinical diagnosis of patients who died in a psychiatric hospital. Of patients diagnosed as having organic mental disorder, 75% had moderate or severe cerebral degeneration, whereas only 25% of patients diagnosed with functional disorders showed comparable change in brain. Tomlinson et al. (1970) found soon afterward that half the brains of clinically demented elders bore the microscopic stigmata (neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles) of a brain disease first described in a 51-year-old woman by alienist-pathologist Alois Alzheimer more than 60 years earlier (Alzheimer 1907). Alzheimer’s disease is the most prevalent of the dementing illnesses of adult life. Based on data from the Framingham study, the prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease in persons ages 65–69 years is approximately 1%; the prevalence in those ages 80–84 is approximately 10% (Bachman et al. 1992). Risk factors include advancing age, having an affected first-degree relative, Down syndrome (trisomy 21), severe head injury, and inheritance of the e4 allele of apolipoprotein E (Morrison-Bogorad et al. 1997; see also Chapter 14). Familial Alzheimer’s disease is uncommon, accounting for less than 10% of cases. The pathology of Alzheimer’s disease is similar, whether it is familial or sporadic, and whether of early or late onset. On microscopic examination of the neocortex, and especially in the medial temporal lobes, there are abundant neuritic plaques (Figure 5–1) composed of amyloid b peptide and neurofibrillary tangles (Figure 5–2) composed of hyperphosphorylated tau protein. The pathophysiological process, which is detailed in Chapter 14, appears to be related to the abnormal processing of amyloid precursor protein and the resultant cascade of events. Alzheimer’s disease is characterized by the gradual erosion of intellectual function. The DSM-IV-TR diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease (dementia of the Alzheimer’s type) has four basic requisites: 1) presence of dementia; 2) gradual onset with continuing cognitive decline; 3) exclusion of all other specific causes of dementia by history, physical examination, and laboratory tests; and 4) not occurring exclusively during the course of a delirium (American Psychiatric Association 2000). Alzheimer’s disease is defined by the presence of amnesia (memory disorder) plus one or more of the following: agnosia (perceptual disorder), aphasia (language disorder), apraxia (disorder of voluntary movements), and abstraction/executive functioning. Criteria for probable Alzheimer’s disease include the establishment of dementia by clinical examination and documented by Mini-Mental State
Differential Diagnosis
FIGURE 5–1. Neuritic plaques. Source.
Courtesy of Dr. Charles White.
FIGURE 5–2. Neurofibrillary tangles. Source.
Courtesy of Dr. Charles White.
139
140
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Exam, Blessed Dementia Rating Scale, or similar examination and confirmed by neuropsychological testing (McKhann et al. 1984). Criteria require deficits in two or more areas of cognition, progressive worsening of memory and other cognitive functions; no disturbance of consciousness; onset between ages 40 and 90; and no systemic disorders that could in and of themselves account for the progressive deficits in memory and cognition. The clinical diagnosis of possible Alzheimer’s disease can be made on the basis of dementia in the absence of other disorders sufficient to cause dementia and when there are variations in onset or clinical course. The diagnosis of possible Alzheimer’s disease may also be made in the presence of a second systemic or brain disorder that could be sufficient to produce dementia but which is not considered to be the cause of the dementia. In a large autopsy series, the diagnosis of probable Alzheimer’s disease was confirmed in 92% of the subjects. Possible Alzheimer’s disease was pathologically diagnosed as Alzheimer’s disease in 87% of the subjects (Gearing et al. 1995). Alzheimer’s disease manifests most often in the seventh and eighth decades of life. Although DSM-IV-TR divides Alzheimer’s disease into earlyonset and late-onset forms, there is little evidence of important differences in the disease based on this distinction. The clinical presentation and course of Alzheimer’s disease vary for each individual, but problems with memory, reasoning, and judgment usually herald the onset of the disease. Among the frequent early signs are memory lapses, such as forgetting where the house keys have been placed or where the car is parked in a shopping center, but memory deficits occur more and more frequently and become gradually more severe. Patients often lack insight into their illness and tend to deny or minimize any problems; therefore, they are usually brought to medical attention by their family and do not usually present independently for evaluation. Eventually, more disorientation and confusion are seen. The syndrome of sundowning occurs when the patient’s symptoms worsen in the evening. (The disease occasionally presents as delirium, occurring when the person enters an unfamiliar environment [e.g., on vacation] or is hospitalized for medical treatment of an unrelated condition, such as in the case of elective surgery.) Affected individuals have increasing difficulty with so-called executive functions, such as interpreting abstract concepts and performing complex tasks. Independent activities of daily living, including such functions as grocery shopping and managing finances, become impaired. Persons with Alzheimer’s disease often develop difficulty with visuospatial skills, increasing their tendency to become lost or impairing their ability to copy simple diagrams (constructional apraxia). Other apraxias (the inability to correctly perform voluntary movements) include dressing apraxias, such as difficulty tying shoelaces or neckties. Aberrant motor behavior, such as
Differential Diagnosis
141
pacing and wandering may also occur. Impairments of language (aphasia) may be manifested by difficulty in word finding and object naming (dysnomia/anomia), with resultant word substitutions (paraphasias). Patients develop more and more difficulty with expression and comprehension and may eventually lose the ability to read and write. Late in the course of disease, patients may echo others (echolalia), repeat themselves (palilalia), or even become mute. Perceptual difficulties known as agnosias may occur. Loss of facial recognition (prosopagnosia) is common later in the disease, with patients frequently being unable to identify loved ones or to recognize themselves in a mirror. Social judgment tends to be retained until late in the disease, but behavioral problems such as apathy may be seen early in the course of disease. Psychiatric symptoms are common in Alzheimer’s disease (Burns et al. 1990). Behavioral symptoms in early Alzheimer’s disease include passivity; agitation; and low level of interest, concentration, and energy (Rubin and Kinscherf 1989). Denial of memory deficit is common (Sevush and Leve 1993). Depressive symptoms are common, but full-blown major depression is rare (Weiner et al. 1994). Paranoid ideation is also common, and in one series it occurred in 31% of subjects (Rubin et al. 1988). The primary focus for these patients was on other people stealing objects from the home or on being the object of a foul plot. The patient may develop the delusion that the spouse has a double or that the spouse is being impersonated. Visual hallucinations may occur in as many as 15% of patients, and auditory hallucinations in 10% (Rubin et al. 1988). Hallucinations are no longer reported when language function disintegrates. Behavioral and psychiatric disturbances tend to increase as the disease progresses (J. K. Cooper et al. 1990; Swearer et al. 1988). Physical aggression is not rare (Deutsch et al. 1991). It may be that much of the agitated behavior of severely demented patients is in response to ideas and perceptions they can no longer communicate to others. If they live long enough, Alzheimer’s disease patients become unable to perform basic self-care: feeding, dressing, and hygiene. Incontinence of bowel and bladder occur, and the patients may become unable to walk. Death often results from pneumonia, urinary tract infection, or other intercurrent illnesses. The duration of illness is variable, ranging from 4 to 10 years or more, depending on the patients’ general health, the quality of nursing care available, and the vigor with which feeding and other life support measures are pursued in the later stages. Neurological abnormalities often present in the middle or late stages of the disease. Cortical release signs including snout, suck, palmomental, and grasp reflexes appear. Myoclonic jerks occur often, as do mild Parkinsonian features (Chen et al. 1991). Postural changes also occur, with turning en bloc a fairly frequent phenomenon, along with a lowering of the center of
142
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
gravity that results in a bent-knee, shuffling gait. Contractures occur when patients become bed bound. Most of the tests done when Alzheimer’s disease is suspected are to exclude other etiologies of dementing illness. Some tests may show abnormalities in Alzheimer’s disease (such as the presence of cerebral atrophy on brain magnetic resonance imaging [MRI]), but these are too nonspecific for diagnosis and should be cautiously interpreted in the context of the individual history and presentation. Except for very rare cases of early-onset familial Alzheimer’s disease, genetic testing is not diagnostically useful. Alzheimer’s disease is detected early by signs of temporal and parietal lobe dysfunction, the triad of recent memory impairment, construction dyspraxia, and dysnomia. The five cognitive domains of attention/concentration, memory, language, visuospatial skills, and executive functions should be assessed. Neurobehavioral and/or neuropsychological evaluation (see Chapter 8) may demonstrate the encoding memory deficits typical of Alzheimer’s disease, especially early on. Signs of brain atrophy on computed tomography (CT) or MRI scans may be present early in the course of the disease. Frequent findings on both CT and MRI are shrinkage of the temporal lobes with widening of the sylvian fissures and hydrocephalus ex vacuo. On MRI, the cortical ribbon is seen to narrow more than the white matter. Single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) regional cerebral blood flow studies show bilateral temporoparietal blood flow reductions. Positron emission tomography (PET) scans also show reductions in temporoparietal blood flow, but other cortical regions have been found to be involved. Cholinesterase inhibitors and vitamin E have been shown to slow the clinical progression of Alzheimer’s disease. Cholinesterase inhibitors may be helpful in slowing, maintaining, and (occasionally) transiently improving function and cognition. They also improve behavioral problems in some cases. Psychological and behavioral management of Alzheimer’s disease is discussed in Chapter 6. Medications used to treat Alzheimer’s disease are discussed in Chapter 7, and future directions are discussed in Chapters 14 and 15. Behavioral and emotional complications are managed using psychological measures and drugs described in Chapters 6 and 7. Palliative drugs for cognitive deficit are described in Chapter 7, experimental drugs are discussed in Chapter 15, and environmental measures are discussed in Chapter 13.
Dementia With Lewy Bodies The clinical literature uses the term dementia with Lewy bodies to encompass two separate but related disorders, the Lewy body variant of Alzheimer’s
Differential Diagnosis
143
disease (LBV) and dementia with Lewy bodies without Alzheimer pathology. Lewy bodies are intracytoplasmic intraneural inclusions, 5–25 m in diameter, that show on staining with hematoxylin and eosin in a dense hyaline eosinophilic core and a surrounding halo (see Figure 5–3). They are more easily detected with anti-ubiquitin or anti-a-synuclein stains (Weiner 1999). There are three basic types of dementia with Lewy bodies: dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease (Lewy bodies in neocortical and substantia nigra neurons and loss of pigmented cells in the substantia nigra), dementia associated with neocortical Lewy bodies without the development of full-blown Parkinson’s disease (with or without substantia nigra pathology) and Alzheimer’s disease accompanied by neocortical Lewy bodies. When Alzheimer’s disease is accompanied by diffusely scattered neocortical Lewy bodies, the prevalence of neurofibrillary tangles tends to be less than in “pure” Alzheimer’s disease. For the sake of clarity, these three entities should be termed the dementia of Parkinson’s disease, dementia with Lewy bodies, and Lewy body variant of Alzheimer’s disease, their common feature being neocortical Lewy bodies.
FIGURE 5–3. Two small neocortical neurons, each containing a single cytoplasmic Lewy body (arrows) immunostained with an antibody to ubiquitin (magnification 125´; hematoxylin counterstain). Source.
Courtesy of Dr. Charles White.
144
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
At a clinical level, the diagnosis of dementia with Parkinson’s disease is based on signs and symptoms of Parkinson’s disease antedating cognitive impairment by at least a year. The clinical diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies is based on marked fluctuation of cognitive function, persistent hallucinations, and mild parkinsonism (McKeith et al. 1996). Unfortunately, dementia with Lewy bodies and LBV are clinically indistiguishable, and neither can be differentiated with confidence from pure Alzheimer’s disease. McKeith et al. (2000) reported a diagnostic sensitivity and specificity of 83% and 95% for the diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies in a prospective study of 50 persons who came to postmortem study. Other groups using these criteria have had diagnostic accuracy as low as 50% (Hohl et al. 2000). In terms of prevalence, LBV is common, comprising nearly onethird of autopsy-diagnosed cases of Alzheimer’s disease (Hansen et al. 1990). Pure dementia with Lewy bodies is uncommon; we have seen only 11 such cases at autopsy over 14 years. Weiner et al. (1996) analyzed available data on the clinical features of 58 patients with Alzheimer’s disease and 24 patients with LBV who underwent postmortem examination. The proportion of men in the LBV group was significantly larger than in the Alzheimer’s disease group (2/3 vs. 1/3). There was a significantly increased prevalence of hallucinations, delusions, and depressed mood in those with LBV compared with Alzheimer’s disease patients, but there were no significant differences in educational attainment, family history of dementia, age at onset, duration of illness, cognitive impairment, overall severity of illness, or neuropsychological findings. LBV patients tended to experience more frequent extrapyramidal side effects of neuroleptic medications compared with Alzheimer’s disease patients, but there was little difference in the frequency of extrapyramidal side effects between Alzheimer’s disease and LBV patients who were not exposed to neuroleptics.
Frontotemporal Dementia Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) is also known as frontotemporal lobar degeneration (Neary et al. 1998) or Pick complex (Kertesz et al. 1994). The term is used herein in its broadest sense, referring to a number of neurodegenerative diseases that vary in clinical presentation and pathological findings. Although the clinical syndromes vary, they characteristically involve problems with language, behavior, and/or neurological signs such as parkinsonism. (The FTD clinical syndrome characterized by executive dysfunction and apathy and/or disinhibition is specifically referred to herein as the FTD clinical profile.)
Differential Diagnosis
145
The archetypal FTD is Pick’s disease, first clinically delineated by Arnold Pick (1892), who described language impairments and behavioral disturbances in the setting of focal atrophy. Alzheimer (1911) made the first histopathological description of Pick’s disease with argyrophilic inclusions (later called Pick bodies) and swollen, achromatic cells (later called Pick’s cells). The Lund-Manchester criteria (1994) delineated the clinical features of FTD; these criteria were later refined by a consensus panel that used the term frontotemporal lobar degeneration (Neary et al. 1998). Three common clinical presentations of FTD have been identified: the FTD clinical profile (Gustafson 1987; Neary et al. 1988), primary progressive aphasia (Mesulam 1982; Weintraub et al. 1990), and semantic dementia (Hodges et al. 1992; Snowden et al. 1989). All are characterized by an insidious onset and gradual progression of disease. The FTD clinical profile is a syndrome characterized by executive dysfunction, social and interpersonal conduct problems, and apathy and/or disinhibition. Progressive nonfluent aphasia involves expressive aphasia with word-finding difficulty, agrammatism, and phonemic paraphasias. Semantic dementia is a fluent dysphasia with impairment of semantic verbal memory (severe difficulty in naming and the comprehension of word meaning) and an associative agnosia (e.g., impairments in stating or demonstrating the function of an object such as a tool or utensil). The current clinical criteria for FTD lack precise definition and have not been extensively studied with regard to their sensitivity and specificity. One autopsy-based retrospective study found that most patients with FTD met clinical diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer’s disease (Varma et al. 1999). Another study reported that 18 of 21 patients with autopsyproven Pick’s disease were misdiagnosed clinically as having Alzheimer’s disease (Mendez et al. 1993). The current clinical criteria for FTD also fail to account for many neurogenetic and neuroimaging aspects of the diagnosis of FTD. New clinical consensus criteria for FTD have been published (McKhann et al. 2001). Neuroimaging with SPECT is widely used in the evaluation of patients with FTD, but there are no data concerning its sensitivity and specificity for FTD (Knopman et al. 2001). Tests such as the Mini-Mental State Exam (Folstein et al. 1975) are insensitive to the early and isolated executive and/or language deficits of patients with FTD (Gregory et al. 1997). Proposed alternatives include the Frontal Behavioral Inventory (Kertesz et al. 1997), the Frontal Assessment Battery (Dubois et al. 2000), and Adenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination (ACE) (Mathuranath 2000). The ACE has high reliability and sensitivity in relatively young subjects with FTD who had mainly primary progressive aphasia and language involvement. Its specificity, especially in the FTD clinical profile with disinhibition, is unknown. In addition, using higher cutoff values for scores yielded false-positive results in which Alzheimer’s
146
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
disease was classified as FTD. Moreover, the ACE is culturally specific to the United Kingdom, because it includes questions such as naming the leader of the opposition in the British Parliament. The Frontal Assessment Battery has been shown to distinguish healthy control subjects from patients with mild Parkinson’s disease, multiple system atrophy, corticobasal degeneration (CBD), and progressive supranuclear palsy. It involves testing similarities, verbal fluency, motor programming, and inhibitory control. It has not been tested in persons with Alzheimer’s disease. Tests used in this battery and others may be usefully incorporated into a clinical evaluation to assess frontal lobe function. For example, patients may be asked to perform the Luria maneuver, in which they imitate the examiner in making a “fist-cut-slap” series of hand gestures (making a fist, then placing the hand perpendicularly as in a handshake, and then turning the hand palm downwards). The examiner should perform the sequence of movements two or three times with the patient, then ask the patient to continue with the pattern. Patients with severe frontal lobe dysfunction will have difficulty even imitating the examiner on this motor program. Patients should be tested for frontal release signs (described in Chapter 3). Patients with FTD, particularly the FTD clinical profile, will often display echopraxis (imitating the examiner), perseveration, and motor impersistence. Historically, the FTD disorders have been divided into Pick’s disease and non-Pick lobar atrophy (Dickson 1998; Hulette and Crain 1992). The different subtypes of FTD share some neuropathological features. Both have grossly appreciable frontal and temporal atrophy. Microscopically, frontotemporal degeneration is characterized by upper cortical microvacuolation, variable caudate nucleus atrophy and nigral pallor, superficial cortical microvacuolation and gliosis in the frontal and/or temporal lobes, and variable ballooned neurons in the cortex and amygdala (Brun 1987, 1993). Pick bodies (round to oval argyrophilic, tau- and ubiquitin-positive neuronal cytoplasmic inclusions in the hippocampal dentate gyrus and neocortex) are seen only in Pick’s disease. Non-Pick lobar atrophy has recently been subclassified on the basis of the presence or absence of newly discovered immunohistochemically distinct inclusions (Bergeron et al. 1998; P.N. Cooper et al. 1995; Jackson and Lowe 1996). In the nonfamilial non-Pick lobar atrophies, there are at least three separate diagnostic categories. They are defined by the presence or absence of specific tau and ubiquitin inclusions in the hippocampal dentate gyrus and/ or superficial frontal and/or temporal cortex. Differences in tau and ubiquitin immunohistochemistry are important in classifying pathologic FTD subtypes. One subtype is motor neuron type dementia (frontotemporal degeneration with ubiquitinated inclusions, also called motor neuron inclusion dementia), which has ubiquitin-positive, tau-negative inclusions in the
Differential Diagnosis
147
hippocampal dentate nucleus and in neocortical neuronal layers II and III. CBD has tau-positive neuronal inclusions and glial plaques, along with ballooned neurons, in the cortex, basal ganglia, brainstem, and cerebellum. Frontotemporal degeneration with neuronal loss and spongiosis (also known as dementia lacking distinctive histopathology) (Knopman 1993) has no tau or ubiquitin inclusions. Despite the shared pathology in FTD there may be a variety of pathological findings within the same clinical FTD subtype (Munoz 1998). These include classic Pick’s disease with Pick bodies and Pick’s cells, frontal degeneration with microvacuolation and gliosis, ubiquitin-positive inclusions similar to those found in motor neuron disease (with or without anterior horn cell disease), familial cases with tau mutations with neuronal and glial tau inclusions, and corticobasal degeneration (see below). Familial multiple system tauopathy is one of the many cases of familial FTD and parkinsonism linked to chromosome 17 (FTDP-17). These families have a variety of clinical presentations, including disinhibition-dementia-parkinsonism-amyotrophy complex, and neuropathological findings. The incidence of dementia in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) is reported to be 5%, but a prospective study is lacking (Stron and Grace 1998). The predominant form of dementia in ALS is FTD. Linkage to chromosome 9q21-q22 for familial ALS patients with FTD has been reported (Hosler et al. 2000). No treatment has been shown to alter the course of FTD, but selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors are useful in treating many of the behavioral symptoms. Other symptomatic treatments that have been tried are dopaminergic therapies for parkinsonism and language problems.
Corticobasal Degeneration Pick’s disease and CBD (also called corticobasal ganglionic degeneration) have classically been regarded as clinically distinct entities (Lang et al. 1994; Watts et al. 1994). CBD is one of the Parkinson’s-plus syndromes that tend to progress more rapidly than Parkinson’s disease and are usually less amenable to treatment. The authors of the first description of corticodentatonigral degeneration (later renamed CBD) did, however, recognize the resemblance of the pathology to Pick’s disease (Rebiez et al. 1968). CBD is usually reported to be clinically characterized by unilateral rigidity, apraxia, the alien hand syndrome, reflex myoclonus, and cortical sensory loss (Riley et al. 1990). Retrospective neuropathological studies have shown that cases of clinically diagnosed Pick’s disease or FTD may have pathologic findings of
148
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
CBD and vice versa (Boeve et al. 1999; Kertesz et al. 2001; Schneider et al. 1997). Movement disorders specialists may overlook the dementia, whereas neurobehavioral or psychiatric specialists may underplay problems such as parkinsonism. Prospective study of clinically diagnosed FTD and CBD with neuroimaging and pathologic examination is crucial in refining the clinical diagnosis of these diseases, in raising awareness of their overlap, and in refining treatment. Whether FTD, Pick’s disease, and CBD are distinct entities, overlapping entities, or the same entity remains controversial (Neary 1997).
Progressive Supranuclear Palsy Progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) is another Parkinson’s-plus syndrome that has clinical and pathologic overlap with FTD. Both may be pathologically classified as tauopathies, and the dementia of PSP may be clinically classified as FTD. This disorder begins in late life and is characterized by balance difficulty, falls, visual disturbances, slurred speech, dysphagia, and personality change (Richardson et al. 1963). Dementia tends not to be pronounced. A characteristic triad of ophthalmoplegia, pseudobulbar palsy, and axial dystonia develops. First, downward gaze is impaired, then upward gaze, then voluntary gaze in all directions. If the eyes are fixed on a target and the head is turned, full eye movement occurs, indicating that the motor nerves are intact. The etiology of this disease is unknown. Pathological findings include loss of neurons; gliosis; and the presence of neurofibrillary tangles in the surviving neurons in the midbrain, cerebellar peduncles, and subthalamic nucleus. Impairment proceeds to anarthria and total immobility.
Parkinson’s Disease Parkinson’s disease is characterized by bradykinesia, resting tremor, rigidity, and postural instability. Facial expression generally decreases, rate of blinking diminishes, and arm swing diminishes during walking. The clinical syndrome is slowly progressive and appears to result from loss of pigmented neurons in the pars compacta of the substantia nigra. Loss of these cells reduces dopaminergic input to the limbic system, forebrain, and basal ganglia. Cells are lost in other pigmented nuclei as well, including the locus coeruleus and the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus nerve. Lewy bodies are seen in surviving cells of the pigmented nuclei. There is considerable pathological overlap between Parkinson’s disease and Alzheimer’s disease, with Joachim et al. (1988) reporting that 18% of
Differential Diagnosis
149
their Alzheimer’s disease patients had sufficient neuronal loss and Lewy bodies in the substantia nigra to warrant a diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease. Dementia occurs frequently in Parkinson’s disease, with estimates ranging from 18% to 60% (Cummings 1985; Tison et al. 1995). Laboratory and neuroimaging studies are not helpful in the diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease. Treatment may include levodopa, dopamine agonists, catechol-O-methyltransferase inhibitors, and anticholinergic agents. Anticholinergic agents should be avoided in Parkinson’s disease patients with dementia because these agents are likely to increase confusion. Cholinesterase inhibitors may be helpful in treating the dementia in some cases, but caution should be used because they may exacerbate motor symptoms.
Vascular Dementia Vascular dementia is the second most common dementia in elders in the United States, and it may complicate Alzheimer’s disease. Vascular dementia may result from multiple infarcts, strategically placed single infarcts, small-vessel disease, hypoperfusion, and brain hemorrhage. Dementia as a result of brain tissue infarction occurs in relation to the volume of tissue infarcted and to the location of the infarcts. Loss of approximately 100 mL of brain tissue produces severe cognitive impairment regardless of location (Tomlinson et al. 1970). Localized infarcts in the hippocampus, mammillary bodies, thalamus, or basal forebrain produce severe memory deficits. If small-vessel infarcts primarily involve white matter, the term Binswanger’s disease is often used (Román 1987). When brain tissue is reabsorbed following infarction, cavities develop. Figure 5–4 shows an MRI study of a man with a history of strokes, mild cognitive impairment, and sensory loss in both lower extremities. Multiple lacunae are present in the basal ganglia. The following criteria have been proposed for the diagnosis of vascular dementia (Román et al. 1993): 1. Dementia, defined by cognitive decline from a previously higher level of functioning with impairment of memory and two or more additional cognitive domains; established by clinical examination and confirmed by neuropsychological testing; severe enough to interfere with activities of daily living and not due to the physical effects of stroke alone. 2. Cerebrovascular disease, defined by focal neurological signs (with or without history of stroke) and evidence of relevant cerebrovascular disease by CT or MRI including multiple large-vessel infarcts, a single strategically placed infarct (angular gyrus, thalamus, basal forebrain,
150
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
FIGURE 5–4. T1-weighted magnetic resonance image showing multiple basal ganglia lacunae, marked gyral atrophy, and hydrocephalus ex vacuo. Source.
Courtesy of Dr. Myron Weiner.
posterior cerebral artery, or anterior cerebral artery territories), and multiple basal ganglia and white matter lacunae or extensive periventricular white matter lesions. 3. A relationship between dementia and cerebrovascular disease as suggested by onset of dementia within 3 months after a recognized stroke; abrupt deterioration in cognitive function or fluctuating stepwise progression of cognitive defects.
Differential Diagnosis
151
Vascular dementia may exhibit predominantly cortical (aphasia, agnosia, apraxia, amnesia) or subcortical (slowness, depression, apathy) signs but often presents with both. There may be pseudobulbar palsy and dysarthria. Vascular dementia is associated with hypertension, diabetes, and other evidence of cardiovascular disease, such as a history of myocardial infarction. There is often a history of transient ischemic attacks or frank stroke. Vascular dementia may progress in a stepwise fashion, with acute episodes of confusion accompanied by localizing or lateralizing neurological signs. Mental status examination may not distinguish vascular dementia from Alzheimer’s disease, but depression and emotional lability are more characteristic of vascular dementia. There may be focal or generalized electroencephalographic slowing. When there are multiple infarcts, CT shows areas of lucency, MRI shows areas of decreased signal on T1-weighted images (Figure 5–4), and SPECT indicates multiple areas of low flow that are wedge shaped when infarcts involve the cortex. Treatment of vascular dementia consists of prophylaxis, including control of hypertension and diabetes, and the use of aspirin and other drugs to reduce intravascular clotting (see Chapter 7). Anticoagulants are employed only when a definite cardiac embolic source has been identified or when repeated transient ischemic attacks occur in an individual with largely preserved function. Signs and symptoms of depression should be actively sought (see section on stroke in this chapter, and also Chapter 1). In questionable cases, a trial of methylphenidate or low-dose antidepressant may be indicated, as suggested in Chapter 7.
Mixed Dementia There are many cases in which the clinical picture has elements of both Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. There are cases in which the progress of the disease has been smooth, but unilateral hyperreflexia, dysarthria, unilateral facial palsy, or a unilateral Babinski’s sign is present along with CT or MRI evidence of an old infarct. In other cases, a smooth downward course is punctuated by one or more stepwise declines. Such cases—in which both diseases seem to be present but neither disease fully explains the clinical picture—are designated as mixed dementia.
Reversible Dementias The etiologies of potentially reversible dementias are listed in Table 5–1. Of the etiologies listed, the most often reversible are depression, drugs, and metabolic factors. It is important to be aware that, in many instances,
152
TABLE 5–1.
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Etiologies of potentially reversible dementia
Psychiatric Depression Schizophrenia Ganser’s syndrome Malingering Toxic Drugs (prescription or street) Alcohol Chemical poisoning (arsenic, mercury, lead, lithium and other metals; organic compounds and solvents) Metabolic Azotemia/renal failure (diuretics, dehydration, obstruction, hypokalemia) Hyponatremia (diuretics, excess antidiuretic hormone, salt wasting, water intoxication) Volume depletion Hypoglycemia or hyperglycemia Hepatic encephalopathy Hypothyroidism or hyperthyroidism Hyperparathyroidism Cushing’s syndrome Wilson’s disease Acute intermittent porphyria Infection and/or fever (in elders, pneumonia, urinary tract infection) Anoxic Anemia Congestive heart failure Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease Vitamin deficiencies (B12, folic acid, thiamine, niacin) Central nervous system disorders Vascular (ischemic or hemorrhagic stroke, ischemic-hypoxic brain lesions) Trauma (subdural hematoma, postconcussion syndrome) Human immunodeficiency virus and opportunistic infections Other infections (neurosyphilis, chronic meningitis, brain abscess, progressive multifocal leukoencephalopathy) Neoplasm (primary or metastatic) Cerebral vasculitis Normal-pressure hydrocephalus Multiple sclerosis
Differential Diagnosis
153
reversible does not mean fully reversible. The effects of a stroke may be largely overcome, but there may also be residual cognitive impairment. Often, a reversible cause of dementia complicates a progressive dementing illness. For example, a person with Alzheimer’s disease who lives alone may not maintain nutrition or water intake during a febrile illness, aggravating the preexisting cognitive impairment. With correction of the nutritional or metabolic abnormalities, cognition improves but does not return to normal.
Psychiatric The primary psychiatric cause of the dementia syndrome seen in dementia clinics is major depression, which is discussed at length in Chapter 1. Although depression is reversible with antidepressant medication or electroconvulsive therapy, depressed patients with significant cognitive impairment deserve concomitant evaluation for other causes of cognitive impairment. Dementia may appear in long-standing schizophrenia (Granholm and Jeste 1994), hence Kraepelin’s (1913) term dementia praecox. The dementia of community-dwelling persons with chronic schizophrenia is nonprogressive (Heaton et al. 2001) and seems to be related to difficulty with concentration and organization of thought and to inability to avoid internal distractions that manifest as delusions and hallucinations. Perceptions are distorted, and information processing becomes disorganized. Communication may be characterized by echolalia, palilalia, and neologistic word salad and may be difficult to distinguish from fluent aphasia. Occasionally, patients are reduced to a state of near vegetation and mutism. Some patients with bipolar disorder will show cognitive deterioration. Poorer cognitive performance is associated with the number of episodes of mania or depression (Zubieta et al. 2001). When stable patients with bipolar disorder receiving care at community and rehabilitation programs were compared with schizophrenic patients, their cognitive and social deficits were equivalent in severity (Dickerson et al. 2001). It seems likely that the cognitive deterioration in these illnesses is a combination of yet-unknown psychological and metabolic/anatomic factors. In a series of 100 persons with schizophrenia (ages at death, 52–101 years), there was no identifiable neuropathology (Purohit et al. 1998). In those with cognitive impairment, there was a negative correlation with choline acetyltransferase activity and evidence of cortical noradrenergic and serotoninergic deficits (Powchik et al. 1998).
Toxic The classes of cognition-impairing drugs are listed in Table 5–2. Drugs of abuse are the most important toxins in young adults; prescription drugs are
154
TABLE 5–2.
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Cognition-impairing drugs
Psychotropic
Nonpsychotropic
Sedative-hypnotics Benzodiazepines Flurazepam Diazepam Chlordiazepoxide Barbiturates Meprobamate Antidepressants Imipramine Amitriptyline Doxepin Trazodone Serzone Antihistamines H2 antagonists Cimetidine Ranitidine Anti-inflammatory agents Corticosteroids Nonsteroidals
Diuretics Cardiac glycosides Antiarrhythmics Calcium channel blockers Antimanics Lithium Carbamazepine Valproate Antipsychotics Thioridazine Chlorpromazine Clozapine Antihypertensives Rauwolfia alkaloids >-Blockers =-Methyldopa Analgesics Narcotic analgesics Propoxyphene Anticonvulsants Diphenylhydantoin Topiramate
the most important in elders (Wilcox et al. 1994). In young adults, drug ingestion leads to delirium or acute psychosis rather than a dementia syndrome. In elders, dementia syndromes arise from the use of long-acting benzodiazepines, barbiturates, meprobamate (Equanil), and a host of others depending on their dose and the length of time they have been used. Drugs such as flurazepam (Dalmane), with a half-life of more than 120 hours, accumulate rapidly. Others accumulate more slowly or require relatively high doses for toxicity to develop.
Alcohol Victor (1994) maintains that there are only four distinct brain diseases associated with chronic alcoholism: Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome, Marchiafava-Bignami syndrome, pellagrinous encephalopathy, and acquired hepatocerebral degeneration. Victor holds that the role of alcohol in each is secondary. Nevertheless, there is evidence of alcohol toxicity in animal developmental models and in neuronal cell culture (Luo et al. 1999). Fur-
Differential Diagnosis
155
thermore, Brun and Andersson (2001) found loss of synapses in the superior laminae of Brodmann’s area 10 in frontal cortex of 18 heavy drinkers who were compared with a control group with regard to liver disease and preexisting mental disorder. They suggested that this type of change might be responsible for the personality deterioration of persons with alcoholism. We have observed seven elderly persons referred to our clinic for evaluation of memory impairment who had a history of long-standing alcoholism or alcohol intake equivalent to four or more ounces of alcohol per day. All had good nutrition. Their cognitive impairment was mild but definite and primarily affected short-term memory and abstract reasoning. Followup of these patients over several years revealed progression to Alzheimer’s disease in all but one case.
Chemical Poisoning Chemical poisons that impair CNS function usually have their primary effects on other systems: gastrointestinal, renal, hepatic, hemapoietic, and peripheral nervous systems.
Organic Compounds, Solvents, and Inhalants The list of potential toxins is long and includes carbon disulfide, halogenated hydrocarbons such as carbon tetrachloride, naphthalene, benzene and its derivatives, and many others. Neural and cognitive symptoms tend to occur preterminally. Toluene (methylbenzene), one of the toxins in glue and paint sniffing, causes extensive white matter damage (Filley et al. 1990) and profound cognitive impairment in addition to tremor, ataxia, and loss of vision and hearing (Fornazzari et al. 1983). Commonly used inhalants, in addition to those listed above, include Freon, butane lighter fluid, and nitrous oxide (McGarvey et al. 1999)
Metabolic Disorders Azotemia Uremic encephalopathy may develop slowly or acutely. If it develops slowly, it usually produces apathy, impaired concentration, and irritability. The mechanism of uremic encephalopathy is presumed to be multiple metabolic derangements, including the accumulation of toxic metabolites (Moe and Sprague 1994). If uremia is acute, there is altered sensorium. Prerenal azotemia may be caused by diuretics, dehydration, and hypokalemia; postrenal azotemia may be caused by urinary tract obstruction.
156
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Hyponatremia Hyponatremia may cause cognitive impairment (Pentimone and Del Corso 1992). Serum sodium levels below 120 mEq/L are accompanied by impaired attention, drowsiness, and stupor. Hyponatremia may be due to diuretics, inappropriate antidiuretic hormone (ADH) secretion, salt-wasting renal disease, and water intoxication. Diuretic abuse is common in young women preoccupied with losing weight, and hyponatremia occurs frequently in elders treated with diuretics for hypertension (Vieweg et al. 1994). Polyuria due to decreased renal reabsorption of water is common with lithium treatment (Siegel et al. 1998). Inappropriate ADH secretion may occur with various brain lesions, lung tumors, and use of chlorpromazine and carbamazepine (Vishwanath et al. 1991); it is commonly reported with use of serotonin reuptake inhibitors (Kirby and Ames 2001). Saltwasting nephropathy may lead to hyponatremia. Water intoxication has been reported in subjects diagnosed with schizophrenia, mental retardation, depression, and alcoholism (Jose et al. 1979).
Volume Depletion Volume depletion may occur as a result of diuretic administration, diarrhea, vomiting, bleeding, or inadequate fluid intake. Electrolyte abnormalities may accompany the use of diuretics or may be associated with vomiting and diarrhea.
Hypoglycemia and Hyperglycemia Hypoglycemia primarily appears as delirium (Fishbain and Rotundo 1988) but may manifest as sudden worsening of cognitive impairment, especially in elders. Oral hypoglycemic agents are the most common cause of hypoglycemia in patients over age 60 years (Seltzer 1972). The oral hypoglycemic agent chlorpropamide is contraindicated in elders because of its 35-hour half-life. Other drugs may potentiate the action of sulfonylureas, including aspirin, dicumarol, lithium, and monoamine oxidase inhibitors. Other conditions leading to hypoglycemia in elders include alcoholism, sepsis, malnutrition, and myxedema. With rapid decrease in blood glucose, symptoms may appear at blood sugar levels greater than 45 mg/dL. Hyperglycemia may cause impaired mentation in uncontrolled diabetic patients.
Hepatic Encephalopathy Hepatic encephalopathy may occur in both acute and chronic liver failure. About 30% of persons with cirrhosis die in hepatic coma (Abou-Assi and Vlancevic 2001). Liver failure is usually accompanied by obvious clouding
Differential Diagnosis
157
of sensorium, but intermittent mental dulling may be the primary symptom. Subclinical hepatic encephalopathy (e.g., significant impairment on neuropsychological testing) is present in as many as 60% of individuals with cirrhosis, and these individuals are prone to develop full-blown hepatic encephalopathy (Das et al. 2001). Diagnosis of hepatic encephalopathy is by demonstration of arterial ammonia concentration greater than 100 mg/dL (A.J.L. Cooper and Plum 1987). Borderline low serum albumin and increased prothrombin time and asterixis should raise suspicion of liver failure and hepatic encephalopathy. Recommendations for the treatment of both acute and chronic hepatic encephalopathy have been published by the American College of Gastroenterology (Blei and Cordoba 2001).
Thyroid Disease Although hypothyroidism is associated with lethargy, slowed thinking, slowed speech, and impaired recent memory (Haupt and Kurz 1993), there is no strong evidence that hypothyroidism produces a dementia syndrome or that the neurocognitive deficits associated with hypothyroidism remit fully with treatment (Dugbartey 1998). We have seen individuals with chemical hypothyroidism, but we have not seen a single case of hypothyroid dementia among more than 3,000 persons evaluated for cognitive symptoms over the past 14 years. Indeed, the few reports in the literature suggest a stronger relationship between hypothyroidism and symptoms of depression than with dementia. Hyperthyroidism is associated with impaired attention and concentration and diminished short-term memory. The usual symptoms of tachycardia, increased sweating, and fine tremor may not be apparent in elders (Meneilly et al. 1988), who may present with apathy.
Hyperparathyroidism and Hypercalcemia Hyperparathyroidism is most often due to parathyroid adenoma. Demineralization of bone and the presence of kidney stones are the most common clinical indicators. Blood calcium is increased, as is parathyroid hormone level. The predominant mental symptoms are depression and lassitude, but both delirium and dementia syndromes may occur (Alarcon and Franceschini 1984; Molaschi et al. 1994). Hypercalcemia itself can cause mental dulling. In one case, a woman with multiple myeloma had been given calcium supplementation to minimize pathological fractures that were occurring in the course of her illness. Over a period of months, she became increasingly dull and lethargic. When she was found to have an elevated serum calcium concentration, her calcium supplementation was discontinued, and her mentation improved considerably.
158
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Although hypoparathyroidism with its accompanying neuromuscular irritability and paresthesias has been suggested as a cause of dementia (Sier et al. 1984), there is little to support this contention.
Hypercortisolemia/Cushing’s Syndrome Chronic elevation of cortisol concentration is associated with hippocampal atrophy and subsequent deficits in declarative, episodic, spatial, and contextual memory performance (McEwen 1997). Cushing’s syndrome is caused by a chronic excess of circulating cortisol, whether endogenous or exogenous. Its clinical features include facial and truncal obesity, flushed facies, purplish abdominal striae, hypertension, diabetes, and osteoporosis. Depression and delirium are the most common associated mental states; neuropsychological testing shows impairment in nonverbal, visual ideational, visual memory, and spatial-constructional abilities more frequently than impairment in language or verbal reasoning (Whelan et al. 1980).
Wilson’s Disease This recessively inherited disorder of copper metabolism has symptomatic onset in the second or third decade of life; its first manifestations are usually related to impaired liver function, including jaundice and hepatosplenomegaly. Neurological symptoms begin with head or limb tremor, slowed movement, dysarthria, dysphagia, and choreic movements or dystonia (Dening 1991). As many as 20% of Wilson’s disease patients develop prominent emotional symptoms before the diagnosis is made, but cognitive impairment is not a prominent feature early in the disease (Dening and Berrios 1989). As the disease progresses, patients drool and become dysphagic; have rigid, slow-moving limbs; develop a fixed, vacuous smile; and develop a wing-beating tremor when their limbs are outstretched. The disease is diagnosed by the findings of low serum ceruloplasmin (<20 mg/dL) or high urinary copper and is confirmed by Kayser-Fleischer rings on slit lamp examination. Liver biopsy shows high copper content. The pathogenesis of the disease seems to be related to the toxic effects of excessive copper deposition in the liver and the lentiform nuclei. Treatment is reduction of dietary copper intake and administration of the copper chelating agent d-penicillamine.
Acute Intermittent Porphyria (Pyrroloporphyria, Swedish Type) Inherited as an autosomal dominant, this type of porphyria is not associated with photosensitivity. It is caused by a metabolic defect in the liver that
Differential Diagnosis
159
causes increased production and urinary excretion of porphobilinogen and of the porphyrin precursor d-aminolevulinic acid. The definitive test is measurement of monopyrrole porphobilinogen deaminase in red blood cells (Burgovne et al. 1995). The physical signs and symptoms of porphyria are bouts of abdominal pain, peripheral neuropathy, altered mental state (usually delirium), and intermittently wine-colored urine (Massey 1980). Attacks are frequently induced by barbiturates, sulfonamides, estrogen, phenytoin, griseofulvin, and succinimide anticonvulsants. The diagnosis is confirmed by finding large quantities of porphobilinogen and d-aminolevulinic acid in urine. The urine of such patients turns dark on standing due to the oxidation of porphobilinogen to porphobilin.
Acute Infection and/or Fever Severe or systemic infections and fever often cause delirium. In frail elders, febrile response may not be great, and the first sign of a severe infection may be cognitive impairment. This is most often true in elders with borderline cognitive reserve or those who are already demented. The most common infections are pneumonia, urinary tract infection, and generalized sepsis. Thus, the evaluation of a sudden change in a frail elder’s cognitive status should include a chest X ray, urinalysis, complete blood count, and (when indicated) blood culture. The same holds true in immunocompromised persons of all ages. In this group, which includes persons with acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS), lymphoma, or lymphocytic leukemia or those being treated with immunosuppressants, additional studies should include cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) examination and a CT scan of the head (with contrast) as part of a search for opportunistic infections.
Anoxia Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD) produces hypoxemia, hypercarbia, and acidosis. Patients with COPD are well known to experience the form of delirium known as sundowning (i.e., worsening of sensorium and orientation in the evenings). Measures of immediate and delayed memory, complex attention, and speed of information processing correlate highly with arterial CO2 partial pressure in these individuals (Stuss et al. 1997). Generally speaking, however, persons with mild COPD perform within the normal range on neuropsychological measures (Kozora et al. 1999). The cognitive impairment produced by COPD is commonly superimposed on the effects of alcohol, Alzheimer’s disease, or vascular dementia (Incalzi et al. 1993).
160
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Vitamin Deficiencies Thiamine (Vitamin B1) Wernicke’s syndrome (polioencephalitis hemorrhagica superior) is characterized by sensorial impairment, apathy, and inattentiveness accompanied by nystagmus, abducens and conjugate-gaze palsies, and ataxic gait. MRI shows increased T2 signal in the paraventricular regions of the thalamus and periaqueductal region of the midbrain (Antunez et al. 1998). The symptoms are usually of abrupt onset and may occur singly or in various combinations. Although it is most commonly associated with the nutritional deficiency of chronic alcoholism, Wernicke’s syndrome has been reported with hemodialysis (Ihara et al. 1999) and gastric surgery for morbid obesity (Toth and Voll 2001). Prompt treatment with parenteral thiamine is indicated, with 100 mg/day given in divided doses until patients are able to eat, at which time they can take 100 mg/day orally. There is usually prompt remission of the ophthalmoplegia and ataxia, but many patients will continue to manifest the deficits in memory and learning that are described as Korsakoff’s syndrome (Victor 1993). Korsakoff’s syndrome (alcohol-persisting amnestic disorder) develops independently of Wernicke’s syndrome in 75% of cases (Blansjaar et al. 1992), but Victor et al. (1971) found that 84% of Wernicke’s syndrome survivors went on to manifest an amnestic syndrome. In this syndrome, immediate memory (as tested by digit span) is fairly well preserved, but shortterm memory is devastated, and long-term memory is markedly impaired. A frequently reported aspect of Korsakoff’s syndrome is confabulation— the tendency to invent memories. Confabulation is generally elicited by asking patients what they did and who they saw yesterday and by comparing the patient’s account with that of a reliable witness. Psychological functions other than memory are affected in Korsakoff’s syndrome. Executive cognitive functions, including abstract reasoning and the ability to change cognitive set, are also impaired. With abstinence, some patients with Korsakoff’s syndrome improve cognitively, with the recovery process taking as long as a year (Victor 1993). In both Wernicke’s and Korsakoff’s syndromes, there are lesions in the medial thalamic region, tegmentum of the pons, and cerebellar cortex. Anterograde memory deficits may be due to damage in the circuit involving the mammillary bodies, mammillothalamic tract, and anterior thalamus, whereas the retrograde amnesia may involve frontal circuits (Kopelman 1995). MRI and SPECT studies are similar in both Wernicke’s and Korsakoff’s syndromes, with atrophy of mammillary bodies and thalamus seen on MRI and reduction of blood flow to the anterior temporal regions observed using SPECT (Jauhar and Montaldi 2000). Wernicke-Korsakoff pathology is found in 10%–15% of autopsied alco-
Differential Diagnosis
161
holic persons; this suggests that thiamine should be prescribed routinely for alcoholic individuals (Victor 1993). Intravenous glucose solution administered to a malnourished patient may precipitate a thiamine deficiency due to depleted reserves. Furthermore, administering thiamine may produce hypoglycemia in nutritionally deprived persons by stimulating the metabolism of glucose.
Cyanocobalamin (Vitamin B12) Cyanocobalamin (vitamin B12) and folic acid are required in the methylation of homocysteine to methionine and in the synthesis of S-adenosylmethionine (SAM), which in turn is an important methyl donor in biological reactions (Bottiglieri 1996). Vitamin B12 deficiency affects the peripheral nerves, spinal cord, and brain. A megaloblastic anemia, it may be due to dietary lack or failure of absorption due to lack of intrinsic factor. The spinal cord lesions are termed subacute combined degeneration and affect the posterior and lateral columns. Symptoms usually begin with distal, symmetrical paresthesias in all limbs, followed by unsteadiness, stiffness, and weakness of the limbs. Vitamin B12 deficiency may produce depression, psychotic states, and cognitive impairment. Because patients often receive folic acid in over-the-counter multivitamin preparations, the hematological findings characteristic of pernicious anemia may not be present and may be absent even without folate supplementation. Treatment involves a brief course of intramuscular injections of 1,000 mg of cyanocobalamin daily, followed by injections weekly and then monthly for the remainder of the patient’s life. Hector and Burton (1988) argue that their extensive literature review revealed little evidence that vitamin B12 deficiency was associated with a full-blown dementia syndrome or that reversing vitamin B12 deficiency causes improvement in cognitive state. Unfortunately, those authors’ criterion for dementia was a formal diagnosis of dementia rather than signs and symptoms of impaired cognition. Evidence of cognitive impairment was impressive in the studies they cited, but there was little evidence of improvement following vitamin B12 therapy. Of 1,457 cases of hypovitaminosis B12, only one patient showed cognitive improvement after replacement therapy. In a study of 125 persons with low vitamin B12 levels that were discovered during assessment at a memory disorders clinic, 66 with dementia and 22 with cognitive impairment were reassessed after treatment with vitamin B12 (Eastley et al. 2000). Most had normal blood indices. Treatment had no effect on those with dementia, but the patients with milder cognitive impairment showed increased verbal fluency. These data are difficult to interpret, but from a practical standpoint, a trial of weekly and then
162
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
monthly parenteral B12 seems justified in persons with low serum concentrations of vitamin B12. There is little expense, no risk, and the possibility of gain.
Folic Acid In a review of the psychiatric literature, Hutto (1997) found folate deficiency to be related to depression but not to reversible dementia. A community study of elders showed folate deficiency to be a risk factor for stroke and noted that persons with folate deficiency were more likely to be demented, institutionalized, or depressed (Ebly et al. 1998). There was no difference in cognitive scores in persons with low folate levels and dementia, but among the mildly cognitively impaired, lower folate levels were associated with lower cognitive scores. Low folate levels were common in all types of dementia and were associated with weight loss, lower body mass index, and lower serum albumin concentrations. Appropriate treatment for folate deficiency and related elevation of serum homocysteine is large oral doses (1–5 mg/day) of folic acid (van Guldener and Stehouwer 2001).
Nicotinic Acid The pellagrinous triad of dermatitis, diarrhea, and dementia due to nicotinic acid deficiency is now rare in the United States, and it is limited to the alcoholic population in most developed countries. This disorder is characterized by a thickened, cracked rash in sun-exposed areas of the skin, occasionally accompanied by spastic leg weakness. It is difficult to ascertain how much of the symptom complex is due to nicotinic acid deficiency and how much is due to lack of other vitamins, such as pyridoxine. In a series of 18 adults with pellagra, 33% had dermatitis alone, 28% had cognitive impairment, and 17% had dermatitis and diarrhea (Spivak and Jackson 1977). Pellagra is invariably associated with malnutrition; patients with this disorder require general nutritional measures in addition to specific vitamin supplementation.
Brain Disorders Stroke (See Also Vascular Dementia) Single strokes have variable effects on cognition, depending on their size and location. Some strokes are essentially silent; many are highly symptomatic because of their interference with production or comprehension of speech, writing, or emotion. It is a reasonable assumption that all strokes involving the cerebral hemispheres will produce some degree of cognitive impairment due to interruption of association fibers or loss of specific cor-
Differential Diagnosis
163
tical neurons. The mental impairment produced by stroke will ordinarily show partial resolution over time, with maximum resolution occurring after about 1 year. The use of thrombolytic agents within the first 6 hours after a stroke reduces poor functional outcome, with best results achieved when these agents are administered within 3 hours (Wardlaw 2001). Regional cerebral blood flow studies show that the prognosis for recovery is related to the volume of penumbra tissue that escapes infarction (Herholz and Heiss 2000). Risk factors for cognitive decline after stroke include advanced age, preexisting cognitive decline, increased severity of deficit at initial presentation, diabetes mellitus, and silent infarcts (Henon et al. 2001) Stroke raises a number of issues with regard to differential diagnosis. As mentioned in Chapter 1, dementia needs to be differentiated from expressive and receptive aphasia and from specific agnosias. In addition, depression is a frequent sequel of stroke. Its incidence is estimated conservatively at 25%–33% over a period of 2 years after a stroke (Robinson et al. 1984); at 7 years poststroke, the prevalence of combined major and minor depression was found to be 20% (Dam 2001). Depression aggravates the cognitive loss due to stroke (Murata et al. 2000; Robinson et al. 1986). Depression due to left-hemisphere lesions may produce greater cognitive impairment than depression due to right-hemisphere lesions (Bolla-Wilson et al. 1989). Making the diagnosis of poststroke depression is complicated by patients’ communication problems. Many are unable to report their mood or specifically deny depression. However, if observation and history indicate crying spells, lack of interest in the environment, irritability, uncooperativeness, loss of appetite, weight loss, and sleep disturbance with terminal insomnia, a trial of antidepressant medication (see Chapter 7) is indicated. Prompt, positive responses to such medication are frequently seen at low doses.
Traumatic Brain Injury An important cause of cognitive impairment in alcoholic persons and of suddenly worsened cognition in cognitively impaired elders is head trauma–induced subdural hematoma (Traynelis 1991). In these two groups, head trauma often results from an unremembered fall. In persons taking anticoagulant medication (including aspirin), the trauma may be trivial. Symptoms of increased intracranial pressure may be present. Localizing signs may or may not occur. A frequent sign is ipsilateral pupillary dilation caused by pressure of the herniating temporal lobe on the oculomotor nerve. Skull films may show shift of midline structures. CT scan shows hypodensity in the area of the hematoma, as does MRI. A lumbar puncture may show xanthochromic fluid. Treatment of a subdural
164
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
hematoma is neurosurgical evacuation of the clot after care has been taken to ascertain and correct any predilection to abnormal bleeding. Concussive brain injury may be accompanied by transient loss of consciousness and variable periods of retrograde and antegrade amnesia. It may produce long-term impairment of concentration and recent memory, but it is not likely to produce a dementia syndrome. A study of 35 persons age 50 years or older who had sustained traumatic brain injury were examined with cognitive and neurobehavioral measures up to 2 months after injury (Goldstein et al. 2001). Those with mild injury performed comparably with control subjects. Those with moderate injury performed more poorly than either control subjects or mildly injured persons on cognitive testing, but both mildly and moderately injured persons showed greater anxiety, depression, and somatic concern than did control subjects. By contrast, in a group of 65 adults with severe traumatic brain injury, at least 30% showed residual impairment on cognitive testing at 1 year (Kersel et al. 2001). Head injury with prolonged loss of consciousness (days to months) may be followed by a delirium and then a dementia syndrome or profound personality alteration (Capruso and Levin 1992).
Other Neurodegenerative Diseases Huntington’s Disease Huntington’s disease is a completely penetrant autosomal-dominant disorder (Koroshetz and Martin 1997). Its onset is usually between ages 35 and 45 years, but 25% of patients have onset of motor symptoms past age 50 (Wojcieszek and Lang 1994). Huntington’s disease is characterized by loss of neurons in the caudate nucleus and the putamen of the thalamus. Various neurotransmitters are reduced in activity, including acetylcholine, g-aminobutyric acid (GABA), substance P, cholecystokinin, and angiotensinconverting enzyme. The genetic abnormality is located on the short arm of chromosome 4. It consists of an excessive number of cytosine-adenineguanine (CAG) trinucleotide repeats. With rare exceptions, persons destined to develop Huntington’s disease have 37–86 CAG repeats, in contrast to 11–34 repeats in normal individuals (Gusella et al. 1993). Thus, at-risk diagnosis is possible from DNA samples from individuals who have more than 38–40 repeats but is problematic for individuals with only 35–38 repeats. Cognitive or affective disturbances are often the presenting symptoms. Typically, the dementia is frontal, with prominent behavioral problems and disruption of attention. Although choreiform movements are characteristic of the disease, it may present in the juvenile form as an akinetic-rigid disorder. Physical signs include exaggerated deep tendon reflexes, cortical release signs, and signs of impersistence, such as inability to maintain
Differential Diagnosis
165
tongue protrusion. Atrophy of the caudate nucleus is evident on both CT and MRI scans (Figure 5–5), as is frontal atrophy. SPECT shows decreased frontal blood flow, and PET shows decreased basal ganglia metabolism. Huntington’s disease is gradually progressive, eventually resulting in profound dementia and death. Choreic symptoms are treated with a dopamine blocker such as haloperidol in doses ranging from 2 to 10 mg/day, bearing in mind the possibility of inducing tardive dyskinesia. Baclofen, a GABA analog, is also helpful as an antichoreic agent, as is low-dose bromocriptine.
FIGURE 5–5. Computed tomographic scan showing Huntington’s disease. Destruction of the head of the caudate nucleus gives the ventricles a square appearance. Source.
Courtesy of Dr. Linda Judge.
Infectious Prion Diseases Prions are the only known infectious pathogens lacking nucleic acid. They reproduce by recruiting normal cellular prion protein (PrPC) and stimulating its conversion to the disease-causing isoform (PrPSc), the scrapie agent. Prion diseases produce a range of neurological symptoms, including dementia, ataxia, insomnia, paraplegia, paresthesias, and behavioral disturbances. Sporadic Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease (CJD) accounts for about 85%
166
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
of cases, whereas infectious and inherited prion diseases account for the remainder, which include familial CJD, Gerstmann-Sträussler-Scheinker disease, and familial fatal insomnia. The familial forms are caused by mutations in the prion protein gene (Prusiner 2001).
Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease CJD is a rapidly progressive dementing disorder often accompanied by cerebellar ataxia and myoclonic jerks. The electroencephalogram (EEG) frequently shows 1-Hz periodic complexes. Ordinarily a disease of older adults, death usually occurs within 6 months to a year. True sporadic cases are due to spontaneous mutations in the prion protein gene with production of an infectious agent PrPSc. The agent is resistant to chemical and physical agents and produces no inflammatory response (Lehrich 1985). The disease is diagnosed by detection of PrPSc in brain (Serban et al. 1990). The source of infection, when caused by transmission from person to person, is not known in most cases. Known sources of transmission include corneal transplants, contaminated depth electrodes, and injections of human growth hormone from pooled cadaveric pituitary glands. In iatrogenic cases, the incubation period is 10 years or more (Gibbs et al. 1985). PrPSc is of very low infectivity, but because of its transmissibility, precautions should be taken comparable to those taken with hepatitis B. Contact with body fluid requires that gloves be worn. Infected materials should be autoclaved at 132°C and 15 lb/in2 for 1 hour or immersed in 1% sodium hydroxide for 1 hour. Spongiform changes and gliosis are present in the cerebral and cerebellar cortex. Heidenhain’s variant of the disease affects the parieto-occipital regions primarily. The presence in CSF of brain protein 14-3-3 strongly supports the diagnosis of CJD but may be confounded by recent stroke (Hsich et al. 1996). Familial CJD and Gerstmann-Sträussler-Scheinker syndrome are due to mutations in the prion protein gene (Gambetti et al. 1999). GerstmannSträussler-Scheinker syndrome has a course of about 5 years and is characterized by cerebellar ataxia and dementia.
Limbic Encephalitis Limbic encephalitis is a progressive dementia, often preceded or accompanied by agitation, depression, and behavioral disturbances. It is associated with systemic neoplasms, including oat-cell lung carcinoma, and with carcinoma of the ovary, breast, uterus, and stomach (Case Records of the Massachusetts General Hospital 1985). Autopsy reveals an inflammatory reaction confined largely to the temporal lobes, including hippocampus and amygdala.
Differential Diagnosis
167
CSF may show mild lymphocytosis. An EEG may be normal or abnormal. A CT scan may reveal no abnormality, whereas MRI shows medial temporal lobe enhancement. Paraneoplastic antibodies such as Hu, MaTa, or Yo autoantibodies may be positive. Treatment of the related malignancy does not reverse the course of the illness, and the use of immunosuppressives is generally unsatisfactory (Senties-Madrid and Vega-Boada 2001).
Adult Forms of Hereditary Metabolic Diseases Hereditary metabolic diseases that manifest in adults include metachromatic leukodystrophy, adrenoleukodystrophy, and adult polyglucosan body disease. Metachromatic leukodystrophy is caused by lack of the enzyme arylsulfatase. It is transmitted as an autosomal recessive, usually manifesting in the first decade of life. There is widespread degeneration of myelinated fibers in the central and peripheral nervous system, leading to combined upper and lower motor neuron signs. Adults may become demented, but they also lose deep tendon reflexes and have Babinski signs. Cases are reported to occur as late as age 62 years (Bosch and Hart 1978). Diagnosis is by determination of arylsulfatase activity in leukocytes or cultured skin fibroblasts (Berger et al. 2001). Adult polyglucosan body disease may develop in late adulthood, with upper and lower motor neuron involvement, marked sensory loss, neurogenic bladder, and dementia. In this disease, there is massive accumulation of polyglucosan bodies throughout the central and peripheral nervous system. Diagnosis is made by sural nerve biopsy demonstrating polyglucosan inclusions (Lafora bodies) in processes of nerve cells and in astrocytes (Bigio et al. 1997). Adrenoleukodystrophy (also termed adrenomyeloneuropathy) generally presents with spastic paraparesis and peripheral neuropathy (Menza et al. 1988) but occasionally causes dementia. It is a peroxisomal disorder that results in the systemic accumulation of very-long-chain fatty acids, with concentrations highest in the adrenal cortex and CNS. Although transmitted as an X-linked recessive, it is occasionally symptomatic in women. The diagnosis is made by demonstrating elevated C26/C22 fatty acids in serum and tissue assay (Berger et al. 2001). Ceroid lipofuscinosis may begin in adolescence or early adulthood, with personality changes, myoclonus, and seizures. Later, cerebellar ataxia spasticity and rigidity of limbs and dementia develop. Other diseases not mentioned earlier that can manifest past age 50 are Hallervorden-Spatz disease (Paulson et al. 1997) and neuroacanthosis (Shoulson and Kurlan 1997).
168
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Human Immunodeficiency Virus Infection Human immunodeficiency virus type 1 (HIV-1) crosses the blood-brain barrier early in the course of HIV-1 infection (Resnick et al. 1988). The cognitive disorders associated directly with HIV-1 infection are minor cognitive/motor disorder (MCMD) and HIV-1–associated cognitive/motor complex. Criteria for MCMD include at least two of the following symptoms having been present for at least 1 month by self-report and confirmed by neurological examination or neuropsychological testing: impaired attention or concentration, mental slowing, impaired memory, slowed movements, incoordination, personality change or irritability or emotional lability, mild functional status decrement, and exclusion of other causes of cognitive/motor impairment (American Academy of Neurology AIDS Task Force 1991). The presence of MCMD is associated with a threefold increase in risk for subsequent development of HIV-1–associated dementia (Stern et al. 2001).
HIV-1–Associated Dementia HIV-1–associated dementia (HAD) is a complication of HIV infection and in some individuals may be the earliest or the only clinical manifestation of HIV-1 infection. Early reports indicated that by 12 months after the first diagnosis of AIDS, 7% of patients were demented and that the proportion of demented patients increased to 15% when the same patients were followed up until death (McArthur et al. 1993). In a case-control study of HIV-1–positive injecting drug users followed up over 10 years, at least 6% developed HAD (Goodwin et al. 1996). In this cohort, HAD was associated with a more advanced stage of the systemic disease, increased rates of decline in CD4 cell count, and reduced survival. The administration of zidovudine (Retrovir) has been reported to have a protective effect (Portegies et al. 1993), but protection appears to last only about 18 months (Baldeweg et al. 1998). The onset of HAD is typically insidious; acute, abrupt onset usually occurs in the setting of systemic illness or secondary infection (Navia et al. 1986b). The earliest clinical features include subtle mood changes, inattentiveness, difficulty concentrating, distractibility, forgetfulness, and apathy. Patients often complain of being increasingly absent minded and mentally slow. Other early manifestations may include confusion, disorientation, malaise, lethargy, anhedonia, social withdrawal, diminished quality of work, and disinterest (Pajeau and Román 1992). Motor symptoms such as clumsiness, deterioration in fine motor tasks, leg weakness, loss of balance, hyperreflexia, and sustention tremor may also be present (Simpson and Tagliati 1994). With disease progression, widespread cognitive
Differential Diagnosis
169
impairment and psychomotor retardation dominate the clinical picture. Rarely, hallucinatory or delusional symptoms develop (Navia 1990). The severity of clinical deterioration correlates generally with the severity of brain pathology. Some degree of cerebral atrophy is present in almost all demented HIV patients, with pathology found primarily in subcortical structures, particularly the basal ganglia and thalamus. Histological examination demonstrates diffuse pallor of the centrum semiovale with a mononuclear inflammatory response in the white matter and deep gray nuclei (Navia 1986a). The presence of multinucleated giant cells and diffuse myelin pallor is specific for HIV dementia but is not seen in all clinically demented patients (Glass et al. 1993). The neuronal injury may be due to direct viral effects and to toxic substances (tumor necrosis factor a, interleukins 1 and 6, and chemokines) produced by infected macrophages (Glabinski and Ransohoff 1999). The neuropsychological deficits of HIV dementia include impaired attention and concentration, psychomotor slowing, diminished visuospatial performance, poor complex reaction time, memory disturbance, and personality or mood alterations such as apathy and irritability (see also Chapter 8). There is evidence for three distinct types of individuals seropositive for HIV-1: 1) a subcortical group with depressed mood, psychomotor slowing, and forgetfulness; 2) a cortical group with verbal and visuospatial deficits, some psychomotor mood, and euthymia; and 3) a group without neuropsychological impairment (Van Gorp et al. 1993). Neuroimaging techniques are valuable in the diagnosis and management of HIV patients. CT and MRI show some degree of atrophy in almost all demented HIV patients. MRI typically reveals diffuse signal change in the subcortical white matter. Progressive multifocal leukoencephalopathy is the most common cause (Broderick et al. 1993). Neuroimaging techniques are primarily helpful in ruling out opportunistic infections and focal lesions, although functional techniques such as SPECT, PET, and functional MRI can demonstrate disease progression as well as response to treatment (Chang et al. 2000).
Nonviral Opportunistic Infections The protozoan Toxoplasma gondii is an intracellular parasite that frequently produces CNS infections in AIDS patients. CT scan with contrast shows multiple-ring enhancing lesions in the deep white matter. MRI is highly sensitive to the lesions of toxoplasmosis. Cryptococcosis is a fungal infection that occurs primarily in chronically immunosuppressed individuals and presents as a subacute meningoencephalitis with impaired cognitive function, cerebellar ataxia, or spastic paraparesis. Diagnosis is made by
170
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
finding the organism in India ink preparations of CSF or by a positive latex agglutination test for the cryptococcal polysaccharide antigen. Other nonviral infections include tuberculosis, infection with Mycobacterium avium, candidiasis, aspergillosis, histoplasmosis, and coccidioidomycosis.
Progressive Multifocal Leukoencephalopathy and Other Viral Infections Progressive multifocal leukoencephalopathy (PML), caused by reactivation of the JC virus that infects 70%–90% of adults, occurs in persons with defects in cell-mediated immunity such as AIDS and lymphoproliferative disorders (Thorner and Katz 2001). The reactivated virus infects oligodendrocytes and astrocytes, causing cell lysis followed by focal areas of demyelination and necrosis in white matter that cause the characteristic MRI pattern. It evolves subacutely, with cognitive impairment as one of many symptoms, including multiple paralyses, cortical blindness, aphasia, ataxia, and dysarthria (von Einsiedel et al. 1993). CT shows subcortical areas of patchy attenuation. MRI shows patchy, diffuse increase in signal in white matter. Definitive diagnosis is made by polymerase chain reaction detection of the JC virus. The usual course is relentless deterioration of neurological function. Other opportunistic CNS viral infections include cytomegalovirus and herpes simplex virus. In addition, varicella-zoster encephalitis and Epstein-Barr virus encephalitis may occur (Simpson and Tagliati 1994).
Other Infections Neurosyphilis may present insidiously as defects in memory, reasoning, deportment, and self-care. One or both pupils may be seen as a small, irregular Argyll Robertson pupil, which is poorly reactive to light but normally reactive to accommodation. In addition, there may be dysarthria, myoclonic jerks, tremor, hyperreflexia, and Babinski’s signs. Polyradiculopathy and neuropathic pain are common (Scheck and Hook 1994). The diagnosis is made by examining CSF, which may show 200–300 cells/mm3 (mostly lymphocytes), elevation of total protein concentration up to 200 mg/dL, increased C-globulin, and frequently a positive serological test. Permanent cognitive impairment occurs frequently after herpes simplex virus (HSV) encephalitis and may manifest as an amnestic disorder or as a full-blown dementia. An EEG is usually abnormal, with focal epileptiform activity; a CT scan shows early lucent areas, with later breakdown in the blood-brain barrier causing increased contrast enhancement on CT and MRI. Diagnosis is by demonstration of HSV antibodies or HSV DNA in CSF (Skoldenberg 1996).
Differential Diagnosis
171
Summary The differential diagnosis of cognitive impairment requires a broad knowledge of the conditions that may impair cognition. The ability to make an accurate diagnosis is becoming ever more important as a means to advance the knowledge of these conditions, to employ the proper treatment, and to counsel patients and their families with regard to the prognosis of these conditions and to the risk for other family members.
References Abou-Assi S, Vlancevic ZR: Hepatic encephalopathy. Metabolic consequence of cirrhosis often is reversible. Postgrad Med 109:52–54, 2001 Alarcon R, Franceschini J: Hyperparathyroidism and paranoid psychoses. Br J Psychiatry 145:477–486, 1984 Alvarez WC: Little Strokes. New York, JB Lippincott, 1966 Alzheimer A, Stelzmann RA, Schnitzlein HN, Murtagh FR: An English translation of Alzheimer’s 1907 paper, “Uber eine eigenartige Erkrankung der Hirnrinde.” Clin Anat 8:429–431, 1995 Alzheimer A: Uber eigenartige Krankheitsfalle des spateren Alters. Zeitscrift fur die Gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 4:356–385, 1911 American Academy of Neurology AIDS Task Force: Nomenclature and case definitions for neurologic manifestations of human immunodeficiency virus–type 1 (HIV-1) infection. Neurology 41:778–785, 1991 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 2000 Antunez E, Estruch R, Cardenal C, et al: Usefulness of CT and MR imaging in the diagnosis of acute Wernicke’s encephalopathy. AJR Am J Roentgenol 171: 1131–1137, 1998 Bachman DL, Wolf PA, Linn R, et al: Prevalence of dementia and probable senile dementia of the Alzheimer type in the Framingham Study. Neurology 42:115– 119, 1992 Baldeweg T, Catalan J, Gazzard BG: Risk of HIV dementia and opportunistic brain disease in AIDS and zidovudine therapy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 65:34–41, 1998 Berger J, Moser HW, Forss-Petter S: Leukodystrophies: recent developments in genetics, molecular biology, pathogenesis and treatment. Curr Opin Neurol 14:305–312, 2001 Bergeron C, Davis A, Lang AE: Corticobasal ganglionic degeneration and progressive supranuclear palsy presenting with cognitive decline. Brain Pathol 8:355– 365, 1998 Bigio E, Weiner MF, Bonte FJ, et al: Familial adult polyglucosan disease. Clin Neuropathol 16:227–234, 1997
172
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Blansjaar BA, Takens H, Zwinderman AH: The course of alcohol amnestic disorder: a three-year follow-up study of clinical signs and social disabilities. Acta Psychiatr Scand 86:240–246, 1992 Blei AT, Cordoba J: Hepatic encephalopathy. Am J Gastroenterol 96:1968–1976, 2001 Boeve B, Maraganore DM, Parisi, JE, et al: Pathologic heterogeneity in clinically diagnosed corticobasal degeneration. Neurology 53:795–800, 1999 Bolla-Wilson K, Robinson RG, Starkstein SE, et al: Lateralization of dementia of depression in stroke patients. Am J Psychiatry 146:627–634, 1989 Bosch EP, Hart MN: Late adult onset metachromatic leukodystrophy. Arch Neurol 35:475–477, 1978 Bottiglieri T: Folate, vitamin B12, and neuropsychiatric disorders. Nutr Rev 54: 382–390, 1996 Broderick DF, Wippold FJ, Clifford DB, et al: White matter lesions and cerebral atrophy on MR images in patients with and without AIDS dementia complex. AJR Am J Roentgenol 161:177–181, 1993 Brun A: Frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer type I. Neuropathology. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 6:193–208, 1987 Brun A: Frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer type revisited. Dementia 4:126–131, 1993 Brun A, Andersson J: Frontal dysfunction and frontal cortical synapse loss in alcohol main cause of alcohol dementia? Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 12:289– 294, 2001 Burgovne K, Swartz R, Ananth J: Porphyria: reexamination of psychiatric implications. Psychother Psychosom 64:121–130, 1995 Burns A, Jacoby R, Levy R: Psychiatric phenomena in Alzheimer’s disease. Br J Psychiatry 157:72–94, 1990 Capruso DX, Levin HS: Cognitive impairment following closed head injury. Neurol Clin North Am 10:879–893, 1992 Case Records of the Massachusetts General Hospital. Weekly clinicopathological exercises. Case 30-1985. A 52-year-old woman with a progressive neurologic disorder and a pelvic mass. N Engl J Med 313:249–257, 1985 Chang L, Ernst T, Leonido-Yee M, et al: Perfusion MRI detects rCBF abnormalities in early stages of HIV-cognitive motor complex. Neurology 54:389–396, 2000 Chen JY, Stern Y, Sano M, et al: Cumulative risks of developing extrapyramidal signs, psychosis, or myoclonus in the course of Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 48:1141–1143, 1991 Cooper AJL, Plum F: Biochemistry and physiology of brain ammonia. Physiol Rev 67:440–519, 1987 Cooper JK, Mungas D, Weiler PG: Relation of cognitive status and abnormal behaviors in Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 38:867–870, 1990 Cooper PN, Jackson M, Lennox G, et al: Tau, ubiquitin, and alpha B–crystallin immunohistochemistry define the principal causes of degenerative frontotemporal dementia. Arch Neurol 52:1011–1015, 1995
Differential Diagnosis
173
Corsellis JAN: Mental Illness and the Aging Brain. London, Oxford University Press, 1962 Cummings JL: Clinical Neuropsychiatry. New York, Grune & Stratton, 1985 Dam H: Depression in stroke patients 7 years following stroke. Acta Psychiatr Scand 103:287–293, 2001 Das A, Dhiman RK, Saraswat VA, et al: Prevalence and natural history of subclinical hepatic encephalopathy in cirrhosis. J Gastroenterol Hepatol 16:531–535, 2001 Dening TR: The neuropsychiatry of Wilson’s disease: a review. Int J Psychiatry Med 21:135–148, 1991 Dening TR, Berrios GE: Wilson’s disease: psychiatric symptoms in 195 cases. Arch Gen Psychiatry 46:1126–1134, 1989 Deutsch LH, Byslma FW, Rovner BW: Psychosis and physical aggression in probable Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 148:1159–1163, 1991 Dickerson FB, Sommerville J, Origoni AE, et al: Outpatients with schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder: do they differ in their cognitive and social functioning? Psychiatr Res 102:21–27, 2001 Dickson DW: Pick’s disease: a modern approach. Brain Pathol 8:339–354, 1998 Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, et al: The FAB: a frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology 55:1622–1625, 2000 Dugbartey AT: Neurocognitive aspects of hypothyroidism. Arch Intern Med 158:1413–1418, 1998 Eastley R, Wilcock GK, Bucks RS: Vitamin B12 deficiency in dementia and cognitive impairment: the effects of treatment on neuropsychological function. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 15:226–233, 2000 Ebly EM, Schaefer JP, Campbell NR, et al: Folate status, vascular disease and cognition in elderly Canadians. Age Ageing 27:485–491, 1998 Filley CM, Heaton RK, Rosenberg NL: White matter dementia in chronic toluene abuse. Neurology 40:532–534, 1990 Fishbain DA, Rotundo D: Frequency of hypoglycemic delirium in a psychiatry emergency service. Psychosomatics 29:346–348, 1988 Folsom CF: Mental diseases, in A System of Practical Medicine. Edited by Pepper W, Starr L. Philadelphia, PA, Lea Bros, 1886, pp 99–104 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189– 198, 1975 Fornazzari L, Wilkinson DA, Kapur BM, et al: Cerebellar, cortical and functional impairment in toluene abusers. Acta Neurol Scand 67:319–329, 1983 Gambetti P, Petersen RB, Parchi P, et al: Inherited prion diseases, in Prion Biology and Diseases. Edited by Prusiner SB. Cold Spring Harbor, NY, Cold Spring Harbor Press, 1999, pp 509–583 Gearing M, Mirra SS, Hedreen JC, et al: The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD); part X: neuropathology confirmation of the clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 45 (3 part 1):461–466, 1995
174
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Gibbs CV, Joy A, Heffner R, et al: Clinical and pathological features and laboratory confirmation of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease in a recipient of pituitary derived growth hormone. N Engl J Med 313:734–738, 1985 Glabinski AR, Ransohoff RM: Chemokines and chemokine receptors in CNS pathology. J Neurovirol 5:3–12, 1999 Glass JD, Wesselingh SL, Selnes OA, et al: Clinical-neuropathologic correlation in HIV-associated dementia. Neurology 43:2230–2237, 1993 Goldstein FC, Levin HS, Goldman WP, et al: Cognitive and neurobehavioral functioning after mild versus moderate traumatic brain injury in older adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 7:373–383, 2001 Goodwin GM, Pretsell DO, Chiswick A, et al: The Edinburgh cohort of HIV-positive injecting drug users at 10 years after infection: a case-control study of the evolution of dementia. AIDS 10:431–440, 1996 Granholm E, Jeste DV: Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Psychiatric Annals 24:484–490, 1994 Gregory CA, Orrell M, Sahakian B, et al: Can frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease be differentiated using a brief battery of tests? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 12:375–383, 1997 Gusella JF, MacDonald ME, Ambrose CM, et al: Molecular genetics of Huntington’s disease. Arch Neurol 50:1157–1163, 1993 Gustafson L: Frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer type, II: clinical picture and differential diagnosis. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 6:209–223, 1987 Hansen L, Salmon D, Galasko D, et al: The Lewy body variant of Alzheimer’s disease: a clinical and pathological entity. Neurology 40:1–8, 1990 Haupt M, Kurz A: Reversibility of dementia in hypothyroidism. J Neurol 240:333– 335, 1993 Heaton RK, Gladsjo JA, Palmer BW, et al: Stability and course of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58:24–32, 2001 Hector M, Burton JR: What are the psychiatric manifestations of vitamin B12 deficiency? J Am Geriatr Soc 36:1105–1112, 1988 Henon H, Durieu I, Guerouaou H, et al: Poststroke dementia: incidence and relationship to prestroke cognitive decline. Neurology 57:1216–1222, 2001 Herholz K, Heiss WD: Functional imaging correlates of recovery after stroke in humans. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 20:1619–1631, 2000 Hodges JR, Patterson K, Oxbury S, et al: Semantic dementia. Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain 115:1783–1806, 1992 Hohl U, Tiraboschi P, Hansen LA, et al: Diagnostic accuracy of dementia with Lewy bodies. Arch Neurol 57:347–351, 2000 Hosler BA, Siddique T, Sapp PC, et al: Linkage of familial amyotrophic lateral sclerosis with frontotemporal dementia to chromosome 9q21-q22. JAMA 284:1664–1669, 2000 Hsich G, Kenney K, Gibbs CJ Jr, et al: The 14-3-3 brain protein in cerebrospinal fluid as a marker for transmissible spongiform encephalopathies. N Engl J Med 335:924–930, 1996
Differential Diagnosis
175
Hulette CM, Crain BJ: Lobar atrophy without Pick bodies. Clin Neuropathol 11:151–156, 1992 Hutto BR: Folate and cobalamin in psychiatric illness. Compr Psychiatry 38:305– 314, 1997 Ihara M, Ito T, Yanagihara I, et al: Wernicke’s encephalopathy associated with hemodialysis: report of two cases and review of the literature. Clin Neurol Neurosurg 101:118–121, 1999 Incalzi RA, Gemma A, Marra C, et al: Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: an original model of cognitive decline. Am Rev Respir Dis 148:418–424, 1993 Jackson M, Lowe J: The new neuropathology of degenerative frontotemporal dementias. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 91:127–134, 1996 Jauhar P, Montaldi D: Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome and the use of brain imaging. Alcohol Alcohol Suppl 35 (suppl 1):21–23, 2000 Joachim CL, Morris JH, Selkoe DJ: Clinically diagnosed Alzheimer disease: autopsy results in 150 cases. Ann Neurol 25:50–56, 1988 Jose CJ, Barton JR, Perez-Cruet J: Hyponatremic seizures in psychiatric patients. Biol Psychiatry 14:839–843, 1979 Kersel DA, Marsh NV, Havill JH, et al: Neuropsychological functioning during the year following severe traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 15:283–296, 2001 Kertesz A, Hudson L, Mackenzie IRA, et al: The pathology and nosology of primary progressive aphasia. Neurology 44:2065–2072, 1994 Kertesz A, Devidson W, Fox H: Frontal behavioral inventory: diagnostic criteria for frontal lobe dementia. Can J Neurol Sci 24:9–36, 1997 Kertesz A, Martinez-Lage P, Davidson W, et al: The corticobasal degeneration syndrome overlaps progressive aphasia and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 55:1368–1375, 2001 Kirby D, Ames D: Hyponatraemia and selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors in patients. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 16:484–493, 2001 Knopman D: Overview of dementia lacking distinctive histology: pathological designation of a progressive dementia. Dementia 4:132–136, 1993 Knopman DS, DeKosky ST, Cummings JL, et al: Practice parameter: diagnosis of dementia (an evidence-based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 56:1143–1153, 2001 Kopelman MD: The Korsakoff syndrome. Br J Psychiatry 166:154–173, 1995 Koroshetz WJ, Martin JB: Huntington’s disease, in The Molecular and Genetic Basis of Neurological Disease, 2nd Edition. Edited by Rosenberg RN, Prusiner SB, DiMauro S, et al. Boston, MA, Butterworth-Heinemann, 1997, pp 545– 564 Kozora E, Filley MC, Julian LJ, et al: Cognitive functioning in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease and mild hypoxemia compared with patients with mild Alzheimer disease and normal controls. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 12:178–183, 1999 Kraepelin E: Lectures on Clinical Psychiatry, 2nd Edition. Translated by Johnstone J. New York, Wm. Wood, 1913
176
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Lang A, Riley D, Bergeron C: Cortical-basal ganglionic degeneration, in Neurodegenerative Disease. Edited by Calne D. Philadelphia, PA, WB Saunders, 1994, pp 877–894 Lehrich JP: Unnamed agents of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease and kuru, in Principles and Practice of Infectious Disease. Edited by Mandell GC, Douglas RG, Bennett JE. New York, Wiley, 1985, pp 1041–1043 Lund and Manchester Groups: Clinical and neuropathological criteria for frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 57:416–418, 1994 Luo J, West JR, Crook RT, et al: Ethanol induces death and cell cycle delay in cultures of pheochromocytoma PC12 cells. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 23:644–655, 1999 Massey EW: Neuropsychiatric manifestations of porphyria. J Clin Psychiatry 41:208–213, 1980 Mathuranath PS, Nestor PJ, Berrios GE, et al: A brief cognitive test battery to differentiate Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 55: 1613–1620, 2000 McArthur JC, Hoover DR, Bacellar H, et al: Dementia in AIDS patients: incidence and risk factors: Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study. Neurology 43:2245–2252, 1993 McEwen BS: Possible mechanisms for atrophy of the human hippocampus. Mol Psychiatry 2:255–262, 1997 McGarvey EL, Clavet GJ, Mason W, et al: Adolescent inhalant abuse: environments of use. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 25:731–741, 1999 McKeith IG, Perry RH, Fairbairn AF, et al: Operational criteria for senile dementia of Lewy body type (SDLT). Psychol Med 22:911–922, 1992 McKeith IG, Galasko D, Kosaka K, et al for the Consortium on Dementia With Lewy Bodies: Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathological diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLBD): report of the Consortium on DLBD International Workshop. Neurology 47:1113–1124, 1996 McKeith IG, Ballard CG, Perry RH, et al: Prospective validation of consensus criteria for the diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology 54:1050– 1058, 2000 McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al: Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of the Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 34:939–944, 1984 McKhann GM, Albert MS, Grossman M, et al: Clinical and pathological diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia. Arch Neurol 58:1803–1809, 2001 Mendez MF, Selwood A, Mastri AR, et al: Pick’s disease versus Alzheimer’s disease: a comparison of clinical characteristics. Neurology 43:289–292, 1993 Meneilly GS, Greenspan JL, Rowe J, et al: Endocrine systems, in Geriatric Medicine, 2nd Edition. Edited by Rowe J, Besdine RW. Boston, MA, Little, Brown, 1988, pp 402–430 Menza M, Blake J, Goldberg L: Affective symptoms and adrenoleukodystrophy: a report of two cases. Psychosomatics 29:442–445, 1988 Mesulam M-M: Slowly progressive aphasia without generalized dementia. Ann Neurol 11:592–598, 1982
Differential Diagnosis
177
Moe SM, Sprague SM: Uremic encephalopathy. Clin Nephrol 42:251–256, 1994 Molaschi M, Ponzetto M, Romin R, et al: Neuropsychologic symptoms of primary hyperparathyroidism in the elderly: report of a clinical case. Minerva Endocrinol 19:169–174, 1994 Morrison-Bogorad M, Weiner M, Rosenberg RN, et al: Alzheimer disease, in The Molecular and Genetic Basis of Neurologic Disease. Edited by Rosenberg RN, Prusiner SB, DiMauro S, et al. London, Butterworths, 1997, pp 581–600 Munoz DG: The pathology of Pick complex, in Pick Disease and Pick Complex. Edited by Kertesz A, Munoz DG. New York, Wiley-Liss, 1998, pp 159–168 Murata Y, Kimura M, Robinson RG: Does cognitive impairment cause post-stroke depression? Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 8:310–317, 2000 Navia BA: The AIDS dementia complex, in Subcortical Dementia. Edited by Cummings J. New York, Oxford University Press, 1990, pp 181–198 Navia BA, Cho ES, Petito CK, et al: The AIDS dementia complex, II: neuropathology. Ann Neurol 19:525–535, 1986a Navia BA, Jordan BD, Price RN: The AIDS dementia complex, I: clinical features. Ann Neurol 19:517–524, 1986b Neary D: Frontotemporal dementia, Pick’s disease, and corticobasal degeneration: one entity or 3? Arch Neurol 54:1425–1427, 1997 Neary D, Snowden JS, Northen B, et al: Dementia of frontal lobe type. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 51:353–361, 1988 Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, et al: Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 51:1546–1554, 1998 Pajeau AK, Román GC: HIV encephalopathy and dementia. Psychiatr Clin North Am 15:455–466, 1992 Paulson GW, Reider C, Dadmehr N: Hallervorden-Spatz disease, in The Molecular and Genetic Basis of Neurological Disease, 2nd Edition. Edited by Rosenberg RN, Prusiner SB, DiMauro S, et al. Boston, MA, ButterworthHeinemann, 1997, pp 1291–1297 Pentimone F, Del Corso L: Hyponatremia, cause of reversible dementia in the elderly. Minerva Psichiatr 33:165–167, 1992 Pick A: Über die Beziehungen der senilen Hirnantropie zur Aphasie. Prager Medizinische Wochenschrift 17:165–167, 1892 Portegies P, Enting RH, de Gans J, et al: Presentation and course of AIDS dementia complex: 10 years of follow-up in Amsterdam. AIDS 7:669–675, 1993 Powchik P, Davidson M, Haroutunian V, et al: Postmortem studies in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 24:325–341, 1998 Prusiner SB: Neurodegenerative diseases and prions. N Engl J Med 344:1516– 1526, 2001 Purohit DP, Perl DP, Haroutunian V, et al: Alzheimer disease and related neurodegenerative diseases in elderly patients with schizophrenia: a postmortem neuropathologic study of 100 cases. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:205–211, 1998 Rebiez JJ, Kolodny EH, Richardson EP Jr: Corticodentatonigral degeneration with neuronal achromasia. Arch Neurol 18:20–23, 1968
178
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Resnick L, Berger JR, Shapshak P, et al: Early penetration of the blood-brain barrier by HIV. Neurology 38:9–14, 1988 Richardson JC, Steele J, Olszewski J: Supranuclear ophthalmoplegia, pseudobulbar palsy, nuchal dystonia and dementia. Transactions of the American Neurological Association 88:25–29, 1963 Riley DE, Lang AE, Lewis A, et al: Cortical-basal degeneration. Neurology 40: 1203–1212, 1990 Robinson RG, Starr LB, Price TR: A two year longitudinal study of mood disorders following stroke: prevalence and duration at six months follow-up. Br J Psychiatry 144:256–262, 1984 Robinson RG, Bolla-Wilson K, Kaplan E, et al: Depression influences intellectual impairment in stroke patients. Br J Psychiatry 148:541–547, 1986 Román GC: Senile dementia of the Binswanger type: a vascular form of dementia in the elderly. JAMA 258:1782–1788, 1987 Román GC, Tatemichi TK, Erkinjuntti T, et al: Vascular dementia: diagnostic criteria for research studies: report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology 43:250–260, 1993 Rubin EH, Kinscherf DA: Psychopathology of very mild dementia of the Alzheimer type. Am J Psychiatry 146:1017–1021, 1989 Rubin EH, Drevets WC, Burke WJ: The nature of psychotic symptoms in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1:16–20, 1988 Scheck DN, Hook EW III: Neurosyphilis. Infect Dis Clin North Am 8:769–795, 1994 Schneider JA, Watts RL, Gearing M, et al: Corticobasal degeneration: neuropathologic and clinical heterogeneity. Neurology 48:959–969, 1997 Seltzer HS: Drug-induced hypoglycemia: a review based on 473 cases. Diabetes 21:955–966, 1972 Senties-Madrid H, Vega-Boada F: Paraneoplastic syndromes associated with antiHu antibodies. Isr Med Assoc J 3:94–103, 2001 Serban D, Taraboulos A, DeArmond SJ, et al: Rapid detection of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease and scrapie prion proteins. Neurology 40:110–117, 1990 Sevush S, Leve N: Denial of memory deficit in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 150:748–751, 1993 Shoulson I, Kurlan R: Inherited disorders of the basal ganglia, in The Molecular and Genetic Basis of Neurological Disease, 2nd Edition. Edited by Rosenberg RN, Prusiner SB, DiMauro S, et al. Boston, MA, Butterworth-Heinemann, 1997, pp 565–579 Siegel AJ, Baldessarini RJ, Klepser MB, et al: Primary and drug-induced disorders of water homeostasis in psychiatric patients: principles of diagnosis and management. Harv Rev Psychiatry 6:190–200, 1998 Sier HC, Hartnell J, Morley JE, et al: Primary hyperparathyroidism and delirium in the elderly. J Am Geriatr Soc 36:157–170, 1984 Simpson DM, Tagliati M: Neurologic manifestations of HIV infection. Ann Intern Med 121:769–785, 1994 Skoldenberg B: Herpes simplex encephalitis. Scand J Infect Dis 100:8–13, 1996
Differential Diagnosis
179
Snowden JS, Goulding PJ, Neary D: Semantic dementia: a form of circumscribed atrophy. Behav Neurol 2:167–182, 1989 Spivak JL, Jackson DL: Pellagra: an analysis of 18 patients and a review of the literature. Johns Hopkins Med J 140:295–309, 1977 Stern Y, McDermott MP, Albert S, et al: Factors associated with incident human immunodeficiency virus-dementia. Arch Neurol 58:473–479, 2001 Stron MJ, Grace GM: Dementia and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, in Pick Disease and Pick Complex. Edited by Kertesz A, Munoz DG. New York, Wiley-Liss, 1998, pp 159–168 Stuss DT, Peterkin I, Guzman DA, et al: Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: effects of hypoxia on neurological and neuropsychological measures. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 19:515–524, 1997 Swearer JM, Drachman DA, O’Donnell BF, et al: Troublesome and disruptive behaviors in dementia: relationships to diagnosis and disease severity. J Am Geriatr Soc 36:784–790, 1988 Thorner AR, Katz JT: Progressive multifocal leukoencephalopathy. Curr Infect Dis Rep 3:352–359, 2001 Tison F, Dartigues JF, Auriacombe S, et al: Dementia in Parkinson’s disease: a population-based study in ambulatory and institutionalized individuals. Neurology 45:705–708, 1995 Tomlinson BE, Blessed G, Roth M: Observations on the brains of demented old people. J Neurol Sci 11:205–242, 1970 Toth C, Voll C: Wernicke’s encephalopathy following gastroplasty for morbid obesity. Can J Neurol Sci 28:89–92, 2001 Traynelis VC: Chronic subdural hematoma in the elderly. Clin Geriatr Med 7:583– 598, 1991 Van Gorp WG, Hinkin C, Satz P, et al: Neuropsychological findings in HIV infection, encephalopathy, and dementia, in Neuropsychology of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias. Edited by Parks RW, Zec RF, Wilson RS. New York, Oxford University Press, 1993, pp 153–185 van Guldener C, Stehouwer CD: Homocysteine-lowering treatment: an overview. Expert Opin Pharmacother 2:1449–1460, 2001 Varma AR, Snowden JS, Lloyd JJ, et al: Evaluation of the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria in the differentiation of Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 4:126–131, 1999 Victor M: Persistent altered mentation due to ethanol. Neurol Clin North Am 11:639–661, 1993 Victor M: Alcoholic dementia. Can J Neurol Sci 21:88–99, 1994 Victor M, Adams RD, Collins GH: The Wernicke-Korsakoff Syndrome: A Clinical and Pathological Study of 245 Patients, 82 With Post-Mortem Examinations. Philadelphia, PA, FA Davis, 1971 Vieweg V, Lombana A, Lewis R: Hyper- and hyponatremia among geropsychiatric inpatients. Int J Geriatr Psychiatr Neurol 7:148–152, 1994 Vishwanath BM, Navalgund AA, Cusano W, et al: Fluoxetine as a cause of SIADH. Am J Psychiatry 148:542–543, 1991
180
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
von Einsiedel RW, Fife TD, Aksamit AJ, et al: Progressive multifocal leukencephalopathy in AIDS: a clinicopathologic study and review of the literature. J Neurol 240:391–406, 1993 Wardlaw JM: Overview of Cochrane thrombolysis meta-analysis. Neurology 57 (suppl 2):S69–S72, 2001 Watts R, Mirra S, Richardson E: Corticobasal ganglionic degeneration, in Movement Disorders, 3rd Edition. Edited by Marsden CD, Fahn S. Oxford, Butterworth-Heinemann, 1994, pp 282–299 Wechsler D: The Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence, 4th Edition. Baltimore, MD, Williams & Wilkins, 1958 Weiner MF: Dementia associated with Lewy bodies: dilemmas and directions. Arch Neurol 56:1441–1442, 1999 Weiner MF, Edland SD, Luszczynska H: Prevalence and incidence of major depression in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 151:1006–1009, 1994 Weiner MF, Risser RC, Cullum CM, et al: Alzheimer’s disease and its Lewy body variant: a clinical analysis of post-mortem verified cases. Am J Psychiatry 143:1269–1273, 1996 Weintraub S, Rubin NP, Mesulam M-M: Primary progressive aphasia: longitudinal course, neuropsychological profile and language features. Arch Neurol 47:1329–1335, 1990 Whelan TB, Schteingart DE, Starkman MN, et al: Neuropsychological deficits in Cushing’s syndrome. J Nerv Ment Dis 168:753–757, 1980 Wilcox SM, Himmelstein DU, Woolhandler S: Inappropriate drug prescribing for the community-dwelling elderly. JAMA 272:292–296, 1994 Wojcieszek JM, Lang AE: Hyperkinetic movement disorders, in the American Psychiatric Press Textbook of Geriatric Neuropsychiatry. Edited by Coffey EC, Cummings JL. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1994, pp 404– 431 Zubieta JK, Huguelet P, O’Neil RL, et al: Cognitive function in euthymic bipolar I disorder. Psychiatry Res 102:9–20, 2001
CHAPTER
6
Psychological and Behavioral Management Myron F. Weiner, M.D. Linda Teri, Ph.D.
Behavioral symptoms were the first manifestations of illness in Alzheimer’s report of the disease that now bears his name. Auguste D., a 51-year-old woman, became suspicious of her husband, and along with her increasing memory deficit, believed that people wanted to murder her. She dragged objects from place to place; she screamed incessantly (Alzheimer 1907/1987). These symptoms, “a heterogeneous range of psychological reactions, psychiatric symptoms, and behaviors occurring in people with dementia of any etiology” (Finkel and Burns 2000, p. 10) have been the object of intense study in recent years. These perceptual, ideational, affective, behavioral, and vegetative symptoms are as much a part of the symptom complex of dementing illness as are the cognitive symptoms. They may precede or develop in parallel with cognitive deficits and are often more disabling to patients and more troublesome to caregivers than are patients’ cognitive deficits (Coen et al. 1997; Donaldson et al. 1997). Behavioral symptoms often underlie the decision of families to institutionalize persons with Alzheimer’s disease (Ferris et al. 1987). Indeed, Steele et al. (1990) found that behavioral disturbance was more important in predicting institutionalization of community-dwelling persons with Alzheimer’s disease than were formal measures of cognition. Behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementing illnesses may not be solely attributable to patients’ cognitive deficits. Rather, they likely result from the interaction between a damaged brain, a preexisting personality, and the interpersonal and physical environment. Consequently, many symptoms can be treated by psychological or behavioral means, either
Linda Teri’s participation in this chapter was supported in part by a grant from the Alzheimer’s Association.
181
182
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
alone or in conjunction with medications. Certain principles apply to the psychological and behavioral management of persons with cognitive impairment, but the most efficacious interventions are based on understanding the individual patient’s disease, the patient as a person, and the social and interpersonal context in which he or she lives. Because impaired brain function materially reduces the likelihood of cognitively impaired persons implementing what they have learned due to decreased initiative, unawareness of deficit, impaired judgment, or failure to generalize from one situation to another, treatment is addressed largely to caregivers, who use clinicians’ suggestions in their everyday contact with patients. In this chapter, we discuss the physiological and psychological factors that might suggest the most helpful type of psychological or behavioral intervention for cognitively impaired persons. We also provide a set of general principles governing such interventions and discuss specific strategies for dealing with depressed mood and disruptive behaviors. Our primary focus is on persons living in the community. Issues related to institutional and nursing care are discussed in Chapter 12. Environmental factors that contribute to and ameliorate dementia-related behaviors are described in Chapter 13.
Physiological Factors The physiological factors that dictate the types of intervention to be employed include the predominant function(s) lost or impaired, the severity of that impairment, and whether the impairment is static, reversible, or progressive.
Predominant Function(s) Lost or Impaired Tissue destruction may be confined to one area of the brain or may be asymmetrical, even when the pathology is diffuse and bilateral. Thus, Alzheimer’s disease or frontotemporal dementias may present with predominant left- or right-hemisphere symptoms. The most complex functions may not be those first affected, and the functions damaged may have been developed to different levels before they were compromised. Well-socialized Alzheimer’s disease patients, for example, may preserve their ability to act appropriately in social situations until they become severely impaired. Even with markedly impaired verbal fluency and comprehension, many individuals with Alzheimer’s disease nod and smile at the appropriate time in social interactions without knowing to whom they are nodding or what is being said. Persons with primary progressive aphasia frequently behave appropriately until virtually all expressive language function is lost. By con-
Psychological and Behavioral Management
183
trast, individuals with frontotemporal dementias frequently manifest disinhibition, poor executive function, and apathy. Behavioral symptoms have been shown to be more strongly associated with functional disability in dementing illness than with scores on cognitive tests (Norton et al. 2001). Careful evaluation of patients will reveal the extent to which aspects of cognitive functioning are impaired, including understanding of spoken and written language, affective communication, memory, spatial orientation, orientation to self, judgment, and so on. Orientation to self is presented as an example. When a person is well oriented to self, calling her “Mrs. Smith” helps preserve her dignity as an adult. When orientation to self is impaired, she may no longer remember her married name. Thus, addressing her by her first name at this stage is better than using her married name. Another loss that occurs in dementing illness is loss of executive or regulatory functioning. This includes the ability to concentrate or focus attention, to distinguish the important aspect of a task or a situation from an unimportant one, to prioritize, to perform mental or physical activities in proper sequence, and to modulate behavior based on social cues. Many persons with impaired executive function are stimulus bound; they react to stimuli without regard to context. They open drawers or doors because they have knobs whose function is to open them. They put on two pairs of underwear because there are two pairs of underwear in the drawer. Recognizing deficits in regulatory function and constructing the environment and personal demands to match the level of functioning can reduce frustration for both cognitively impaired persons and caregivers. For example, knobs can be removed from drawers to prevent rummaging. Doorknobs can be concealed to prevent inappropriate entering and exiting, or a special set of drawers can be provided to enable rummaging as a time-filling activity.
Severity of Impairment The cerebral cortex and its association pathways enable the formulation and understanding of symbolic communication. The greater the neuronal damage or loss, the greater the impairment of comprehension and reasoning, and the greater the difficulty in symbolic communication to and by brain-damaged persons. For the purpose of patient management, it is useful to stage cognitive impairment. A convenient hypothetical staging for the purpose of patient management follows Piaget’s (1954) schema for cognitive maturation in children. 1. Sensorimotor. In this stage, what cannot be experienced by the senses does not exist. The child in the sensorimotor stage is unable to maintain
184
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
a mental image of a person or an object and experiences an intense affective storm when the nurturing person or object disappears from view at a time the child is experiencing physical tension such as hunger or thirst. Adults functioning at this level frequently shadow their caregivers and will often demand entry into the bathroom while spouses are toileting. This can be seen as an attempt to physically maintain the object constancy that they are unable to maintain cognitively. At times, they will accept a substitute caregiver or contact by voice, but many times they will not. Such individuals may find pets a source of comfort; later, this function may be served by a soft, fuzzy toy animal or even a blanket. 2. Preoperational. Preoperational children can remember objects and actions and grieve when important persons or objects are lost from view, but they have learned that matter is conserved and will search for what has been lost. Thus, the cognitively impaired person, whose possessions now seem unfamiliar, may engage in extensive rummaging in search of unnamed objects that appear to be lost. Wanting to go home also seems to be related to this level of functioning. Not recognizing present surroundings but remembering having felt comfortable elsewhere, the patient longs to go home when feeling emotionally taxed. The insistence of many such individuals on going home is often dealt with by taking them for a walk or a ride, arriving back at their place of residence, and saying, “Here we are!” By this time, the cognitively impaired person’s emotional tension has often subsided, and the residence is accepted as home. Cognitively impaired persons functioning at the preoperational level may be unable to understand the need for changes, such as relinquishing the checkbook and the steering wheel, nor is the person able to learn from mistakes. It is often necessary, at this stage, to employ subterfuge to avoid the adult equivalent of a temper tantrum. For example, when a woman with dementia who was accustomed to paying household bills realized that they were probably due, her husband said that he did not want to bother her and mailed them in her stead and offered to let her see what he had done. When poor judgment and memory impair driving at this stage of thinking, the husband can tell his wife that the car is being repaired, that the keys have been misplaced, or that their daughter has borrowed it. When functioning at the preoperational level, many persons are fearful in the absence of their spouse and are not consoled when told that the spouse will return soon, because they cannot remember the reassurance. Shadowing of spouses, as indicated above, also substitutes for the patient’s inability to hold the spouse in memory and anticipate his or her return.
Psychological and Behavioral Management
185
3. Concrete operations. At the stage of concrete operations, children can change their thinking when evidence is presented that their view is incorrect. Children at this stage can imagine what an object looks like from another physical point of view or imagine how others might react in a given situation. A person functioning at this level can accept that a wallet, initially perceived as stolen, may actually have been misplaced, and may be willing to join in a search for it. Many cognitively impaired persons have difficulty with depth perception, possibly related to difficulty sorting the significant from the insignificant aspects of their sensory input. A change in the texture or color of a floor covering suggests a change in the level of the floor, and patients may attempt to step up or down. Having made this mistake several times, some individuals can learn that there is no change in level. Persons who experience illusions or hallucinations can sometimes learn that their perceptions are false and can describe them as misperceptions or hallucinations. 4. Formal operations. In this stage, achieved by only 30% of adults according to Piaget, children become able to transcend reality and imagine what could be. It allows the generation of hypotheses that can be tested by observation and experimentation. Persons who have achieved the stage of formal operations in their development often maintain the ability to use their imagination early in the course of a dementing illness. They imagine what it will be like when they can no longer reason or remember. Many have written moving accounts of anticipating loss of their personhood (Cohen and Eisdorfer 2001). Cognitively impaired persons still functioning at the level of formal operations can understand the need for a joint bank account, for making a will, and for assigning power of attorney. The Piagetian stages do not exactly parallel the development of dementing illness. Young children are able to remember and thus learn, whereas much of the behavior seen in cognitively impaired persons results from an admixture of retained and lost ability to remember and to understand various means of communication, including spoken and written language, signs, and symbols. Placing a “do not enter” sign on a door may deter the entry of a person early in the course of a dementing illness who maintains the ability to read and to understand that the sign indicates a prohibition. Telling a person about to enter an off-limits area (in a gentle yet clear voice) “No!” and accompanying the “no” with appropriate gestures may be a deterrent as long as emotional speech and symbolic gestures can be comprehended. A person with profound cognitive impairment who can no longer read or
186
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
understand verbal communication may respond to a physical cue to stop or may be distracted into some other activity. When even that level of reaction to the external environment is gone, environmental changes may be found to prevent such behavior. Such means may consist of making door opening a two-stage procedure requiring the throwing of a bolt and the turning of a knob, or it may involve substituting a thumb latch for a doorknob. A common cause of agitation is caregivers using modes of communication their care recipients no longer understand. Caregivers then become angry about care recipients’ seeming obstinacy and defensiveness. Thus, effective communication, based on knowledge of the nature and extent of individuals’ impairment, is a good preventive measure for agitated behaviors.
Reversibility and Progression Working with a person with reversible cognitive impairment differs from working with a person who has static or progressive cognitive impairment. When there is a reasonable expectation that certain functions will return, individuals can be challenged to use those functions. A person with a reversible cognitive impairment can be encouraged to remember the day of the week or the month of the year. When a person who is recovering from a traumatic brain injury or a stroke is confronted with a problem, he or she can be encouraged to attempt to solve it. The presumption in these instances is that challenge will stimulate attention and help the individual improve function through concentration and practice. Similarly, in the months following brain trauma or stroke, it is useful to challenge patients to function to the limit of their abilities so that they can maximally employ the functions that have been preserved and can learn to compensate for functions that have been damaged or lost. When cognitive impairment is fixed, as is often the case 1–2 years after head injury or stroke, it is no longer useful to challenge the patient; the patient’s limits will have been established by this point. Persons in this situation should be helped to find the optimal environmental circumstances in which to operate. Many persons can live independently; others require supervision. Some persons will require assistance in dressing and grooming. Others will be able to manage those functions on their own but will become lost if unescorted. The principle to be observed in the management of fixed cognitive impairment is to encourage patients to function with a certain degree of autonomy but not so close to their maximal function that they continually experience or anticipate failure. For each person, a comfort zone can usually be established. If there is some question, they can be
Psychological and Behavioral Management
187
formally assessed through history taking, mental status examination, and neuropsychological testing. Many individuals will rail against their limitations and experience disappointment that they are no longer the persons they once were. This situation may be alleviated by praising them for the level of function that they are able to achieve and involving them in distracting activities that are experienced as pleasant diversions or seen as worthwhile pursuits. Some cognitively impaired persons do well with distracting activities such as playing simple games or taking walks. Others can be occupied in helping to perform household tasks or can be employed in sheltered workshops (Maddox and Burns 1999). When cognitively impaired persons continue to be discouraged, treatment for depression should be considered. The management of persons with progressive cognitive impairment begins with the principle of accepting individuals at their present level of functioning. After the level of functioning has been established by history and appropriate clinical means, including caregiver questionnaires (Baum et al. 1993), caregivers are encouraged to support, not challenge, patients. It is common for cognitively impaired persons to turn to accompanying loved ones for the answers to questions they cannot manage. In the clinical setting, we discourage this so that an adequate evaluation can be performed. We advise to the contrary in the caregiving situation. We advise caregivers to observe a second principle: filling in for the functions lost by persons with progressive dementia. To deal with memory problems, we ask caregivers to do the remembering or to supply sufficient cues so that impaired persons can remember. Medication taking illustrates this principle. Persons who are mildly impaired can be reminded to take their medications by laying out the medications for the day with reminders of the times they are to be taken. Medication compliance is assessed by counting pills at the end of the day. When it is obvious that compliance cannot be maintained by reminding, it becomes the caregiver’s job to actually administer the medication at the appropriate time in the appropriate dose. The strategy of filling in avoids painful confrontation of dementing persons with their fading abilities. Another strategy in working with progressive cognitive impairment is finding a person with whom the patient has rapport and designating certain aspects of care to that person. That person is frequently a housekeeper or a sitter from whom the dementia patient can accept both nurturing and limits. Usually, such a person is affectionate, has a sense of humor, and has the ability to set limits without being punitive. Positive bonding may occur in these cases through prolonged acquaintance or may occur almost instantaneously as a special kind of rapport. This type of caregiver often serves as a bridge between family members and dementia patients.
188
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Psychological Factors Premorbid Personality Individuals respond in different ways to cognitive and physical impairment depending on their psychological makeup. Those who are highly self-critical or who have high performance standards may have a difficult time maintaining their self-esteem. It is important for these persons to find activities that they view as productive. This may require some rudimentary cognitive reframing, such as suggesting that helping to prepare meals, cleaning up the kitchen, or doing the laundry is in fact productive. One mildly impaired man maintained his self-esteem by collecting undeliverable magazines at a local post office and delivering them to medical clinics to serve as reading material for persons waiting for their appointments. With more impaired persons, for whom autonomy and individuality remain an issue, it may be better to assign simple, repetitive tasks, such as drying dishes or taking out the trash, so that they can perform independently. On a dementia special care unit, a resident who was a retired nurse busied herself leafing through charts that she no longer understood. Others, who value personal relatedness over autonomy, seem more comfortable operating in tandem with another person on whom they can lean cognitively and psychologically. Some cognitively impaired persons pose problems because they become overly aggressive or inappropriately sexual as cortical inhibition diminishes. In many cases, aggression arises from misunderstanding the behavior of caregivers, seeing them as hiding possessions instead of helpfully placing them where they can be found. Inappropriate sexual display is frequently a consequence of having the urge to urinate or being interrupted during toileting and forgetting to cover up. With irritable persons, efforts are made to reduce the number and frequency of stimuli, such as avoiding social interactions and keeping them in quiet areas devoid of objects that can be thrown or damaged. Aggressive confrontations are also avoided, with caregivers stepping out of the way when violence is threatened instead of demanding that cognitively impaired persons control themselves. Sexual display is managed in the same way. Without a confrontation, clothing is arranged to cover the sexual parts. If masturbation occurs, patients can be steered into a place where others do not have to watch. Special care may need to be taken that caregivers are not sexually stimulating to these patients. The issue of continued marital sexual activity is discussed in Chapter 9.
Predominant Coping Mechanisms Normal coping mechanisms are described in Chapter 2 and are summarized in Table 2–1. Enabling patients to use their most intact or most needed
Psychological and Behavioral Management
189
mechanisms serves to heighten self-esteem and to reinforce a sense of personal integrity. The needs to make decisions, to employ one’s own will, and to assume responsibility remain strong in many cognitively impaired persons, often leading to conflict between them and their caregivers. The best general management principle is to allow decision making, exercise of will, and assumption of responsibility to the extent of the patient’s capability. Dressing is a good example. As cognitive impairment progresses, individuals have increasing difficulty with choosing clothing. In persons with mild cognitive impairment, this may cause only minor hesitation. With more severe cognitive impairment, inappropriate clothing may be chosen for both the weather conditions and the events of the day. In the former case, nothing need be done. In the latter case, caregivers can lay out two sets of appropriate clothing and allow a choice. Mastery can be exercised in less complex tasks. Many persons who can no longer function at complex jobs or pastimes can still perform many household tasks well. Keeping the yard neatly trimmed and adequately watered in summer and free of leaves in the winter can provide both needed activity and a needed sense of mastery. Indoor activities can include sweeping floors and folding laundry (Mahoney et al. 2000). Many persons who are aware of their cognitive deficits hold the hope that they will improve or at least remain static. If the cognitive impairment is deemed reversible or if the person’s functioning appears to be worsened by emotional factors such as anxiety or depression, hope of improvement can be reinforced. Where cognitive impairment appears irreversible and psychological and environmental factors do not seem to be reducing performance, the most reasonable attitude is to offer hope that the patient’s condition will not progress or that it will progress slowly. Some patients and caregivers can regard the issue of cognitive impairment with humor. In our clinic, we often poke fun at our own difficulty with memory, pointing to the various memory cues that we employ. For example, instant photographs are taken of each patient and the accompanying caregiver so that the staff will recognize patient and caregiver on return visits. The extensive records we keep are evidence that memory difficulties are common and that the person with dementia is not so different from others. Altruism can also play a role in maintaining morale. Many patients seen by our clinic value the opportunity to be involved as research subjects. It gives meaning to their lives and enables them to give to others, not only as passive subjects for study and experimentation but also as active teachers about dementing illness.
Mistaken Beliefs and Accusations Cognitively impaired persons frequently blame caregivers for stealing or may hide their possessions to keep them from being stolen, thus worsening
190
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
the problem when they cannot remember where objects are hidden. Such beliefs generally should not be challenged. For example, persons who believe that they are still able to drive may become confrontational or argumentative if told they cannot. Families often find it easier (and kinder) to tell them that the car is in the shop, that the keys have been lost, or the car has been borrowed. They may be told that the doctor does not want them to drive because of his or her concern over memory function, that they are being treated for memory dysfunction, and that when they improve, they will be allowed to drive again. In this way, caregivers avoid the primary responsibility of saying no and place the decision beyond their control, a ploy that partially diverts the patient’s anger from the caregiver. Acting out of impulses may be related to diminished cortical inhibition and occurs often in crowded dementia units in long-term care facilities. Cognitively impaired persons in long-term care facilities often strike out at others who get in their way or who appear to be invading their territory. Because such aggression is usually justified in patients’ minds, it does little good to reason with them. Instead, it is best to remove the patient or the aggravating stimulus from the scene and to distract the patient into some other activity. Lecturing about the evils of violence does no good because the lecture will not be remembered, and the chastising tone sets the caregiver in opposition to the cognitively impaired person rather than allying with him or her and empathizing with the person’s sense of having been trespassed against or injured. Repeated violence in response to trivial environmental stimuli may require relative isolation of certain individuals or placement in a simplified environment with few stimuli. When objects are consistently broken, it may be necessary to remove all breakable objects from individuals’ rooms and from their path if they cannot be constantly supervised. Some extremely aggressive or episodically violent nursing home residents will require psychiatric consultation or hospitalization for the management of their behavior.
General Principles The general principles of managing cognitively impaired persons, summarized in Table 6–1, lay the groundwork for the ABC approach described later in this chapter. The principles take into account impairment of individuals’ ability to learn.
Correct Sensory Impairment Persons with apparent cognitive impairment may benefit from an assessment of vision and hearing. An adequate screening eye examination
Psychological and Behavioral Management
TABLE 6–1.
191
General principles of managing dementia patients
Correcting sensory impairment Nonconfrontation Finding optimal level of autonomy Simplification Structuring Multiple cueing Repetition Guiding and demonstration Reinforcement Reducing choices Optimal stimulation Avoiding new learning Determining and using overlearned skills Minimizing anxiety Distraction
includes evaluation of distant and near vision and funduscopic examination. An adequate screening hearing evaluation includes assessing the ability to understand whispered speech and direct inspection of the auditory canals and eardrums. The patient’s hearing evaluation is important for the caregivers as well, many of whom attribute the patient’s lack of comprehension to hearing impairment rather than cognitive dysfunction. Caregivers can consequently focus their efforts on the modality of communication rather than sheer volume. An uncontrolled study of augmenting hearing in community-dwelling hearing-impaired persons with Alzheimer’s disease (n=8) showed that most could learn to wear hearing aids (range of 4–13 hours per day). After 2 months of hearing-aid treatment, 1–4 problem behaviors were found to be reduced, including forgetting and negative statements (Palmer et al. 1999). It has been our experience that the introduction of electronic hearing aids is not useful for hearing-impaired persons with dementia. The process of inserting and tuning them is too difficult to master, and the devices are often lost or thrown away. Mildly impaired persons who are accustomed to hearing aids may profit from having them adjusted if they are still able to manipulate them. On the other hand, correction of presbyopia with magnifying lenses, and improvement of hearing by removal of impacted cerumen are simple measures that can improve the ability of cognitively impaired persons to understand their environment. Cataract removal is also helpful in some individuals with cognitive impairment. Even with maximal correction, many persons still have significantly compromised vision and hearing. For that reason, it is useful to speak and
192
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
interact at a closer distance than would ordinarily be interpersonally appropriate and to face cognitively impaired persons when addressing them. A hand on the shoulder while addressing these persons also serves to reinforce their attention. Subtle physical guidance while walking is useful, such as walking arm in arm. In addition to having frequent physical contact when interacting, the use of gestures or other nonverbal communication often facilitates communication.
Acceptance The first principle in managing persons with cognitive impairment is accepting their present level of functioning: learning to value what is still there, and not dwelling on functions they have lost (Raia 1999). It is caregivers’ most difficult task, because it involves a fundamental change in the relationship between individuals (see Chapter 9). In many instances, caregivers are helped in this transition by being present during the mental status examination. We rarely find that caregivers overestimate cognitive impairment. Failure to appreciate the patient’s degree of cognitive impairment is common and is usually based on the wish or need of the caregiver to see the cognitively impaired person as being essentially intact. This is often the case with spouses or children who are heavily dependent on the affected person. It is often necessary to tell these individuals directly that their loved one can no longer be left alone or to maintain nutrition independently.
Nonconfrontation The principle of nonconfrontation is important in dealing with persons who are unaware of their deficits. A nonconfrontational approach requires caregivers to note the abilities and disabilities of cognitively impaired persons and then to fill in or compensate for those disabilities. Caregivers are advised to deal with deficits in memory, spatial orientation, and judgment of their loved ones in a matter-of-fact way that does not draw attention to the deficit. A story is told of a woman with mild dementia who was reluctant to attend a family gathering for fear she would be embarrassed by not remembering names. Her daughter cheerfully told her not to worry; she would help. She did so by greeting everyone by name. Her mother then repeated the name and continued the conversation. Caregivers often attempt to teach skills that have been lost or to encourage cognitively impaired persons to use their memory in the hope that those functions will be stimulated and return. Such attempts may be useful
Psychological and Behavioral Management
193
after an acute insult such as a stroke. With progressive dementing illnesses, these well-intentioned attempts may result in a cyclic pattern of caregiver frustration and patient agitation. Caregivers are frustrated when cognitively impaired persons ask the same questions repeatedly. Our usual advice is that to the limit of caregivers’ patience, questions should be answered succinctly, and a distracting activity or line of conversation should be introduced. Bourgeois (1993) has increased positive verbal behavioral and decreased negative or nonproductive verbalizations of persons with Alzheimer’s disease by having them use collections of picture and sentence stimuli such as memory books and memory wallets as self-prompts during social interactions. Family members are often concerned that dementia patients will become too dependent on others thinking for them and will fail to do what they are actually able to do. It can be gently suggested that the issue is not whether their loved one is dependent on them, but the quality of that dependence. Those who are sure that they have someone to depend on are often more secure in doing what they can for themselves. Those who sense their caregivers abandoning them to their own meager cognitive resources are more likely to become emotionally determined burdens. Many dangers can be avoided without making an issue of them. The gas or electric supply to a stove can be turned off. Weapons such as guns can be removed from the house. Poisons can be removed from places where they might be confused with foods. Excuses can be made for the nonworking condition of the stove; weapons can be borrowed. These nonconfrontational subterfuges help to maintain individuals’ sense of autonomy without reminding them of the painful facts of their disabilities.
Optimal Autonomy To a greater or lesser extent, all persons value their ability to govern themselves and their environment. The task in dealing with cognitively impaired persons is helping them to find and operate at the level of autonomy most consistent with their personal needs and coping ability. Much of the distress experienced by these individuals and their caregivers results from tension over this issue. The most common problem is that patients’ ability to regulate their own behavior and environment is not commensurate with their psychological need for autonomy. Indeed, part of the normal need for autonomy is exaggerated by a need to demonstrate that nothing is really wrong. More cognitively compromised persons demonstrate their autonomy by negativism. Although they do not know exactly what they want, they know what they do not want, for example, conforming to the caregiver’s schedule for taking a bath.
194
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Another autonomy-related issue is space. Territory becomes important to persons whose living space has become greatly encroached on, as in nursing homes in which residents’ private living space is reduced to one room or half a room. Many persons become very upset when other residents enter their room or a roommate crosses into their half of the room; after all, neither resident selected the other to share living space. In these situations, the right of the person intruded on needs to be defended, and the simplest way to do so is to divert the offending person. An explanation that the intruder meant no harm is often beyond the ken of the nursing home resident with dementia. The staff needs to serve as protectors in this instance, even if the reaction is out of proportion to the realistic danger posed by the intruder.
Simplification Simplification refers to reducing the number and complexity of environmental demands (Mace and Rabins 1999) and introducing tasks in simple steps rather than as a set of serial or contingent (“if this happens, then do such-and-such”) instructions. Many cognitively impaired persons are able to remain at home and can answer the telephone if the caller wishes to communicate directly with them. On the other hand, they are not able to take telephone messages for others when alone because they forget the content of the messages or forget to write them down. The life of a cognitively impaired person who is left alone at home during the day can be simplified by turning off the telephone ringer and using an answering machine to take messages. In this way, the cognitively impaired person can still make outgoing calls, but there will be no arguments over forgotten messages for other members of the family and no signing up with telephone solicitors for unwanted magazine subscriptions, and so on. When asking a cognitively impaired person to do chores, serial instructions, such as “Sweep the kitchen floor and then take out the trash,” are to be avoided. Instead, it is preferable to say “Sweep the kitchen floor, please,” while handing a broom to the person as an extra cue. Contingent instructions are to be avoided as well. Instead of saying “Look to see whether the water on the stove is boiling, and if it is, turn the heat down,” the better instruction is “Tell me if the water on the stove is boiling.” If the water is found to be boiling, a second instruction can be given to turn the heat down. Simplification is especially useful with persons who have difficulty dressing and undressing. For men, beltless trousers are the most convenient clothing. For women, a house dress is the simplest clothing. On the other hand, it is better not to argue with the dementia patient whose identity demands wearing a girdle or pantyhose.
Psychological and Behavioral Management
195
Structuring All persons attempt to organize their activities and environment to suit their abilities and personal needs. Cognitively impaired persons are limited in their ability to provide structure for themselves, and structuring daily activities and the environment often becomes the responsibility of caregivers. Some cognitively impaired persons require little structure and seem very comfortable without any planned or organized activity. They may sit for hours reading and rereading the same magazine or watching the television without seeming to pay much attention. Persons who require activity are in greater need of structure because they are frequently unable to organize their own activities. Such persons may need a day-care program or at least a structured daily routine. Bourgeois et al. (1997) have used 3´5-inch index cards containing the person’s schedule for the day as a means to reduce repetitive questioning. Providing structure often reduces the tension and restlessness of being unable to channel energy in a consistent direction without guidance. Suggestions for providing structure for cognitively impaired individuals are widely available (Alzheimer’s Association 2000; Dowling 1995; Stehman et al. 1996), but when devising schedules one should take into account each individual’s preferences. To this end, Teri and Logsdon (1991) devised a check sheet, the Pleasant Events Schedule–Alzheimer’s Disease, to help identify activities preferred by community-dwelling Alzheimer’s disease patients. Closely related to a need for structure is a need for environmental constancy or predictability. Most adults are faced with an ever-changing environment. Jobs demand change. Children grow up. Parents grow old. All of these changes require adaptation. Cognitively impaired persons are limited in their adaptive ability and therefore require sameness to function comfortably. Sameness means having meals at roughly the same time each day, going to bed at roughly the same time each evening, managing personal hygiene at the same times each day, and going for walks or engaging in other activities at the same times daily. Each day has an orderly progression with which cognitively impaired persons can become familiar. There is not the challenge of having to understand different activities or having to modify the overall daily schedule. Although persons with little cognitive impairment may enjoy a varied weekend schedule, those with more severe impairment seem to do better with the same routine every day of the week.
Multiple Cueing Multiple cueing refers to using several different types of stimulus to initiate and maintain a suggested action or activity. In an earlier example, a broom was use to cue a request to sweep the floor. Often, helping with dressing
196
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
can be accomplished by handing items to the person in the sequence they are to be worn. The ability to understand nonverbal cues and to imitate is maintained longer than the ability to understand verbal communication; therefore, nonverbal cues are useful augmenting devices.
Repetition Repetition is necessary because of attention deficit and slowness of information processing. If attempting to engage a cognitively impaired person in a new activity, such as going for a walk, the person’s attention must be first engaged, usually by calling out his or her name. If the response is uncertain or not apparent, the person’s name is repeated until attention is secured. The verbal repetition may be underscored by an additional cue of placing a hand on the person’s arm and entering the field of vision.
Guiding and Demonstration Accompanying commands and suggestions with pantomiming movements or direct physical guidance reinforces the communication and serves as a reminder of how things are done.
Reinforcement Reinforcement involves the process of encouraging positive behaviors. For example, in keeping with the principle of simplification, it may be best to begin with the suggestion of standing up, reinforcing that suggestion with a hand under the elbow or a gesture indicating rising: “Let’s stand up.” This may be followed by reinforcement, such as “Good!” and “There, you’re up,” and then “Let’s go for a walk.” Lesser amounts of repetition are necessary when comprehension is greater, but it is better to be overly repetitive than to further aggravate confusion. To shape behavior effectively, the reinforcing stimulus must immediately follow the behavior to be reinforced or extinguished. Even basic bodily functions have been shown to be responsive to reinforcement. Toileting behaviors can be facilitated. Urinary incontinence usually begins as an occasional accident, often causing great embarrassment. Individuals are helped to avoid repetitions of this behavior by reminding them to go to the bathroom or taking them to the bathroom at fixed intervals during the day, or by making bathrooms readily available and easy to find (Hutchinson et al. 1996). Times are marked on a piece of paper and checked off by the caregiver until the caregiver has learned the routine. Each time individuals succeed in urinating when they are taken to the bathroom, they are praised.
Psychological and Behavioral Management
197
Eating behavior can also be reinforced by having cognitively impaired persons patients eat in the company of others in designated eating areas at designated eating places (see also Chapter 12) and by providing readily available snack foods. Social behaviors are similarly encouraged by immediate and positive feedback.
Reducing Choices Advanced cognitive impairment makes choosing difficult; it is no longer possible to easily weigh the relative value of multiple alternatives. It is not possible to hold them in memory long enough to compare and contrast them or to assign them relative weights. The use of choices in relation to dress is illustrated above. Individuals can be asked what they would like to wear, being given a choice of two ensembles and asked which they prefer. It may not be possible to give a choice of when or what to eat in an institutional setting, but it is possible to give individuals a choice of what they would like to have first: meat or vegetables, milk or salad. By offering limited choices, individual dignity is maintained without creating confusion for cognitively impaired persons or frustration for their caregivers.
Optimal Stimulation The need for stimulation varies greatly depending on many factors, including cognitive intactness, alertness, emotional state, and physical state. The amount of stimulation received also influences each person’s behavior. Minimizing stimulation leads to sleep. Moderate amounts of stimulation awaken and may arouse interest and attention. Intense stimulation may cause overload with emotional distress, retreat, or aggression (Gerdner et al. 1996). Generally speaking, the more severe the cognitive impairment, the fewer stimuli that can be integrated and the simpler the stimulation must be (Hall and Buckwalter 1987). For example, it may not be possible for cognitively impaired persons to have a television or radio blaring while they are trying to play cards. Attempts should be made to determine the optimal level of stimulation for each person at various times of the day. Often, relatively complex situations can be tolerated in the morning but not in the afternoon, when many persons are fatigued. The early evening may provide insufficient orienting cues, contributing to “sundowning” (increasing confusion as sensory cues wane in the evenings) in some persons. For such individuals, stimulating activities may be offered in the mornings; afternoons may involve quiet television watching; and in the evenings, brief walks or social interaction may be offered.
198
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Determining and Using Overlearned Skills Memories and skills that are well imprinted often remain. For example, an accountant who was no longer able to do simple mathematical operations was still able to umpire softball games. Many musicians continue to play their instruments. These overlearned behaviors can be used to advantage and are established by taking a history of both occupational and avocational pursuits. Families are then encouraged to facilitate the use of these talents and abilities. Making a scrapbook from old family photographs is a good pastime that keeps individuals on familiar ground. Cooking may no longer be feasible, but setting and clearing the table may still be possible. The process of planting seeds and weeding a garden is usually well understood. The list of potential activities is limited only by the imagination and ingenuity of caregivers and the time and energy available to them to supervise and participate in these activities. A list of potential pleasant events (Logsdon and Teri 1997) may help generate ideas. For pleasant events to achieve the desired effect, they should be enjoyable, easily achievable, accessible, and reinforced. They should be introduced slowly and in small steps and be consonant with the individual’s cognitive and functional ability. Caregivers need to gently motivate care recipients to be involved in activities and to be flexible in their approach. If cognitively impaired individuals do not enjoy the activities in which their caregivers are attempting to enlist them, other activities may be sought. Such activities are catalogued in Sheridan’s (1987) Failure-Free Activities for the Alzheimer’s Patient and Thorsheim and Roberts’s (2000) I Remember When: Activity Ideas to Help People Reminisce. Reality orientation posits that confusion results from understimulation, lack of insistence or expectation that normal activities be performed, and lack of reinforcement of desired behaviors when they are performed. It attempts to bring cognitively impaired persons into contact with their present-day reality, including date, day, time of day, and geographical location (Taulbee and Folsom 1966). This type of program was often implemented as a 24-hour system in which the staff provided orientation and reality cues at every contact, and often included formal didactic classes. Most of the empirical research in this technique centers on orientation classes (Kasl-Godley and Gatz 2000). A recent summary of this research (Specter et al. 2000) suggests small positive benefits on cognition and behavior and the possible need for a continuing program to maintain benefits. Gerber et al. (1991) suggest that the small, transient therapeutic effects attributable to this technique in persons with cognitive impairment are attributable to the social interaction and stimulation rather than the specifics of the technique. By contrast, vigorous reorientation is appropriate in delirium. Delir-
Psychological and Behavioral Management
199
ious persons tend to be calmed by developing a sense of where they are in time and space. However, persons with progressive cognitive impairment may be unable to learn well, and when material to be learned is constantly re-presented, it leaves cognitively impaired persons frustrated and painfully reminded of their deficits (Dietch et al. 1989). Therefore, we agree with Raia (1999) that vigorous attempts at reality orientation are inappropriate in the progressive dementias. Responding to patients’ requests for orienting information is quite another story. Such requests should be answered succinctly and matter of factly, recognizing that the information may not be retained and that the same questions may be asked again.
Avoiding New Learning Although total impairment of new learning ability is not usual in cognitively impaired persons, the best assumption is that, if an activity requires learning new principles, such as bidding conventions in bridge, it is more likely to be frustrating than entertaining. If a newly introduced card game is based on already-learned principles, such as matching suits or arranging cards by face value, cognitively impaired persons are more likely to grasp them. Sometimes a demand for new learning cannot be avoided; for instance, parents may move into the home of their adult children. However, their bedroom can be fitted out with their own furnishings, and the hallway can be decorated with pictures from their former home that are arranged as they previously were in relation to the bathroom.
Minimizing Anxiety Cognitively impaired persons often become anxious when they cannot comprehend their environment. Additional anxiety can trigger suspiciousness, formation of transient delusions, or frank delirium. For this reason, attempts are made to minimize anxiety. This is accomplished by many of the means already discussed: keeping the environment simple, providing a structured routine, reducing choices, avoiding new learning, and so on. In addition, it is also useful to reduce anticipation, whether of positive or negative events. Discussing plans months in advance for a trip gives time for anticipatory fears to develop. Mentioning a visit to the doctor a week in advance is likely to stimulate incessant questions as to why the trip is necessary. It is more effective not to burden persons with moderate degrees of cognitive impairment with information or speculation about the future and keeping them focused on the present day.
200
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Distraction Distraction is a useful strategy for managing anxiety, anger, or depression in cognitively impaired persons. The dynamic is similar to that in dealing with a small child experiencing intense emotion: the emotion propagates itself and becomes less and less controllable. To suggest to agitated dementia patients that they calm down is frequently asking something beyond their voluntary control and intensifies their agitation as they struggle vainly to assert self-control. Instead, caregivers find means to help cognitively impaired persons to calm down. Thus, a woman struggling angrily to master her checkbook may be distracted by calling her to the kitchen to help with chores “just for a minute.” The anger having been interrupted, it is then possible to suggest that she put aside the checkbook and allow the caregiver to help her.
The ABCs of Behavior Management The general principles elaborated in the previous sections can be used to help deal with the emotional, behavioral, ideational, and perceptual disturbances that are common in dementia patients, with an ABC approach to analyze the specifics of the problem under consideration (Teri and Uomoto 1991). A refers to the antecedents of B, the behavior. Did anything or anyone trigger the behavior? What was happening before the behavior started? C refers to the consequences of the behavior. What happened as a result of the behavior? What was the caregiver’s response? Although many behaviors appear to arise de novo, this is seldom the case. By carefully examining the ABCs of behavior, factors that trigger and maintain the behavior can be identified and modified. When multiple behaviors must be dealt with, it is useful to begin with one behavior at a time and to restrict its definition to observable phenomena. In addition to ascertaining precisely what the patient does and what the caregiver does, it is also important to know how often the behavior occurs, when it happens, where it happens, and with whom it happens. Interventions are developed with the following characteristics: • They address an antecedent or a reinforcing consequence of the behavior. • They have objective, realistic, clearly defined goals and identified steps to meet these goals. • The steps to goal attainment are objective, realistic, and clearly defined. They are small, simple, straightforward, easily achievable steps to the larger goal. • Each step is constantly reevaluated. Problems may resolve quickly, but they more often require time, creativity, and flexibility. It is necessary to
Psychological and Behavioral Management
201
continually modify and adapt solutions to individuals’ current level of cognitive and functional impairment and behavioral disturbance. The ABC approach is illustrated by the following case: Mrs. B, a woman with Alzheimer’s disease, tended to talk loudly during church services (the behavior). On reflection, the daughter recalled that her mother tended to be more vocal after interacting with her friends (the antecedent) before the service began and that her friends seemed to enjoy her obstreperousness. The daughter dealt with the situation by arriving exactly on time for services, thereby avoiding the stimulating interaction of her mother with her friends until after services were over.
Increased behavioral disturbance has been associated with family visits to nursing home residents on a dementia special care unit. However, in one study the increase in behavioral disturbance was related to the degree of discomfort felt by family members during the visit (Martin-Cook et al. 2001). Thus, educating families about dementia and facilitating family members entering into pleasant activities with their cognitively impaired loved ones might reduce the level of behavioral disturbance during visits.
Surrogate Caregiving Caregivers fatigue, and tension inevitably arises between caregivers and persons with cognitive impairment. At a certain point, it is imperative for many caregivers to share the load so that they can take care of their ordinary business, get enough rest, and experience emotional relief. One way to distribute the load is to use a surrogate caregiver—finding or hiring a person with whom the patient has rapport and designating certain aspects of care to that person. That person is frequently a housekeeper or a sitter from whom the patient can accept both nurturing and limits. Usually, such a person is affectionate, has a sense of humor, and has the ability to be set limits without being punitive. Positive bonding may occur in these cases through prolonged acquaintance or may occur almost instantaneously as a special kind of rapport. In one case of a retired military officer, the key was the caregiver responding, “Yes, sir” and “No, sir,” that initiated the bonding. This type of caregiver often serves as a bridge between family members and persons who have difficulty tolerating role reversal with adult children and who can save face with the family by depending on the professional caregiver instead of their children. Although it would seem as if the dynamism of face operates at a higher level of abstraction than might be possible in dementia, it appears to weigh heavily in the behavior of many cognitively impaired persons.
202
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Dealing With Specific Syndromes and Symptoms In this section, the general principles elaborated earlier and some specific techniques are described for the management of depression, delirium, anxiety, agitation, wandering, sleep-wake disturbances, suspiciousness and delusion formation, hallucinations, apathy and withdrawal, and issues related to driving.
Depression Depression is a treatable condition that may be confused with dementing illness; however, many patients have depression concurrent with dementia. Caregivers often equate the loss of interest and initiative that occurs in dementia with depression. Persons who express persistent feelings of worthlessness or guilt or thoughts of death or suicide, or who appear apathetic or withdrawn should be evaluated for depression. Antidepressant medication is useful for major depression, but behavioral measures are useful in both major depression and more transient, less severe lowering of mood in persons with Alzheimer’s disease (Teri et al. 1997). Many cognitively impaired persons become demoralized and have a sense of lowered worth because of their decreased ability to work and to deal with the problems of daily living. It is therefore useful to engage persons capable of focusing their energy in simple repetitive projects with which they are familiar, at which they can succeed, and for which they can be praised. Raking the leaves in the fall, and turning and weeding a garden in the spring are good examples. Walking the dog is another activity that consumes both energy and time. Demoralized persons in nursing homes can be engaged in simple occupational therapy activities such as making lanyards, belts, and ceramic ashtrays and in recreational activities such as sing-alongs and shuffleboard games. A corollary to increasing pleasant events for depressed dementia patients is decreasing unpleasant events. Activities that are too demanding are unpleasant and likely to reinforce a sense of demoralization.
Delirium The principle of optimal stimulation is most important in the psychological management of delirium. Both too little and too much stimulation may aggravate delirium. Maintaining orienting stimuli by keeping window blinds open during the day and a light on at night is important, but televisions that cannot be seen well by visually impaired patients may overburden them and worsen agitation. Having familiar objects such as photographs, blankets, or pillows can be helpful in reducing confusion, as can the pres-
Psychological and Behavioral Management
203
ence of a friendly, reassuring caregiver. For patients with a mild underlying dementia, it is useful to attempt orientation to place, year, month, and day of the week. For those with more severe dementia, attempts at orientation add a further cognitive burden and compound confusion. Caregivers need to frequently identify themselves to patients with delirium and to communicate their presence through physical contact. Soft chest and extremity restraints may be necessary to keep patients in hospital beds or to keep them from dislodging or pulling out intravenous lines, endotracheal tubes, catheters, and so on. When possible, verbal restraint by a caregiver is preferable to mechanical restraint. When sundowning occurs, it may indicate a need for more environmental structure, such as evening activities.
Anxiety Persons who can be engaged in activities often find temporary relief of anxiety through distraction. Both anxiety and depression interfere with cognition and often result in patients repeatedly asking the same question, in part for information and in part as a form of clinging. Attempts to cut short question asking or writing down the answers to questions are generally of no avail in dealing with anxious persons, because it is the need for reassurance and comforting that generates further questioning. Repetitive questions should be answered briefly and often in the same way. Caregivers often tire of trying to think of newer or better answers when the same answers will suffice. The answer to the question is not as important to the cognitively impaired persons as maintaining contact with the caregiver. Unlike the anxious person who is cognitively intact, it is not possible to put off the cognitively impaired anxious person. Allowing these anxious persons to shadow caregivers is frequently helpful, providing a concrete response to the implicit request, “Do not desert me.” This proximity is very tiring for caregivers, who may then need more frequent relief. Anxiety may be aggravated by caregivers attempting to interrupt clinging behavior by threatening desertion, by insisting that their cognitively impaired loved ones already know the answers to the questions they are asking, or by pushing them to perform activities beyond their cognitive or emotional resources. Using the ABC approach, caregivers can be instructed to look for early signs of frustration and to examine the underlying causes of past anxious episodes. In addition, it is important to reduce possible reinforcement of anxiety by praising and paying attention to patients during nonanxious times. Patients who become anxious during social activities can be allowed to withdraw to more solitary activities. Those who become anxious when deprived of social contact can be kept in contact with others. Caregivers often come to discount the communications of chroni-
204
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
cally anxious patients and may thereby ignore needs that could be met, thus escalating anxiety over abandonment.
Agitation The term agitation has been used to categorize a variety of behaviors that are problems for patients, family, and professional caregivers. Agitation includes verbal and physical aggression that may or may not be aimed at others, and it has been studied most extensively by Cohen-Mansfield (Cohen-Mansfield and Billig 1986). Physically aggressive behaviors occur in 25%–50% of community-dwelling dementia patients and even more frequently in nursing home patients (Mann et al. 1984). The antecedent for many agitated behaviors is lack of appropriate stimulation, which may mean either increasing or reducing the frequency and nature of the stimulation patients receive. When they are engaging in appropriate, nonagitated behaviors, such patients should receive additional praise. All too often, a kind touch or word is the consequence of agitated behaviors and may be a reinforcer. Identifying triggers for agitation can be used to prevent these behaviors. If that is not possible, distraction, altering the environment, or removing patients from situations in which they become agitated can alter the behavior as well. Examination of many behaviors will often reveal a chain of events that initiate and maintain them. They include too many or too complicated physical and emotional demands, too many questions, noise, criticism, and fatigue. Many times, cognitively impaired persons impose these demands on themselves, as in the case of a woman who insisted on coming to our clinic alone. She arrived several hours early for an appointment. The staff explained to her that she had arrived early and attempted to keep her occupied. She stayed fairly calm until lunchtime, when the staff left. Then, responding to her own hunger and her frustration over waiting, she exploded and stormed out.
Wandering Many dementia patients seek to go home because they no longer recognize their own homes. Others seem to roam purposelessly from place to place where they reside, exiting when they have the opportunity but having no obvious goal in mind. The latter group is the wanderers. Wandering is not in itself dangerous unless patients are able to leave safe environments, and wandering behavior generally does not need to be interrupted unless the wanderer is imposing on others. When wanderers are relatively intact cognitively, they can be engaged in activities as a substitute for wandering, including simple games and chores. Wandering behavior usually responds
Psychological and Behavioral Management
205
well to redirection, even in advanced dementia. Unfortunately, wandering behavior may occur when caregivers are not present or are asleep, necessitating mechanical restraint from wandering, such as double-locking exits from the home and locking doors of areas into which long-term-care residents should not wander. Wandering is a major issue, especially in nursing homes. The principal means of dealing with residents who wander into other residents’ rooms or out the door is to have an adequate number of staff members to provide supervision and electronically controlled exits. Wandering behavior may be interrupted in more cognitively intact persons by calling out to them and distracting them by verbal means. Persons who do not respond to verbal restraint can be distracted by physically interrupting their wandering and gently redirecting them. Much wandering is due to patients’ inability to find their own rooms. Means for minimizing wandering are discussed in Chapters 12 and 13.
Sleep-Wake Disturbances The principles of sleep hygiene are discussed in Chapter 7. The use as a sleep aid of daytime exposure to bright light is discussed in Chapter 13 and is dealt with extensively by Harper et al. (1999). The most important principle is to provide a structured routine that maintains daytime physical and mental activity. The best way to do this is through establishing a daily routine that is constantly reinforced; for example, “It’s 10 o’clock and time to walk the dog,” or “It’s nearly noon and time for you to help me make lunch.” Patients who are not ambulatory can be helped out of bed and into chairs and stimulated by engaging them in brief conversation or an activity such as having a snack.
Suspiciousness and Delusional Thinking Suspiciousness and delusional thinking must be attended to, to be certain that cognitively impaired persons do not endanger themselves or others should they attempt to attack or defend themselves against imagined persecutors. Often, such delusions will be verbalized when patients are placed in situations they cannot master cognitively. The principle of nonconfrontation is the most important in dealing with suspiciousness and delusion formation. Instead, effort is made to determine under what circumstances suspicion and delusion formation are increased and to find means to avoid those situations. Common causes of suspicion are changes in daily routine and the presence of strangers. The common accusations “Someone has entered my
206
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
room” or “Someone has changed my room” can be managed by asking, “Do you want to see if anything is missing?” Such accusations usually arise when a patient cannot remember what the room looks like; the accusations are often worsened if objects in the room are rearranged in well-meaning efforts to tidy up or to place them more conveniently. At times, apparent delusions are misperceptions. A shadow on the wall may appear to be an intruder. Correcting the lighting may solve the problem. When cognitively impaired persons see strange or threatening persons in mirrors, mirrors may be covered or removed. The environment should be checked for noises that might be misinterpreted and for lighting that may glare or cast shadows. Frightening hallucinations often subside in well-lighted areas when other persons are present. Suspicious patients frequently hide certain of their belongings and later complain that these objects are missing. It is the caregivers’ responsibility to note favorite hiding places so objects can be more easily retrieved. An outburst of delusional accusations following a social outing or a trip to the grocery store may indicate that the trip was too long, the setting was too stimulating, there was too much activity, or the pace was too fast. All of these elements can be modified. However, it may be necessary to temporarily suspend such trips. Minimizing anxiety is another important means of reducing suspiciousness and delusion formation. For that reason, it is best not to inform suspicious dementia patients of plans for a visit to the doctor or a trip to the grocery store until immediately beforehand. That reduces the period of time during which tension can develop. Prosopagnosia contributes to delusion formation, such as Capgras’ syndrome, in which a patient develops the delusion that spouse, children, or other important persons have been replaced with evil look-alikes (Hayman and Abrams 1977). In this situation, it is best if the spouse or caregiver dresses nearly the same way each day as much as possible, wearing the same jewelry, the same perfume, using the same color lipstick, and so on. A matter-of-fact assertion that “I am your wife, Mary,” is all that is useful in response to an accusation of being a double or an impostor. More vigorous assertions and offering various types of proof tend only to increase puzzlement as to why a person would go so far to impersonate a spouse.
Hallucinations Hallucinations are common in dementing illnesses, but their form and content should be ascertained, because this information may suggest a treatable disorder. For example, tactile hallucinations accompanied by frightening visual hallucinations may point to a drug-induced delirium. Auditory hallucinations commanding patients to kill themselves may be
Psychological and Behavioral Management
207
part of a treatable major depression. Visual hallucinations are common in Alzheimer’s disease and its Lewy body variant (see Charles Bonnet syndrome in Chapter 2). A frequent complaint of Alzheimer’s disease patients is that children, little adults, or strange creatures are entering the house, but they are often dealt with casually or humorously. Frightening hallucinations may require medications to dampen patients’ emotional reactions, but they can also be dealt with by optimizing perceptual cues and encouraging patients to stay physically close to their caregivers. Hallucinations and delusions in cognitively impaired elders are often concretizations of their wishes and fears—and a principal fear, as noted previously, is that of abandonment. Their fear is best alleviated by having caregivers constantly visible. As is the case with the previously cited disturbances, distraction is often the best course of action in mitigating the distress of hallucinations. Also, the patient’s environment may be changed.
Apathy and Withdrawal Loss of interest in formerly valued activities and withdrawal from social situations are noted frequently by families of cognitively impaired persons and often indicate depression. Early in the course of a progressive dementing illness, efforts can be made to maintain involvement in these activities and in unstructured socialization. It is important for caregivers to develop realistic expectations with regard to the patient’s abilities. If frustration and discouragement arise due to the patient’s realization of the loss of former abilities, caregivers can intervene and provide the needed assistance and structure, praising the skills that remain. Later, apathy and withdrawal can be dealt with by engaging patients in structured activities. Often caregivers attempt to place themselves in the patient’s position and express difficulty in understanding how such activities could be rewarding. Although apathy and withdrawal can be successfully addressed, it is important that caregivers understand the stimulus limitations placed by cognitive impairment on their loved ones. Apathy and withdrawal are common behaviors in nursing homes. They are often defensive responses to abandonment and to living among strangers. Rather than reaching out to staff members who may be transient and roommates who remind them of their own condition, patients may keep to themselves and withdraw from emotional involvement with others. Caregivers need to determine the risk-benefit ratio of pushing patients to be more active and more involved with other persons. If a patient’s apathy is the product of extensive frontal lobe damage, attempts by a caregiver to activate that patient are not likely to be worth the effort involved. If apathy is part of a reversible depression, increased activity is part of the treatment.
208
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
When apathy is a temporary reaction to a new environment, patients can be stimulated gradually to be more active. In drawing apathetic or withdrawn dementia patients into activities, it is best to start by allowing them to be spectators or passive participants, engaging them gradually. In the home environment, nonchallenging activities such as watching television or taking short walks may be a good way to start. In institutional settings, attending group meals or sing-alongs is relatively nonchallenging.
Driving Persons with progressive dementing illnesses such as Alzheimer’s disease seldom have difficulty with the mechanics of driving. They can start the car, shift gears, make turns, put on the brakes, and turn off the ignition. Their loss of executive function and their orientation and visuospatial problems lead to driving in the wrong lane, ignoring traffic lights and warning signs, making inappropriate responses in emergency situations, misjudging distances, and getting lost (van Zomeren et al. 1987). In a simulated driving comparison of 18 persons with mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease with 12 age-matched control subjects, 33% of the Alzheimer’s disease patients experienced crashes, whereas no crashes occurred with the control subjects (Rizzo et al. 2001). To date, no accurate means have been found to distinguish between dementia patients who are able to drive and those who are not. However, a review of the literature by Dubinsky et al. (2000) suggested that patients who receive a cognitive impairment rating of 1 on the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (Hughes et al. 1982) (see Appendix E) pose a significant traffic safety problem. A simple and useful test of dementia patients’ driving ability is to ask caregivers whether they are comfortable riding with the patient. The best test, of course, is assessment of skills through an actual driving examination. In mild dementia, observation may be all that is necessary when orientation is relatively intact, when attention to traffic signs and signals is not greatly impaired, and when the driving is done in familiar areas. A spouse or another caregiver may ask to go along “just to get out of the house.” More severe cognitive impairment requires supervision or cessation of driving.
Memory Training and Retention It has been well demonstrated that cognitively intact persons can greatly increase their ability to memorize lists of related and unrelated words
Psychological and Behavioral Management
209
through the technique of associating visual images with the words to be remembered (Patten 1972). Many studies have been done on memory retraining in persons who have the memory deficits of normal aging (Floyd and Scogin 1997; Lachman et al. 1992; Neely and Backman 1995; Oswald et al. 1996), with modest effects and requiring periodic reinforcement (Schaffer and Poon 1982). Older adults with memory complaints who were followed up 3 years after participation in a self-taught memory program showed no long-term effect (Scogin and Bienias 1988). Memory training has been employed with persons who are brain damaged (Lewinsohn et al. 1977; Poon 1985). Although there are essentially no controlled studies on the subject, persons with static cognitive deficits due to acute episodes such as stroke, head trauma, or central nervous system infection appear to profit from memory training. Their ability to identify frequently seen faces and often-visited places can be improved significantly (Wilson 1987). They also can be helped to recognize the need to pace themselves slowly, to stay with what is familiar, and to use external reminders. Such training also appears to be helpful in combating discouragement and depression. Structured, systematic programs are commonly used to address the sequelae of traumatic brain injury, but these programs have not been validated in controlled trials (Rehabilitation of Persons With Traumatic Brain Injury 1998). Salazar et al. (2000) studied 120 active-duty military personnel who had sustained moderate to severe closed head injuries. The subjects were assigned randomly to an intensive, standardized 8-week in-hospital cognitive rehabilitation program (n=67) or a limited home rehabilitation program with weekly telephone support from a psychiatric nurse (n=53). At 1-year follow-up there was no difference in the primary outcome measure (return to work); 90% or more from both groups returned to work. There were also no differences in cognitive, behavioral, or quality-of-life measures. It seems reasonable to conclude from this study that healthy young men recover well from traumatic brain injury. The role of the rehabilitative process in bringing about their recovery is unclear and remains to be demonstrated. A memory-training program also appeared to be useful in a small series of individuals with multiple sclerosis (Allen et al. 1998). By contrast, it does not seem reasonable to expect that such training could overcome the effects of a progressive disorder, and the frustration that would be generated needs to be considered. However, Quayhagen et al. (1995) performed a successful study comparing active cognitive stimulation with passive cognitive stimulation and a waiting-list control group in persons with Alzheimer’s disease. There were approximately 25 caregiver–Alzheimer’s disease patient dyads in each group. Families in the treatment group had 12 consecutive weekly in-home training sessions including caregivers and patients. After this, the
210
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
cognitive interventions were administered by caregivers for 60 minutes each day for 12 weeks. The intervention addressed memory, problem solving, and social intervention. Alzheimer’s disease patients in the treatment condition improved in general cognitive function, memory, and verbal fluency by the end of the 12-week intervention and declined to baseline after 9 months. The placebo group remained static, and the control group declined below baseline. There was a dropout rate of only 13% over the 9-month study. The outcome of this study does not differ greatly from the outcomes of studies with cholinesterase inhibitors (see Chapters 7 and 15), but the intensity of the training required and the amount of effort required of caregivers seems far more than could be achieved on a large scale.
Formal Psychotherapy There are essentially no controlled outcome studies of formal psychotherapy in persons with dementing illnesses (Kasl-Godley and Gatz 2000). Persons who are cognitively handicapped (i.e., those with low IQ or who are educationally deprived) or who have mild static cognitive impairment can be treated individually or in groups with techniques such as paradoxical intervention, task assignment, promoting identification, reinforcement, education, and advice. These techniques are part of a general style or level of psychotherapy characterized as repressive psychotherapy and are well described elsewhere (Weiner 1986). Reports of formal psychotherapy with cognitively impaired persons such as that of Birkett and Boltuch (1973) give insufficient information about the patients treated and do not suggest significant gains. It is possible, however, that individual or group psychotherapy for persons with mild impairment might help reduce anxiety and depression and provide emotional support. It would be expected that these patients would respond best to techniques that are limited in reflective self-observation or acquisition of new information. These patients can learn to attempt only one task at a time and to make lists to augment memory function. They can accept advice, such as avoiding jobs that involve new learning, and can learn the names of persons to help them negotiate the various agencies with which they interact. In groups, they can be praised for their warmth, openness, and sound advice to others. Campbell et al. (1994) suggest including persons with dysexecutive syndromes in a structured family therapy that focuses on active problem solving. In our experience, cognitively impaired persons enjoy interactions in which they are treated with warmth and respect. Support groups have been established for persons with early Alzheimer’s disease (Yale 1994), and these
Psychological and Behavioral Management
211
groups are widely advocated (see Chapter 9), but there are no controlled studies of their efficacy with regard to cognition or behavioral disturbance. Thompson et al. (1989) proposed the use of cognitive-behavioral therapy for depressed early Alzheimer’s disease patients, suggesting strategies that might map onto various difficulties encountered by persons in the early stages. Behavioral interventions in depression are based on altering the self-reinforcing cycle in which decreased positive person-environment interactions increase depressed mood. Also, depressed persons do little that is pleasant, and the less they do, the worse they feel. A comparison of depressed Alzheimer’s disease patients on a waiting list with Alzheimer’s disease patients in a 9-week behavioral treatment program showed no change for the persons on the waiting list and significant decreases in depressive symptoms in the treated group (Teri 1994). Treatment employed three elements: the ABC model described in a later section to deal with events that lead to or reinforce depressed mood, a focused approach to increasing pleasant events, and a problem-solving approach to decreasing unpleasant events. In addition, patients and caregivers worked from a Pleasant Events Schedule (Teri and Logsdon 1991) to increase activities that patients enjoyed presently or in the past.
Validation Therapy Validation therapy is a group of techniques for communicating with cognitively impaired persons that include the use of empathy, empathic communication, reminiscence, and touch to establish emotional contact with cognitively impaired persons. Its originator claims that the technique restores self-worth, reduces withdrawal, promotes interaction with others, reduces stress and anxiety, stimulates dormant potential, helps finish unresolved life tasks, and facilitates independence (Feil 1993). Although there is no credible scientific evidence to support these contentions (Neal and Briggs 2002), there is little doubt that caregivers who experience and respond to the essential humanness of cognitively impaired persons add to the quality of their lives more than impersonal caregivers who treat them as cases. Caregivers should attempt to reinforce the personhood of the individuals for whom they care, whether by recognizing their favorite food, their favorite activity, or their professional title—whatever they value the most.
Reminiscence and Life Review Reminiscence and life review have been recommended for elders to help integrate their past experiences as a means to give coherence and meaning to their lives (Butler 1963, 1974). Means employed for life review have been written or taped autobiographies, visits to familiar places, the creation
212
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
of scrapbooks, and making collections of old letters. Reminiscence groups have been examined empirically and have been found to increase participant interaction in groups of cognitively impaired elders (Head et al. 1990); the life review technique has not been examined objectively.
Activity Therapies Activity therapies differ from psychotherapies in emphasizing activity instead of self-observation and verbally mediated learning. Patients’ attention is drawn away from themselves and toward the accomplishment of pleasurable or useful tasks. Distracting cognitively impaired persons from dwelling on their deficits and focusing them on tasks they can accomplish and enjoy is probably more useful than formal psychotherapy. In fact, much of what cognitively impaired persons probably gain in psychotherapy groups is just such temporary distraction. Activity or recreational therapies can readily employ the general principles elaborated earlier in this chapter. They avoid confronting patients with their disability, allow the level of autonomy best tolerated by the patient, simplify activities to the point that they can be mastered, provide adequate structure, and so forth. Patients use overlearned skills, such as singing songs or performing familiar repetitive activities. As a result of feeling valued and competent, the patients’ morale and self-esteem are maintained. There is a large body of literature concerning music therapy in dementing illness, in which music therapy is reported as having been helpful in dealing with repetitive disruptive vocalizations, sleep disturbance, and wandering (Hanser 1999). Snoezelen has been proposed as a type of activity for cognitively impaired persons. Originally developed as an activity program for mentally retarded persons, it has been proposed for use with cognitively impaired elders (Pinkney and Barker 1994). It is a means of stimulating the primary senses of sight, touch, hearing and smell. Sight may be stimulated by fiberoptic spray lights, bubble tubes, or color wheels projected onto walls. Hearing may be stimulated through music or animal or nature tapes; touch, through the use of any object or material with interesting tactile qualities; and smell, by the odor of flowers, cookies, or fruits. Because this is not a uniformly administered technique, there is little research confirmation of its efficacy (Brown 1999).
Medical Illness in Cognitively Impaired Persons The behavioral suggestions for the management of cognitively impaired persons at home or in long-term-care facilities apply to their management
Psychological and Behavioral Management
213
when medically ill. A person who is calm at home is more likely to remain calm in a hospital setting if accompanied by a familiar person who can serve as an intermediary and as a source of emotional support and orientation. After determining the need for hospital treatment, the physician asks the family if they can arrange for family members to be with the patient during the daylight hours and evenings, explaining that this is the best way to prevent delirium and to ensure the highest quality of medical care. Professional sitters can be arranged for if family members are not available. The least desirable alternatives are for confused persons to be sedated or restrained. It is helpful to explain to family members or professional sitters what procedures are being carried out, what they entail, and why they are being undertaken. Cognitively impaired persons do not usually tolerate long explanations well; on the other hand, they can comprehend the gist of a procedure. Thus, a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan becomes a brain X ray, and a lumbar puncture becomes a spinal fluid test. Because of attention, concentration, and memory difficulties, cognitively impaired persons cannot be expected to carry out instructions such as being asked to lie flat for several hours following a lumbar puncture. Such instruction must be given to the nurse or the sitter. Because these patients frequently cannot develop a rational understanding of medical procedures or adequately suppress their emotional reactions, they should not be subjected to prolonged or frightening procedures without the presence of a calming person or the use of a calming medication. For example, we prefer MRI to computed tomography as the routine imaging procedure of choice in the assessment of cognitive impairment. In some instances, MRI is unsuccessful because of patients’ poor ability to tolerate this frightening, incomprehensible new environment. In these situations, we employ computed tomography as our imaging procedure and sedate our patients with 0.5–1.0 mg of lorazepam by mouth 30 minutes before the procedure, as indicated in the sections on the management of agitation and delirium in Chapter 7. Delirium is a common complication of hospitalization and may be related to the medical condition under treatment, to prescribed medications, or to being in an unfamiliar environment. It is dealt with behaviorally and medically as indicated earlier in this chapter and in Chapter 7. Compliance with post-hospital medical treatment is a substantial problem for cognitively impaired persons who live alone. There are numerous reminder strategies that can be used, including posting checklists on the refrigerator or labeling various kinds of pill containers specifying when, where, and how medications are to be taken. However, there is no adequate substitute for direct supervision. It is best to prescribe medication that can be taken once a day and by having a family member, neighbor, visiting
214
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
nurse, or geriatric care manager check daily or several times a week to be certain the medication is taken.
Summary The psychological and behavioral management of cognitively impaired persons is based on a global assessment of these individuals and their surroundings. Changes in caregiver attitudes and behavior and environmental changes can both precipitate and ameliorate maladaptive, alienating, or dangerous patient behaviors. When cognitively impaired persons become behaviorally symptomatic, it is important to note the type of behavioral symptoms and to look for both antecedent sources of stress and the consequences of these behaviors. An understanding of individuals’ cognitive, emotional, interpersonal, and environmental assets and liabilities enables clinicians to decide how to construct an interpersonal environment that maintains cognitively impaired persons at the best level of functioning for them as individuals. Cognitively impaired persons are often able to participate in such decisions. Many will raise the question of their own ability to drive or to live independently and will respond to direct suggestion. Others will require environmental manipulations to ensure that they do not endanger themselves or others. Persons with static conditions, such as cognitive impairment that follows a head injury, can be challenged to develop their remaining resources. Progressively dementing persons are constantly losing ground and therefore best dealt with by accommodating their deficits. Autonomy and self-care are promoted when they are not too demanding of these individuals and their caregivers. In other instances, initiative may be discouraged and dependence fostered. In all cases, but especially in the progressive dementing diseases, what is best for patients changes in relation to the progress of their illness and the demands of their interpersonal environment. Ultimately, persons with progressive dementing illnesses may deteriorate beyond the reach of formal language. Communication then becomes nonverbal, conveying to these persons through touch and tone of voice what one would want to convey to a preverbal infant: gentleness, affection, and a consistent soothing response to evidence of discomfort. Unlike infants, persons with profound cognitive impairment are unable to learn that their environment is consistent and they often cannot soothe themselves when caregivers are temporarily unavailable. Thus, caregivers are often unrewarded for their efforts and come to feel frustrated when their charges do not appear to respond to their ministrations. For this and other reasons, caregivers themselves require the various kinds of support described in Chapter 9.
Psychological and Behavioral Management
215
References Allen DN, Goldstein G, Heyman RA, et al: Teaching memory strategies to persons with multiple sclerosis. J Rehabil Res Dev 35:405–410, 1998 Alzheimer A: About a peculiar disease of the cerebral cortex. By Alois Alzheimer, 1907 (Translated by L. Jarvik and H. Greenson). Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1:3–8, 1987 Alzheimer’s Association: Activity-based Alzheimer care. Available at: http://www. alz.org/hc/qcare/activity.html. Updated March 22, 2000 Baum C, Edwards DF, Morrow-Howell M: Identification and measurement of productive behaviors in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Gerontologist 33: 403–408, 1993 Birkett DP, Boltuch B: Remotivation therapy. J Am Geriatr Soc 21:368–371, 1973 Bourgeois MS: Effects of memory aids on the dyadic conversations of individuals with dementia. J Appl Behav Anal 26:77–87, 1993 Bourgeois MS, Burgio LD, Schulz R, et al: Modifying repetitive verbalizations of community-dwelling patients with AD. Gerontologist 37:30–39, 1997 Brown EJ: Snoezelen, in Enhancing the Quality of Life in Advanced Dementia. Edited by Volicer L, Bloom-Charette L. Philadelphia, PA, Brunner/Mazel, 1999, pp 169–185 Butler RN: The life review: an interpretation of reminiscence in the aged. Psychiatry 26:65–76, 1963 Butler RN: Successful aging and the role of the life review. J Am Geriatr Soc 22: 529–535, 1974 Campbell JJ, Duffy JD, Salloway SP: Treatment strategies for patients with dysexecutive syndromes. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 6:411–418, 1994 Coen RF, Swanwick GRJ, O’Boyle CA, et al: Behavior disturbance and other predictors of carer burden in Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 12:331– 336, 1997 Cohen D, Eisdorfer C: The Loss of Self: A Family Resource for the Care of Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders. New York, WW Norton, 2001 Cohen-Mansfield J, Billig N: Agitated behaviors in the elderly, I: a conceptual review. J Am Geriatr Soc 34:711–721, 1986 Dietch JT, Hewett LJ, Jones S: Adverse effects of reality orientation. J Am Geriatr Soc 37:974–976, 1989 Donaldson C, Tarrier N, Burns A: The impact of the symptoms of dementia on caregivers. Br J Psychiatry 170:62–68, 1997 Dowling JR: Keeping Busy: A Handbook of Activities for Persons With Dementia. Baltimore, MD, Johns Hopkins University Press, 1995 Dubinsky RM, Stein AC, Lyons K: Practice parameter: risk of driving and Alzheimer’s disease (an evidence-based review): report of the quality standards subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 54:2205–2211, 2000 Feil N: The Validation Breakthrough. Baltimore, MD, Health Professions Press, 1993
216
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Ferris SH, Steinberg G, Shulman E, et al: Institutionalization of Alzheimer’s disease patients: reducing precipitating factors through family counseling. Home Health Care Serv Q 8:23–51, 1987 Finkel SI, Burns A: Introduction. Behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD): a clinical and research update. Int Psychogeriatr 12 (suppl 1):9–12, 2000 Floyd M, Scogin F: Effects of memory training on the subjective memory functioning and mental health of older adults: a meta-analysis. Psychol Aging 12:150– 161, 1997 Gerber GJ, Prince PN, Snider HG, et al: Group activity with cognitive improvement among patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Hosp Community Psychiatry 42:843–845, 1991 Gerdner LA, Hall GR, Buckwalter KC: Caregiver training for people with Alzheimer’s disease based on a stress threshold model. Image: The Journal of Nursing Scholarship 28:241–246, 1996 Hall GR, Buckwalter KC: Progressively lowered stress threshold: a conceptual model for care of adults with Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 1:399–406, 1987 Hanser SB: Music therapy with individuals with advanced dementia, in Enhancing the Quality of Life in Advanced Dementia. Edited by Volicer L, BloomCharette L. Philadelphia, PA, Brunner/Mazel, 1999, pp 141–167 Harper DG, Rheaume Y, Manning BC, et al: Light therapy in Alzheimer’s disease, in Enhancing the Quality of Life in Advanced Dementia. Edited by Volicer L, Bloom-Charette L. Philadelphia, PA, Brunner/Mazel, 1999, pp 186–206 Hayman MA, Abrams R: Capgras’ syndrome and cerebral dysfunction. Br J Psychiatry 130:68–71, 1977 Head DM, Portnoy S, Woods RT: The impact of reminiscence groups in two different settings. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 5:295–302, 1990 Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL: A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry 140:566–572, 1982 Hutchinson S, Leger-Krall S, Wilson HS: Toileting: a biobehavioral problem in Alzheimer’s dementia care. J Gerontol Nurs 22:18–27, 1996 Kasl-Godley J, Gatz M: Psychosocial interventions for individuals with dementia: an integration of theory, therapy, and a clinical understanding of dementia. Clin Psychol Rev 6:755–782, 2000 Lachman ME, Weaver SL, Bandura M, et al: Improving memory and control beliefs through cognitive restructuring and self-generated activities. J Gerontol 47:P293–P299, 1992 Lewinsohn PM, Dancher BG, Kikel S: Visual imagery as a mnemonic aid for braininjured persons. J Consult Clin Psychol 45:717–723, 1977 Logsdon RG, Teri L: The Pleasant Events Schedule–AD: psychometric properties and relationship to depression and cognition in Alzheimer’s disease patients. Gerontologist 37:40–45, 1997 Mace N, Rabins P: The 36-Hour Day: A Family Guide to Coping for Persons With Alzheimer’s Disease, Related Dementing Illnesses, and Memory Loss in Later Life, 3rd Edition. Baltimore, MD, Johns Hopkins University Press, 1999
Psychological and Behavioral Management
217
Maddox MK, Burns T: Adapted work program: a sheltered workshop for patients with dementia, in Enhancing the Quality of Life in Advanced Dementia. Edited by Volicer L, Bloom-Charette L. Philadelphia, PA, Brunner/Mazel, 1999, pp 56–79 Mahoney EK, Volicer L, Hurley AC: Management of Challenging Behaviors in Dementia. Baltimore, MD, Health Professions Press, 2000 Mann AH, Graham N, Ashby D: Psychiatric illness in residential homes for the elderly: a survey of one London borough. Age Ageing 13:257–265, 1984 Martin-Cook K, Hynan L, Chafetz PK, et al: Impact of family visits on agitation in residents with dementia. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 16:163–166, 2001 Neal M, Briggs M: Validation therapy for dementia. The Cochrane Library, Issue 2, 2002, Oxford, UK, Update Software. Available (by subscription only) at: http://www.update-software.com Neely AS, Backman L: Effects of multifactorial memory training in old age: generalizability across tasks and individuals. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 50:P134–P140, 1995 Norton LE, Malloy PF, Salloway S: The impact of behavioral symptoms on activities of daily living in patients with dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 9:41–48, 2001 Oswald WD, Rupprecht R, Gunzelmann T, et al: The SIMA-project: effects of 1 year cognitive and psychomotor training on cognitive abilities of the elderly. Behav Brain Res 78:67–72, 1996 Palmer CV, Adams SW, Bourgeois M, et al: Reduction in caregiver-identified problem behaviors in patients with Alzheimer disease post-hearing-aid fitting. J Speech Lang Hear Res 42:312–328, 1999 Patten BM: The ancient art of memory—usefulness in treatment. Arch Neurol 26:25–31, 1972 Piaget J: The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York, Basic Books, 1954 Pinkney L, Barker P: Snoezelen: an evaluation of an environment used by people who are elderly and confused, in Sensations and Disability. Edited by Hutchinson R, Kewin J. Chesterfield, UK, Robinson & Sons, 1994, pp 172– 182 Poon LW: Differences in human memory with aging: nature, causes and clinical implications, in Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, 2nd Edition. Edited by Birren JE, Schaie KW. New York, Van Nostrand Reinhold, 1985, pp 427–462 Quayhagen MP, Quayhagen M, Corbeil RR, et al: A dyadic remediation program for care recipients with dementia. Nursing Res 44:153–159, 1995 Raia P: Habilitation therapy: a new starscape, in Enhancing the Quality of Life in Advanced Dementia. Edited by Volicer L, Bloom-Charette L. Philadelphia, PA, Brunner/Mazel, 1999, pp 21–37 Rehabilitation of Persons With Traumatic Brain Injury. NIH Consensus Statement 16:1–41, 1998 Rizzo M, McGehee DV, Dawson JD, et al: Simulated car crashes at intersections in drivers with Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 15:10–20, 2001
218
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Salazar AM, Warden DL, Schwab K, et al: Cognitive rehabilitation for traumatic brain injury: a randomized trial. Defense and Veterans Head Injury Program (DVHIP) Study Group. JAMA 283:3075–3081, 2000 Schaffer G, Poon L: Individual variability in memory training with the elderly. Educ Gerontol 8:217–229, 1982 Scogin F, Bienias JL: A three-year follow-up of older adult participants in a memory-skills training program. Psychol Aging 3:334–337, 1988 Sheridan C: Failure-Free Activities for the Alzheimer’s Patient. Forest Knolls, CA, Elder Books, 1987 Spector A, Davies S, Woods B, et al: Reality orientation for dementia: a systematic review of the evidence of effectiveness from randomized controlled trials. Gerontologist 40:206–212, 2000 Steele C, Rovner B, Chase GA, et al: Psychiatric symptoms and nursing home placement of patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 147:1049–1051, 1990 Stehman JM, Stachan GI, Glener JA, et al: Handbook of Dementia Care. Baltimore, MD, Johns Hopkins University Press, 1996 Taulbee LR, Folsom JC: Reality orientation for geriatric patients. Hosp Community Psychiatry 17:133–135, 1966 Teri L: Behavioral treatment of depression in patients with dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 8 (suppl 3):66–74, 1994 Teri L, Logsdon RG: Identifying pleasant activities for Alzheimer’s disease patients: the Pleasant Events Schedule–AD. Gerontologist 31:124–127, 1991 Teri L, Uomoto J: Reducing excess disability in dementia patients: training caregivers to manage patient depression. Clinical Gerontologist 10:49–63, 1991 Teri L, Logsdon RG, Wagner A, et al: The caregiver role in behavioral treatment of depression in dementia patients, in Stress Effects on Family Caregivers of Alzheimer’s Patients. Edited by Light E, Lebowitz B, Niederehe G. New York, Springer, 1991, pp 124–127 Teri L, Logsdon RG, Uomoto J, et al: Behavioral treatment of depression in dementia patients: a controlled clinical trial. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 52:P159–P166, 1997 Thompson LW, Wagner B, Zeiss A, et al: Cognitive/behavioral therapy with early stage Alzheimer’s patients: an exploratory view of the utility of this approach, in Alzheimer’s Disease Treatment and Family Stress: Directions for Research. Edited by Light E, Lebowitz BD. Washington, DC, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1989, pp 383–397 Thorsheim HI, Roberts BB: I Remember When: Activity Ideas to Help People Reminisce. Forest Knolls, CA, Elder Books, 2000 van Zomeren AH, Brouwer WH, Minderhoud JM: Acquired brain damage and driving: a review. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 68:697–705, 1987 Weiner MF: Practical Psychotherapy. New York, Brunner/Mazel, 1986 Wilson BA: Rehabilitation of Memory. New York, Guilford, 1987 Yale R: Early Stage Alzheimer’s Disease Support Groups: Research, Practice and Training Manual. Palo Alto, CA, Alzheimer’s Association of Greater San Francisco Bay Area Chapter, 1994
CHAPTER
7
Drugs for Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms Myron F. Weiner, M.D. Lon S. Schneider, M.D.
In this chapter, we present the general principles of administering psychotropic drugs to persons with dementing illnesses—individuals who are usually elderly (for an extensive review of geriatric psychopharmacology, see Salzman 1998). We then deal with selected drugs for specific emotional and behavioral symptoms and drugs currently approved in the United States for treatment of cognitive symptoms of dementing disorders. We focus primarily on drugs for which there are controlled studies, but a few drugs are considered based on case reports alone. As noted in Chapter 6, behavioral and psychological symptoms are as integral to dementing illnesses as their cognitive symptoms (Finkel and Burns 2000). The rationale for the use of psychotropic drugs is that many of these symptoms resemble psychiatric syndromes such as delirium, depression, and psychosis. Furthermore, it has been recognized that drugs aimed primarily at the amelioration of cognitive deficit may also have an impact on behavioral and psychological symptoms (Cummings 2000). In this chapter we present the effects of drugs used to treat cognitive symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease on both cognition and emotional and behavioral symptoms. General Principles The use of drugs for behavioral and psychological symptoms is aided by an accurate diagnosis of the underlying disease and by knowledge of patients’
The authors thank Dr. Matt Byerly for his review of the section on antipsychotic drugs.
219
220
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
medical history and general medical status and of their prescribed or selfadministered medications. Often, treatment of behavioral and psychological symptoms must be coordinated with the treatment of a medical condition such as heart failure, pneumonia, or hypothyroidism. The medical, social, and psychological histories are important because they may indicate the factors precipitating the presenting symptoms, such as a change of environment, as indicated in the following example. The daughter-in-law of Mrs. A, an elderly woman with Alzheimer’s disease enrolled in a cognitive enhancer study, called and expressed alarm that Mrs. A initially had a good response to the drug but had suddenly worsened. When asked to describe what had happened, she told the physician that Mrs. A had been living with her and her husband for about a month. They had gone to the apartment where Mrs. A had formerly resided to pack up more of her possessions. While there, Mrs. A suddenly became so confused that she no longer recognized her son. They took her back to their home, where she sat on her bed crying out for her own mother. The physician reassured the daughter-in-law that the mother had experienced a catastrophic response to the emotional stimulus of giving up her apartment and her former lifestyle, and that with a good night’s rest, she would probably settle down. The daughter-in-law called the next morning to say that indeed all was well again.
Many medical conditions aggravate the emotional, behavioral, and cognitive symptoms of dementing illness. This is especially important in latestage dementia patients who are unable to report physical symptoms, including constipation and pain, whether the latter is due to arthritis (Liu et al. 2000), a bone fracture, or a urinary tract infection. Having ruled out or ameliorated the medical conditions that may precipitate or worsen cognitive or behavioral symptoms, physicians next review patients’ medications for drugs that may alter mood or produce confusion or paradoxical excitation. In some cases, the indicated intervention is eliminating the use or reducing the dose of certain medications. Learoyd (1972) found that of 236 persons over age 65 hospitalized for behavioral disturbances, 16% had disorders directly attributable to the effects of psychotropic drugs. Certain classes of drugs should be avoided or used in minimal doses, depending on the type of illness underlying the patient’s present symptoms. For example, the cholinergic deficit in Alzheimer’s disease makes these individuals highly susceptible to further impairment of sensorium by anticholinergic drugs. Thus, the use for sedation of antihistamines such as diphenhydramine (Benadryl) is probably contraindicated, as is the strongly anticholinergic antidepressant amitriptyline (Elavil) for either sedation or its antidepressant effect. Bladder antispasmodics such as oxybutynin (Ditropan) should also be used with caution (Katz et al. 1998).
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
221
Age-Related Considerations In treating elders, consideration must be given to age-related changes in absorption, distribution, protein binding, hepatic metabolism, renal excretion, receptor sensitivity, and neurotransmitters. There is little age-related difference in the absorption of psychotropic drugs administered by mouth (von Moltke et al. 1998). It is possible, however, that the concomitant use of anticholinergic drugs might reduce the rate of absorption by reducing gastrointestinal mobility and that the use of antacids and calcium might delay absorption of psychotropic drugs by decreasing their ionization. In the aging process, lean muscle mass and body water decrease, whereas total body fat tends to increase (Schwartz et al. 1990), despite a tendency toward overall weight loss in late old age. Thus, psychotropic drugs, most of which are lipophilic, will be more widely distributed through the body. Because serum albumin may be lowered by more than 15% in the elderly (Bender et al. 1975), less protein binding occurs and more drug is available to receptor sites. The efficiency of the liver in detoxifying and eliminating drugs decreases with age (Vestal and Wood 1980), due in part to decreased cardiac output with diminution of hepatic blood flow. The result is that the firstpass effect (the rapid elimination of the first dose of a drug by the liver) may be considerably reduced. Therapeutic levels of psychotropic drugs may be reached more rapidly in elders than in younger persons, yet the time to steady-state levels will be longer. Depending on the cytochrome system involved, drugs that are metabolized by the liver may have inordinately long half-lives. For example, the half-life of lorazepam (Ativan) is 19.8 hours in elders in contrast to 11.2 hours in young adults (Swift et al. 1985). Renal function also diminishes with age (D.F. Davies and Shock 1950). Renal excretion of drugs may be further compromised by sodiumdepleting diuretics; nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs; and drugs such as propranolol that reduce glomerular filtration (Jeste et al. 1998). Decreased brain levels of dopamine (Robinson et al. 1972) and acetylcholine (Perry et al. 1977) make elders relatively more susceptible to the side effects of dopamine blockers and anticholinergic agents that cross the bloodbrain barrier.
Psychotropic Drugs Psychotropic drugs are used to treat psychological and behavioral symptoms, including those due to emotionally based responses to dementing illness or to inability to adequately understand and cope with the environment. These include the following:
222 • • • • • • • • • • •
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Depression Delirium Anxiety Agitation Sleep-wake disturbance Suspiciousness Delusions Hallucinations Apathy and withdrawal Hypersexuality Impaired judgment
In addition to their dementing illness and its direct and indirect effects on psychological function, patients may have preexisting mental illness such as schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, or major depression. Or they may have developed mania or depression after having become cognitively impaired. Although the fundamental psychopharmacologic treatment of these illnesses is not different in cognitively impaired persons than in cognitively intact persons, dementing illnesses impose additional considerations in determining dosage and type of drug employed.
Depression Major depression is common in illnesses affecting the basal ganglia—such as Huntington’s disease, Wilson’s disease, and Parkinson’s disease (Rosenblatt and Leroi 2000)—and in vascular dementia (Alexopoulos et al. 1997); it also occasionally accompanies Alzheimer’s disease (Weiner et al. 1993). A preexisting depressive illness may also continue to manifest or worsen in cognitively impaired patients. It is commonly accepted that antidepressants and somatic therapy are effective in treating major depression in persons with and without cognitive impairment (Butters et al. 2000; Greenwald et al. 1989; Reynolds et al. 1987). However, there are few placebo-controlled studies of antidepressants in persons with Alzheimer’s disease (three of which show no benefit over placebo) and none of electroconvulsive therapy. Reifler et al. (1989) reported that in depressed outpatients with Alzheimer’s disease, both imipramine (Tofranil) at approximately 80 mg/day and placebo were equally effective over a period of 8 weeks, with scores on the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (Ham-D) (Hamilton 1967) dropping approximately 8 points in each group. Nyth et al. (1992) reported part of a larger study that included 23 depressed patients with either Alzheimer’s disease or vascular dementia. Of these, 15 received citalopram and
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
223
8 received placebo. Patients were treated for 6 weeks, with doses of citalopram ranging from 10 to 30 mg. There was significant improvement in relation to baseline scores, but only marginal improvement over placebo on the depressed mood scale of the Gottfries-Brane-Steen dementia rating scale (Gottfries et al. 1982). In another study (Petracca et al. 1996), clomipramine (Anafranil) was compared with placebo in 21 Alzheimer’s disease patients with major depression or dysthymia diagnosed according to DSMIII-R (American Psychiatric Association 1987) criteria. Patients were randomized to 6 weeks of active drug or placebo followed by a 2-week washout followed by 6 weeks of crossover treatment. By week 14, there was no difference between drug and placebo on the Ham-D; scores for both groups had dropped from 17 to 8. There was forced weekly titration from 25 to 100 mg at bedtime, with drug dose reduction allowed for untoward side effects. In the final study, sertraline (Zoloft) was compared with placebo over 12 weeks (Lyketsos et al. 2000). There was forced weekly drug titration from 25 mg/day to 150 mg/day or the highest tolerated dose. Mean HamD scores decreased by 8.9 points for sertraline and 3.4 points for placebo (based on intention to treat), but only 16 of 22 subjects completed the study. Of interest, 37 Alzheimer’s disease patients with major depression were assigned randomly to treatment with fluoxetine (Prozac) or amitriptyline (Elavil) (Taragano et al. 1997). They were treated with fixed doses of drug for 45 days, at which time both groups had a mean drop of 9.4 points in Ham-D scores. However, half the amitriptyline group had dropped out by the end of the study, as had 22% of the fluoxetine-treated group. It should be noted that most of the trials cited above included patients with mild depressive symptoms and not major depression. On the basis of these findings, should mildly to moderately depressed persons with Alzheimer’s disease be prescribed psychotropic medication? Despite the weak evidence for drug effect, it is clear that efforts at active treatment are followed by improvement in a substantial proportion of cases. It therefore seems reasonable to offer treatment with medications that are minimally toxic as part of an overall approach to patients’ symptoms. Based on the response time of elderly depressed persons who are cognitively intact, an adequate trial of antidepressant medication should be from 8 to 12 weeks. If tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs) are used, blood levels should be obtained no later than 4 weeks after starting medications, and patients should be monitored closely for anticholinergic side effects, confusion, and syncope. (Georgotas et al. 1987). When psychotic features such as hallucinations and delusions accompany depression, patients may be treated with both neuroleptic and antidepressant agents. The drugs may be started simultaneously or the
224
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
antipsychotic might be started first, followed in 2 weeks by the antidepressant. The psychotic features associated with depression tend to be mood congruent. Common delusions include financial destitution, persecution for past misdeeds, or impending prosecution or execution. Somatic delusions occur, in which patients imagine themselves to be riddled with cancer or filled with feces. The delusions of depression are distinguishable from those of a delusional disorder in their reflection of the patient’s overall pessimistic attitude. Profoundly anorexic or suicidal persons should be hospitalized, either in a closely supervised general medical unit accustomed to managing depressed patients or in a psychiatric unit. Cognitively impaired elderly depressed patients have been found to require longer periods of inpatient treatment and to require neuroleptics more often than do cognitively intact elderly depressed patients (LaRue et al. 1986). There are no studies that specifically address psychotic depression in dementing illness. It is our practice to start psychotically depressed patients first on a neuroleptic drug. Physically frail persons are generally treated with small doses of high-potency or atypical neuroleptics, such as 0.5–4.0 mg/day of haloperidol (Haldol) administered orally twice daily, 0.5–1.5 mg of risperidone (Risperdal) orally twice daily, or olanzapine (Zyprexa) 2.5–10 mg at bedtime, the lower dose being the usual starting dose. High-potency neuroleptics are employed because they are not cardiotoxic, have little anticholinergic activity, and produce little orthostatic hypotension. Atypical neurololeptics are used because of their low potential for causing extrapyramidal symptoms. Neuroleptics prescribed by the authors are listed in Table 7–1. Neuroleptic side effects are listed in Table 7–2. To further treat the depressive symptoms in psychotic depression, or as the primary drug treatment in nonpsychotic major depression, the classes of drugs first employed are the serotonin reuptake inhibitors and the atypical antidepressants (see later in this chapter).
Electroconvulsive Therapy If melancholic or psychotically depressed patients do not respond to drug treatment and continue to present a severe danger to themselves in terms of suicide, dehydration, or starvation, electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) becomes an important consideration (reviewed in Sackeim 1998). ECT is superior to combined antidepressant-neuroleptic treatment in melancholic or psychotically depressed patients (Buchan et al. 1992). However, transient post-ictal confusion is greater in severity and duration in cognitively impaired persons than in cognitively intact persons of similar age, requiring that a patient be monitored for several hours after a treatment (Nelson and Rosenberg 1991).
TABLE 7–1.
Dosages of neuroleptic drugs Trade name
Chlorpromazine Trifluoperazine Thiothixene Haloperidol Clozapine Risperidone Olanzapine Quetiapine
Thorazine Stelazine Navane Haldol Clozaril Risperdal Zyprexa Seroquel
Smallest tablet (mg) Liquid 10 1 1 0.5a 25a 0.25 2.5 25
Adult dosage (mg/day)
10 mg/5 mL 10 mg/mL 5 mg/mL 2 mg/mL
Geriatric dosage (mg/day)
200–400 10–20 10–20 4–8 100–900 1–6 5–20 50–500
1 mg/mL
10–300 4–20 4–20 0.25–2 6.25–25 0.25–1.5 2.5–10 25–400
a
Scored tablet.
TABLE 7–2.
Neuroleptic side effects Class
Chlorpromazine Trifluoperazine Thiothixene Haloperidol Clozapine Risperidone Olanzapine Quetiapine
Phenothiazine (aliphatic) Phenothiazine (piperazine) Thioxanthene (piperazine) Butyrophenone Tricyclic benzodiazepine Benzisoxazole Thienobenzodiazepine Dibenzothiazepine
Note.
Sedation
Hypotension
Extrapyramidal symptoms
Anticholinergic effects
++++ + ++ + ++++ + ++ ++
++++ ++ ++ + +++ + ++ -
++ +++ +++ ++++ ++ + -
++++ ++ ++ ++++ + +++ -
+=minimal; ++=mild; +++=moderate; ++++=marked; -=none.
225
Generic name
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
Generic name
226
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors Serotoninergic abnormalities appear to be an important part of the pathophysiology of major depression (reviewed in Maes and Meltzer 1995). Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) prolong the action of serotonin at the synaptic cleft. This class of drugs includes fluoxetine (Prozac and others), sertraline (Zoloft), paroxetine (Paxil), and citalopram (Celexa). This is the most favored class of antidepressants in elders because of its relative freedom from life-threatening or unpleasant side effects and once-aday dosing, leading to greater compliance with treatment regimens (Mourilhe and Stokes 1998). These drugs are administered in the morning because of their mild stimulating effects. Transient increase in anxiety, sleeplessness, loss of appetite, diarrhea, and sexual dysfunction (inhibited orgasm or ejaculation, loss of libido) may occur with all drugs of this class. Fluoxetine is virtually devoid of anticholinergic, antihistaminic, or antiadrenergic side effects (Harris and Benfield 1995). It has been widely used in elders, but it has been associated with weight loss in nursing home patients (Brymer and Winograd 1992) and with the syndrome of inappropriate antidiuretic hormone (ADH) secretion (Druckenbrod and Mulsant 1994). Sleep disturbance and increased agitation have also been reported. Its long half-life (1–3 days), its active metabolite, and its potent inhibition of cytochrome P450 (CYP) 2D6 and intermediate inhibition of CYP 3A4 can result in drug accumulation. Therefore, low doses or less than daily dosing should be considered in treating very frail elders. Fluoxetine is administered at a dosage of 5–20 mg/day, but this may be reduced if it is overly stimulating by giving one capsule every other day or fluoxetine concentrated solution. It is the only SSRI available in the United States in a generic formulation. Sertraline has a 24-hour half-life. It is a weak inhibitor of CYP 3A4 and CYP 2D6. Transient nausea may occur with its use. The drug appears to be well tolerated by elders (Cohn et al. 1990). When sertraline (50–150 mg/ day) was compared with nortriptyline (25–100 mg/day) or fluoxetine (20 mg/day), it was of at least equal efficacy for depression and was associated with better or equal cognitive function (Bondareff et al. 2000; Newhouse et al. 2000). It has been reported to improve mood and decrease food refusal in persons with advanced Alzheimer’s disease and depressed affect (Volicer et al. 1994). Dosing in elderly outpatients is either 50 or 100 mg, starting at 50 mg; in frail elders dosing begins at 25 mg/day. There is little evidence that doses greater than 100 mg/day are more effective than doses of 50–100 mg. Dose escalation may be required over time. Paroxetine has mild anticholinergic effects. At therapeutic plasma concentrations, it has about one-fifth the anticholinergic potential in serum of
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
227
nortriptyline (Pollock et al. 1998), but it is otherwise comparable to other SSRIs. It has been used successfully in depressed elders (Schone and Ludwig 1993), but it should be discontinued gradually because of potential withdrawal symptoms (Mulsant et al. 1997). Citalopram is the most serotonin-selective of the SSRIs (Hyttel 1994). A dose of 10–30 mg produced significantly more improvement after 4 weeks on the depression subscale of the Gottfries-Brane-Steen dementia rating scale than did placebo treatment in a small group of patients with either Alzheimer’s disease or vascular dementia (Nyth and Gottfries 1990). However, these patients were not specifically diagnosed with depression; they were included in the study because of overall behavioral disturbance. In a later double-blind study, as mentioned earlier, citalopram was only marginally superior to placebo (Nyth et al. 1992). The drug is well tolerated in elders and is initiated at 20 mg/day.
Serotonin Syndrome The serotonin syndrome (Insel et al. 1982; reviewed in P.S. Keck and Arnold 2000), arises after the use of agents that prolong the activity of serotonin by blocking its reuptake into neurons or blocking its oxidation. It is a rare complication, but it is probably more common when combinations of these agents are employed. Combinations of monoamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs) and tricyclic antidepressants, MAOIs and SSRIs, and MAOIs and venlafaxine (Effexor) are the most commonly reported causes. When full-blown, the serotonin syndrome includes mental status changes, agitation or restlessness, myoclonus, hyperreflexia, diaphoresis, tremor, shivering, incoordination, autonomic dysfunction, hyperthermia, and muscular rigidity. Myoglobinuria and renal failure may result, as may seizures or disseminated intravascular clotting. The serotonin syndrome resembles the neuroleptic malignant syndrome (see below) and is difficult to differentiate, especially in persons receiving combinations of serotoninergic and neuroleptic drugs. Treatment of a mild serotonin syndrome entails withdrawal of the precipitating drug. In more severe cases, intravenous fluids and the use of the serotonin antagonist cyproheptadine (Periactin) 4–24 mg/day may be useful (P.S. Keck and Arnold 2000).
SSRI Withdrawal Withdrawal symptoms are common when SSRIs are discontinued, even when they are tapered slowly. Such symptoms tend to be more common with SSRIs with shorter half-lives, such as paroxetine, than with sertraline or fluoxetine. The symptoms persist up to 21 days and most commonly
228
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
include dizziness, lethargy, paresthesia, nausea, vivid dreams, irritability, and lowered mood. They are relieved, usually within 24 hours, by restarting the medication (Coupland et al. 1996) and are not relieved by benzodiazepines.
Heterocyclic Antidepressants Trazodone (Desyrel), a triazolopyridine, has specific but complex effects on the serotoninergic system and has little effect on the dopamine, histamine, and cholinergic systems. Its sedative and hypotensive effects (due to =-adrenergic antagonism) make it unsuitable for treating depression in elders because of the high doses required for an antidepressant effect, but it is commonly used in low doses (25–50 mg three times a day) for the management of agitation in dementia patients (Calkin et al. 1999). Nefazodone (Serzone), a trazodone congener, blocks serotonin type 2 receptors, inhibits serotonin reuptake, and also produces mild =-adrenergic blockade. It strongly inhibits CYP 34A, and therefore it interferes with the metabolism of many drugs, including triazolam (Halcion), midazolam (Versed), alprazolam (Xanax), and cisapride (Propulsid) (Nemeroff et al. 1996). Like trazodone, it is sedating, especially at the higher effective antidepressant doses. For all of these reasons, it should probably not be used to treat depression in elders. Venlafaxine (Effexor) and sustained-release (SR) venlafaxine (EffexorXR) inhibits the neuronal reuptake of serotonin, norepinephrine, and (to a lesser extent) dopamine. It has no affinity for cholinergic, histaminergic, or adrenergic receptors (Muth et al. 1986). The drug has been used effectively in elders (Dierick 1996). Its principal side effects are nausea and dry mouth, but it may increase blood pressure. The short half-life of venlafaxine requires twice-daily dosage. Starting dosage is 37.5 mg twice a day; the maximum dosage in elders is probably 150 mg twice a day. An extended-release form is available that may be dosed once a day beginning with 37.5 mg. In younger patients the effective dosage range is between 75 and 225 mg/day, with most patients receiving less than 150 mg/day. The drug should be tapered slowly when discontinued because of its potential for unpleasant withdrawal symptoms. Bupropion (Wellbutrin) does not significantly affect norepinephrine or serotonin reuptake; it inhibits dopamine reuptake; and it has no affinity for histaminic, adrenergic, or cholinergic receptors. It requires twice-daily or three times daily dosing, beginning with 100 mg twice a day. In its SR formulation it may be administered at an initial dosage of 100 mg each morning. Dosages of bupropion SR of 100–300 mg/day were found to be as effective as paroxetine 10–40 mg/day in the treatment of older (age 60+
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
229
years) outpatients with major depression (Weihs et al. 2000). The side effects of bupropion include headache, dry mouth, dizziness, weight loss, tremor, and nausea, but the drug does not often produce sexual dysfunction (Segraves et al. 2000). Seizures have been reported in dosages above 450 mg/day (J. Davidson 1989). Mirtazapine (Remeron) facilitates norepinephrine and serotonin release through blockade of presynaptic =2-adrenergic autoreceptors (de Boer 1996). Its antihistaminic properties make it quite sedating. The only published report on mirtazapine in elders (age 65+) is pooled data from seven randomized, placebo-controlled studies in which 30 elders were enrolled and 19 completed their studies (Bremner 1999). The most prominent side effects were drowsiness and dry mouth. Neutropenia and agranulocytosis have been reported with this drug (Mulsant et al. 1997). It is initiated at 15 mg at bedtime and may be increased to a maximum dosage of 45 mg at bedtime.
Tricyclic Antidepressants The TCAs include tertiary amines such as amitriptyline, imipramine, doxepin, and clomipramine, and secondary amines such as desipramine (Norpramin) and nortriptyline (Pamelor, Aventyl). These drugs are more toxic in elders because of their prominent anticholinergic, hypotensive, sedative, and cardiac conduction delaying effects. Their metabolism is also affected by age, with decreased protein binding, slowed demethylation in the liver, and reduced hepatic and renal clearance. Amitriptyline and doxepin block serotonin reuptake strongly. Imipramine and nortriptyline produce moderate norepinephrine and serotonin blockade, whereas desipramine strongly blocks norepinephrine reuptake. Elders are more susceptible than younger persons to the therapeutic and toxic effects of these drugs because of decreases in brain norepinephrine, serotonin, and (in Alzheimer’s disease patients) acetylcholine. The tertiary amines are essentially contraindicated in frail elders or individuals with marked cholinergic deficit such as that resulting from Alzheimer’s disease or dementia with Lewy bodies. Secondary amines have fewer side effects and are less sedating than the tertiary amines. Desipramine (or desmethylimipramine), the demethylated metabolite of imipramine, has the least—but still considerable—anticholinergic effects of the tricyclic antidepressants and is the most specifically noradrenergic. It produces anticholinergic side effects and postural hypotension in some patients, and it may cause agitation. Nortriptyline, the demethylated metabolite of amitriptyline, is low in anticholinergic and sedative side effects compared with the tertiary amines (Gerson et al. 1988). Even in frail elders, plasma levels correlate well with dosage. It has a ther-
230
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
apeutic window of 60–260 ng/mL in plasma, but plasma levels in elders should not exceed 150 ng/mL (Alexopoulos and Salzman 1998). In a group of depressed patients with an average age of 84, a dosage averaging 80 mg/ day was required to produce a plasma level of 100 ng/mL (Katz et al. 1989).
Side Effects The common side effects of TCAs are sedation, orthostatic hypotension, cardiac toxicity, and anticholinergic toxicity (see Table 7–4). Orthostatic hypotension usually manifests as dizziness and often leads to falls in elders. Slowing of cardiac conduction makes TCAs relatively to absolutely contraindicated in persons with bundle-branch disease, heart block, or cardiac arrhythmias. Tricyclics have quinidine-like effects and are also contraindicated in persons taking quinidine or procainamide. The hydroxy metabolites of nortriptyline contribute to both therapeutic effects and cardiovascular effects, such that plasma nortriptyline levels that appear normal may be misleading. Therefore, it may be wise to obtain hydroxynortriptyline levels when available or monitor older patients by electrocardiography for widening QT or QRS intervals. Anticholinergic side effects include dry mouth, constipation, urinary retention, precipitation or aggravation of narrow-angle glaucoma, impairment of memory, and delirium. Dry mouth may result in clinically significant xerostomia affecting gingiva and dentition, leading to candidiasis or parotitis or excessive water intake and hyponatremia. Anticholinergic delirium. The primary treatment of anticholinergic delirium is discontinuing the causative drug. Although they are reversible by physostigmine (Weiner and Davis 1986), the potential toxic effects of cholinergic excess require that physostigmine be used in well-supervised medical environments. It is less dangerous to treat agitated anticholinergic delirium in frail or ill persons with a high-potency neuroleptic; lowpotency neuroleptics might aggravate anticholinergic delirium. Recently, practitioners have been treating mild anticholinergic toxicity with starting doses of donepezil (Aricept) or rivastigmine (Exelon), but the evidence for efficacy is limited only to clinical reports.
Administration and Dosage Table 7–3 presents the dosage and therapeutic blood levels of TCAs that can be used in elders; Table 7–4 presents their side effects. Before administering these drugs, a careful check must be made of cardiac status and blood pressure. An electrocardiogram and standing and sitting blood pressure readings should be obtained.
TABLE 7–3.
Antidepressant dosages and therapeutic ranges Trade name
Desipramine Nortriptyline Paroxetine Sertraline Fluoxetine Citalopram Mirtazapine Venlafaxine Bupropion Phenelzine Tranylcypromine
Norpramin, Pertofrane Pamelor Paxil Zoloft Prozac Celexa Remeron Effexor Wellbutrin Nardil Parnate
Starting dosage (mg/day)
Adult dosage (mg/day)
Geriatric dosage (mg/day)
Therapeutic range
25 25 10 25–50 10–20 20 15 75 75 15 10
75–200 50–150 20–40 50–200 20–40 20–40 15–30 75–225 75–450 30–60 20–30
25–75 25–100 10–40 25–100 10–40 20–40 15–30 37.5–225 75–225 15–60 5–15
50–150 ng/mL 50–100 ng/mL N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
Generic name
231
232
TABLE 7–4.
Antidepressant neurotransmitter and side effects at therapeutic doses Reuptake inhibition
Generic name
Norepinephrine
Sedation
Hypotension
Anticholinergic effects
Cardiotoxicity
+ ++ ++++ ++++ ++++ ++++ +++ ++++ + MAO inhibitor MAO inhibitor
++++ +++ ++++ +++ +
+ ++ +++ + + + -
+ + + +++ ++
+ ++ + + -
+ ++ -
Note. MAO=monoamine oxidase; +=minimal ; ++=mild ; +++=moderate ; ++++=marked ; -=none. Source. Adapted from Cohen 1999.
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Desipramine Nortriptyline Sertraline Paroxetine Fluoxetine Citalopram Mirtazapine Venlafaxine Bupropion Phenelzine Tranylcypromine
Serotonin
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
233
Because of a strong correlation between antidepressant effect and plasma concentration of secondary amine TCAs (see Table 7–3), an attempt could be made to reach a plasma level between 100–150 ng/mL. Beyond these levels, therapeutic improvement is unlikely. Nortriptyline has a therapeutic window at 50–150 ng/mL, documented largely in inpatients with major depression. The lower therapeutic blood level range for elderly patients has not been established; elders tend to have clinical responses at low doses, although there is a still a threshold of about 30–40 ng/mL. The best starting dosage for elders is 10–25 mg at bedtime. Nortriptyline and desipramine may stimulate, and therefore they may be best given as a single morning dose. If there are no significant side effects, dosage of desipramine can be increased by 10–25 mg every 20 days to a maximum of 100–150 mg/ day, and nortriptyline may be increased to 75–100 mg/day.
Monoamine Oxidase Inhibitors The enzyme monoamine oxidase (MAO), which degrades norepinephrine, serotonin, and dopamine, increases with advancing age (Robinson 1975). Inhibiting MAO activity increases the availability of norepinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin. The most common side effect of these drugs is orthostatic hypotension. Less common are stimulation, dry mouth, and weight gain. Because MAOIs interfere with their metabolism, pressor amines and sympathomimetic drugs may not be used during MAO treatment. The pressor amine tyramine in foods must also be avoided by eliminating from the diet aged cheeses, yeast extract, caviar, sausage, herring, beef and chicken liver, and large amounts of avocado, chocolate, caffeine, and Chianti wine. MAOIs may have a significant adverse interaction with SSRIs and should not be used with them. Between 2 and 5 weeks should elapse between use of an SSRI and commencement of an MAOI, and 2 weeks should elapse between a switch from an MAOI to an SSRI (Small and Salzman 1998). The two commonly used drugs in this class are phenelzine (Nardil) and tranylcypromine (Parnate). In elders, phenelzine is prescribed in an initial dose of 7.5 mg, raising the dose every 3–4 days to a maximum of 15–60 mg/ day in divided doses. Tranylcypromine, which appears to be more stimulating, is started in elders at 2.5–5 mg/day and increased to 5–15 mg/day in divided doses. Because of the dietary restrictions, frequent drug-drug interactions, and pronounced postural hypotension in many individuals, these are drugs of last resort in elders. Both drugs are irreversible, nonselective MAOIs that inhibit both MAO A and MAO B, thus enhancing both serotonin and dopamine. These medications are more likely associated with hypotension, serotonin syndrome, and hypertensive episodes associated with tyramine intake.
234
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Stimulants The use of stimulants such as dextroamphetamine (Dexedrine), methylphenidate (Ritalin), or pemoline (Cylert) in depressed elders has been essentially abandoned. There is not a great risk of abuse, but they can overstimulate, induce or exaggerate paranoid thinking, increase blood pressure and heart rate, and interact adversely with antihypertensive medications. Withdrawal is often followed by aggravation of depressive symptoms. Special care must be taken with patients having a history of psychosis or bipolar disorder and in the face of severe congestive heart failure or significant hypertension. The use of these drugs to treat apathy and anergy is discussed later in this chapter.
Delirium The most important aspect of treatment for delirium is dealing with the underlying medical disorder, which may be pneumonia, congestive heart failure, or a urinary tract infection. Environmental and interpersonal measures are the first to be instituted in dealing with the emotional, cognitive, and behavioral aspects of delirium. Patients should be kept in well-lighted rooms so that they are not confused by shadows or poor illumination. The room should have a window so that the person can be oriented to day and night. Staff members should constantly reintroduce themselves, briefly describe their role (“I am your nurse”) and mention the place, the day, and the reason why the person is there (“This is the Intensive Care Unit at Memorial Hospital; today is Wednesday. You had open heart surgery three days ago.”). If possible, familiar persons with whom the delirious person has a positive relationship and familiar objects (photographs of loved ones, a pillow, a comforter, or a stuffed animal) should be in the room. Mechanical restraint is to be avoided in favor of distracting the delirious person from pulling out nasogastric tubing, indwelling catheters, and so on. Delirium that occurs quietly and without agitation requires no psychotropic medication. Psychotropic drugs are used when delirious persons become difficult to manage (e.g., refusing to remain in bed, pulling out catheters and intravenous tubes, or attacking staff members and family). Neuroleptic medications are the treatment of choice except in withdrawal delirium. When agitation is mild, these medications should be given orally. When there is danger to life, medication should be administered parenterally. A relatively safe drug to use in agitated delirium is haloperidol, which may be administered intravenously or intramuscularly if needed. An adequate oral, intravenous, or intramuscular dose for an agitated, physically frail person ranges
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
235
from 0.5 to 2 mg. A physically strong person may require up to 5 mg intravenously or intramuscularly as often as every 30–60 minutes, and large doses are well tolerated for a short term. As soon as delirious patients calm down (usually within 24 hours), the neuroleptic medication may be administered orally at twice the total daily dose of the parenterally administered drug, given in divided doses. Thus, if 10 mg intravenous haloperidol was required the first day, 20 mg would be administered as 5 mg four times a day or 10 mg twice a day. Typically, 3–4 days of a gradually lowered dosage of neuroleptic medication are required. The chief complication of this technique of rapid administration of neuroleptic medication is acute dystonia—a dramatic, painful tonic contraction of neck, tongue, and mouth muscles. Oculogyric crisis may occur, as may contraction of large muscle groups. Acute dystonia is treated by the intravenous administration of diphenhydramine (Benadryl) 25 mg or benztropine maleate (Cogentin) 1 mg intravenously (Baldessarini 1977), followed by discontinuation of the offending drug. It is wise to follow the intravenous use of these medications with benztropine maleate 1 mg twice a day for several days, because neuroleptics are long acting. A very different result was obtained with lorazepam as a sole treatment for delirium in medically hospitalized persons with acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS) (Breitbart et al. 1996). In this study, planned as a double-blind prospective comparison of treatment with haloperidol and lorazepam, all six subjects in the lorazepam arm of the study developed untoward side effects—including oversedation, disinhibition, ataxia, and increased confusion—leading to refusal to continue the drug or requiring discontinuation of the drug. By contrast, low-dose haloperidol was effective in reducing symptoms of delirium. Nevertheless, in the case of severe agitated delirium, and when it is essential to achieve sedation (e.g., for patients who are struggling against physical restraints), haloperidol may be combined with lorazepam in a ratio ranging from 3:1 to 5:1 and administered intravenously in the same syringe. If delirium is due to withdrawal from minor tranquilizers, barbiturates, or hypnotics, pentobarbital (Nembutal) may be used in doses of 200–400 mg every 4–6 hours sufficient to cause nystagmus, slurred speech, drowsiness, and/or mild ataxia within an hour after drug administration. When an adequate 24-hour dose is established, it is maintained for several days and then is reduced by approximately one-tenth of the total dose (but not more than 100 mg) per day. Barbiturates are used in preference to benzodiazepines, because it is essential to use a drug that is cross-tolerant with the drug that is being withdrawn from. Delirium tremens is usually manifested initially by diaphoresis and tremulousness. It should be treated in elders with short-acting benzodiaz-
236
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
epines such as oxazepam (Serax) 15–30 mg up to four times a day or lorazepam 1 mg up to four times a day. An intramuscular or intravenous dose of 1 mg of lorazepam may be used for severe agitation. An oral dose of 1 mg of lorazepam every 1–4 hours may then be administered. Patients with alcohol dependence, alcohol withdrawal, or delirium tremens also require treatment with thiamine 100 mg twice a day orally or parenterally, especially if intravenous dextrose solutions are employed. Inadequate stores of thiamine to metabolize a sugar load can produce an acute, potentially lethal Wernicke’s syndrome (Adams and Victor 1989). Too-rapid hydration with lowering of serum sodium below 130 mEq/L can produce central pontine myelinolysis (Adams et al. 1959). As pointed out in Chapter 1, delirium is often superimposed on preexisting cognitive impairment. Cognitively impaired persons are easily made delirious by mild metabolic and toxic insults, including introduction of psychotropic agents. Furthermore, preexisting dementia is a strong predictor of postoperative delirium (Francis 1992). Often, as in the Lewy body variant of Alzheimer’s disease, delirium is the first presentation of the dementing disorder. Delirium superimposed on dementia may respond to very small doses of haloperidol (0.25–1 mg orally or intravenously). In elders, intravenous administration is preferable to intramuscular injections to avoid tissue damage.
Anxiety Many individuals with dementing illness complain of anxiety symptoms. Early in the course of a dementing illness, individuals often express fearfulness, particularly fear that they are losing their minds. Later on, patients are observed to appear frightened and to have rapid pulse, but they lack the ability to communicate in words their emotional state. It is frequently impossible to tell if a restless, agitated patient has the subjective experience of anxiety or if these behaviors are due to confusion.
Minor Tranquilizers Minor tranquilizers of the benzodiazepine class (Table 7–5) are drugs of choice for anxiety disorders in cognitively intact patients. There have been few controlled studies of benzodiazepines in cognitively impaired persons. Shorter-acting drugs such as oxazepam or lorazepam have fewer side effects and produce less sedation than longer-acting drugs such as chlordiazepoxide (Librium) or diazepam (Valium). In a comparison of chlordiazepoxide, oxazepam, and placebo in elderly patients, oxazepam (with
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
TABLE 7–5.
Dosages of benzodiazepine tranquilizers
Generic name
Trade name
Chlordiazepoxide Diazepam Oxazepam Lorazepam Alprazolam
Librium Valium Serax Ativan Xanax
Note.
237
Adult dosage (mg/day) 20–40 10–20 45–60 2–4 2–4
Geriatric Frequency Frequency dosage of dosage in of dosage (mg/day) adults in elders 20–40 2–20 15–60 0.5–4 0.5–2
tid tid tid tid tid
bid qd/qod tid bid/tid bid/tid
tid=three times a day; bid=twice a day; qd=every day; qod=every other day.
76% of patients improved) was superior to chlordiazepoxide (23% improved) and placebo (7% improved), but half of the chlordiazepoxide group was oversedated, ataxic, or dizzy (Chesrow et al. 1965). Diazepam improved daytime restlessness in dementia patients, but 85% appeared oversedated (DeLamos et al. 1965). Because of their tendency to cloud sensorium, impair memory, and produce ataxia, benzodiazepines should be avoided in cognitively impaired persons. In addition, there is concern that they may produce a disinhibition syndrome of rage or agitation. On the other hand, many mildly to moderately impaired persons have been taking such medications for years, and discontinuing the medication results in further increases of agitation that are not well relieved by other classes of drugs. In such instances, the wisest course of action is to determine the smallest dosage of benzodiazepine needed to control the patient’s anxiety and to maintain the patient on that dosage. The side effects of benzodiazepines are sedation, memory impairment, and muscular incoordination leading to dysarthria and ataxia. Withdrawal symptoms occur frequently, especially with triazolam and alprazolam. Therefore, if benzodiazepine withdrawal is undertaken, it should be done slowly to prevent severe withdrawal symptoms, including gastrointestinal upset, tremor, agitation, and (occasionally, after abrupt cessation of high doses) seizures. There do not seem to be important age differences in benzodiazepine withdrawal. A study contrasting withdrawal (reduction of dose by 25% per week) in elderly chronic benzodiazepine users and in young adults showed somewhat lower symptoms in the older group (Schweizer et al. 1989). Because many cognitively impaired persons are extremely susceptible to the extrapyramidal effects of neuroleptics, it may be necessary to consider using benzodiazepines and other drugs. Because oxidation may be impaired in elders, benzodiazepines that are metabolized by conjugation, such as oxazepam, lorazepam, and temazepam, are preferred.
238
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Buspirone Buspirone (BuSpar), a serotonin type 1A agonist, was found to be more effective than placebo for relief of anxiety in elders at 5 mg three times a day (Robinson et al. 1990); in a large open-label study it appeared to be well tolerated by elders receiving treatment for other medical conditions (Bohm et al. 1990). The drug has a long latency to onset of action (weeks), requires three-times-daily dosing, and often requires titration to doses above 40 mg/day. Furthermore, it does not appear to be effective in persons who have previously been treated with benzodiazepines. A multicenter trial for agitated long-term-care residents was done, but the results were not reported. In our experience, buspirone is of little use in calming cognitively impaired persons.
Agitation Agitation is a term that encompasses a host of disturbed and disturbing behaviors. The formal definition of agitation, introduced by Cohen-Mansfield and Billig (1986), was based on observation of residents of a long-termcare facility. It includes “inappropriate verbal, vocal, or motor activities not explained by apparent needs or confusion” (p. 712). So defined, most agitated behaviors are unrelated to syndromes such as delirium, depression, and psychosis and are not the products of undetected painful conditions or systemic illness; nevertheless, care must be taken to rule out these possible etiologies of agitation before treatment is undertaken. Agitated behaviors such as restlessness and violence occur in a substantial minority of cognitively impaired persons. Nondirected violent or aggressive behavior is more common than specifically directed violence. However, verbal and physical violence is often precipitated by an interpersonal event or confrontation or by misperception due to a patient’s cognitive impairment. There have been few randomized, placebo-controlled pharmacotherapeutic studies of agitated behaviors in cognitively impaired persons. Some have been reviewed previously (Schneider and Sobin 1994; Schneider et al. 1990). The following information is largely from controlled studies, case reports, and expert physician evaluations. Classes of drugs used for the treatment of symptomatic behaviors include minor tranquilizers, neuroleptics, lithium, anticonvulsants, b-adrenergic blockers, and serotoninergic agents. Before discussing specific drugs for the management of agitation, it is important to present and address the results of a trial that appeared to show
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
239
lack of efficacy of either psychotropic drugs or behavioral therapy of agitated behaviors in community-dwelling patients with Alzheimer’s disease (Teri et al. 2000). In this study, 149 persons with probable Alzheimer’s disease were randomized to treatment with haloperidol up to 3 mg/day (mean dose, 1.8 mg/day), trazodone up to 300 mg/day (mean dose, 200 mg/day), behavioral intervention, or placebo, and treated for 16 weeks. Although 34% of patients improved in relation to their baseline on a global assessment scale, there was no difference in efficacy between groups. Average drug dosage seemed appropriate, but dosage was reduced when significant side effects (such as sedation or mild extrapyramidal symptoms) manifested. Thus, the cost of drug efficacy may be willingness to tolerate unpleasant side effects. For example, Lanctot et al. (2000) found that the efficacy rate of antipsychotics was equivalent to the side-effect rate. It is possible that in the study by Teri et al. (2000) a so-called floor effect occurred—that is, patients did not have sufficiently severe symptoms to show a drug effect—and that efficacy might have been shown had the subjects been more behaviorally disturbed. However, there may have been considerable selection bias in that investigators who contributed subjects to the study might have been unwilling to enroll subjects whom they did not feel should risk taking placebo or being in behavioral treatment for a period as long as 16 weeks. There are many ways to interpret this study; the two most important ways are that in a substantial number of patients, agitation improves spontaneously, and that there is a strong placebo effect. The latter may be mediated by direct interaction with patients and through the effect on caregivers.
Minor Tranquilizers Minor tranquilizers have been used as a sole or adjunctive treatment for agitation in dementing illness; however, a survey of studies revealed neuroleptics to be consistently more effective than benzodiazepines (Schneider and Sobin 1994). On the other hand, a prospective study of clonazepam in geropsychiatric inpatients, half of whom had dementing illnesses, showed significant improvement on a number of behavioral measures at an average dose of 1.2 mg/day for a minimum of 2 weeks (Calkin et al. 1997). Of the 24 patients studied, only 3 discontinued the drug: 2 because it was no longer needed, and the third because of sedation and confusion. There was no difference in improvement rate between demented and nondemented groups. Christensen and Benfield (1998) compared low-dose alprazolam (Xanax) with low-dose haloperidol for agitation and behavioral disturbance in 68 residents of long-term-care facilities. Subject inclusion criteria were
240
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
use of haloperidol at a dose of 1 mg/day or less and having a DSM-III-R diagnosis of organic mental disorder. Patients were assigned blindly to continuation of their regular dose of haloperidol or to alprazolam 0.5 mg twice a day for 6 weeks, with conditions reversed for another 6 weeks. The mean dose of haloperidol was 0.64 mg/day (range, 0.1–1.0 mg). The 48 completers of the study showed no difference in the number of behavioral episodes per week between the haloperidol and alprazolam conditions. Given the high dropout rate and the low dosage of haloperidol, it is difficult to come to any conclusion concerning the effectiveness of alprazolam from this study. A retrospective chart review evaluated the effects of lorazepam and divalproex sodium (Depakote) in 146 elderly long-term-care residents with dementia and behavioral disturbances (Frenchman et al. 2000). Residents were rated as either improved or unimproved based on chart notes by physicians and nurses. There was a 31% improvement rate in the lorazepamtreated group compared with 57% in the divalproex-treated group (P<0.05). Falls were frequent (41%) in the lorazepam-treated group, in which the range of dosage was 0.25–3 mg/day, with an average maintenance dose of 1.32 mg/day. Falls were rare with divalproex, for which the dose range was 125–2,000 mg/day, with an average maintenance dose of 844 mg/day. Our own clinical experience is that benzodiazepines frequently increase confusion in cognitively impaired persons, and in elders they significantly increase the risk of falls. As a consequence, we use short-acting benzodiazepines such as lorazepam (0.5–1.0 mg orally or intramuscularly) as occasional as-needed medication for acute agitation, being careful to observe patients afterward for gait disturbance.
Conventional or “Typical” Antipsychotics The class of neuroleptics known as conventional or typical antipsychotics is characterized by blockade of dopamine type 2 (D2) receptors and by the frequent development of extrapyramidal symptoms due to nigrostriatal D2 blockade. In the past, they have been the drugs of choice for agitated, restless behavior not related to major depression or an abstinence syndrome, but their efficacy seems unrelated to their antipsychotic effect. Given in a single daily dose at bedtime, they provide some degree of sedation or sleep induction, and reduction of daytime agitation. Higher-potency neuroleptics are useful for persons who are sensitive to the anticholinergic effects, sedation, or postural hypotension produced by these medications. Lowpotency neuroleptics have been used in healthy adults when sedation was needed or if patients were susceptible to the extrapyramidal side effects of
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
241
the high-potency neuroleptics. The dosage ranges and side-effect profiles for a representative group of these drugs are presented in Tables 7–1 and 7–2. Care needs to be taken with these drugs in persons with prolonged electrocardiographic QTc intervals (Gury et al. 2000). Unlike the antidepressants, plasma levels are not helpful in determining dosage. Dosage is determined by striking a balance between calming effects and side effects. These medications are effective by mouth, and with the exception of thioridazine (Mellaril), molindone (Moban), and most of the atypical antipsychotics, they can be administered parenterally. When severe agitation occurs, these medications may be given intramuscularly in approximately half the recommended adult or geriatric dose. Haloperidol (Haldol) may be administered intravenously at the intramuscular dose. Haloperidol, thioridazine, and thiothixene (Navane) have been found to be more effective than placebo for agitation and other symptomatic behaviors in dementia patients in randomized trials (Schneider et al. 1990). However, the manufacturer of thioridazine issued a warning letter on July 7, 2000 (Novartis Pharmaceuticals 2000), stating that thioridazine prolongation of the QTc interval makes it necessary to perform a baseline electrocardiogram and to measure serum potassium level before starting treatment with this drug and that thioridazine is contraindicated with fluvoxamine (Luvox), pindolol (Visken), propranolol (Inderal), and drugs that inhibit the P450 2D6 isoenzyme such as fluoxetine and paroxetine. In a double-blind study, haloperidol (2–16 mg/day; average, 7 mg) was compared with loxapine (10–80 mg/day; average, 36 mg) in 40 demented nursing home residents with aggressive behaviors (Carlyle et al. 1993). Of the 31 patients who completed the 4-week study, a positive response was seen in 81%, with no significant difference between loxapine and haloperidol. As would be expected at these very high doses, haloperidol-treated patients showed many more extrapyramidal symptoms than did loxapinetreated patients. In a more recent study, thiothixene was employed in 33 nursing home residents (mean age, 85 years) with dementia-related behavioral problems (Finkel et al. 1995). Before this trial, two-thirds were receiving neuroleptics. After a 1-week medication washout, subjects were treated with placebo or varying doses of thiothixene. They were treated for 11 weeks, followed by a 6-week crossover period. The average dose of thiothixene was 4.6 mg during the initial 11 weeks and 3.7 mg after crossover. No relationship was noted between dosage and clinical response. Of the thiothixene-treated patients, 69% showed an improvement of more than 5 points on the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (Cohen-Mansfield 1986) compared with 19% of patients taking placebo. One patient had to be dropped because of excessive sedation.
242
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Toxic Effects Neuroleptic drugs have a wide margin of safety in overdose (Baldessarini 1977), but they have a variety of toxic long- and short-term effects. For the low-potency neuroleptics, the prominent short-term toxic effects are antimuscarinic, antihistaminic, and anti–a-adrenergic, resulting in anticholinergic, sedative, and hypotensive side effects. For the higher-potency neuroleptics, the prominent short-term side effects are dystonias; parkinsonian symptoms; and akathisia, an involuntary, restless pacing. Dystonias are treated acutely as described previously. The best treatment for parkinsonian symptoms is lowering the dose of the neuroleptic drug. If that is insufficient, the dopamine agonist amantadine (Symmetrel) may be administered in a dosage ranging from 100 to 300 mg/day. It is preferable to benztropine (Cogentin), diphenhydramine (Benadryl), and trihexyphenidyl (Artane), which impair memory in elders because of their strong anticholinergic effects (Gelenberg et al. 1989; McEvoy et al. 1987). Akathisia responds best to lowering the dose of neuroleptic and may respond to doses of propranolol as low as 30–80 mg/day (Lipinski et al. 1984). Neuroleptic malignant syndrome. An extremely important toxic effect of neuroleptics is neuroleptic malignant syndrome (NMS) (Caroff 1980). This life-threatening complication occurs in approximately 0.9% of patients exposed to neuroleptics (P.E. Keck et al. 1987). NMS is characterized by alteration of consciousness, muscular rigidity, akinesia, fever, diaphoresis, and increased pulse and blood pressure. It may progress rapidly to coma (Pearlman 1986). Laboratory findings include elevated white blood cell count, elevated liver enzymes, markedly elevated creatinine phosphokinase (up to 20,000 U/dL), and myoglobin in plasma due to muscle tissue breakdown. The latter may result in acute renal failure. Symptoms evolve over 24–72 hours and are frequently related to an injection of a long-acting neuroleptic such as fluphenazine decanoate (Prolixin). Treatment is withdrawal of the neuroleptic agent, administration of intravenous fluids, and body cooling measures. Because the syndrome presumably arises due to intense dopaminergic blockade, the treatment of choice following supportive therapy is to counteract dopaminergic blockade and to achieve muscle relaxation (Granato et al. 1983). The dopamine agonist bromocriptine (Parlodel) is administered in 5-mg doses every 4 hours (orally or by nasogastric tube). Muscle relaxation is achieved by using the skeletal muscle relaxant dantrolene (Dantrium) 4–8 mg/kg daily by mouth in four divided doses. It may also be administered intravenously beginning with 1 mg/kg to a maximum cumulative dose of 10 mg/kg. Electroconvulsive therapy has been reported to be effective in more than 50 cases of NMS, but all reports are anecdotal (Trollor and Sachdev 1999).
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
243
Dystonia. Dystonias are slow, sustained muscular contractions or spasms commonly involving the neck and jaw; oculogyric crisis is also an example of an acute dystonia. Dystonias are often painful. They occur in 10%–30% of patients treated with typical antipsychotic drugs (Rupniak et al. 1986); their prevalence with atypical antipsychotics is not known, but they do occur with this class of drugs in elderly persons with dementia (Magnuson et al. 2000). The treatment is lowering the dose of or withdrawing the causative agent. Tardive dyskinesia syndromes. The tardive dyskinesia syndromes consist of involuntary movements appearing after a minimum of 3 months of antipsychotic treatment and for which alternate etiologies of dyskinesia have been excluded (Chouinard et al. 1979). Their principal determinants are prolonged exposure to antipsychotic medications and advancing age, but a primary psychiatric diagnosis of mood disorder and the presence of brain injury may also predispose (Marsden and Harrison 1972). Persons who have received neuroleptic medication for more than a year are estimated to develop tardive dyskinesia at a rate of 3% per year. Tardive dyskinesia syndromes may be reversible or irreversible, and they are frequently unmasked by antiparkinson medication. Jeste et al. (1999) studied elders exposed to conventional (typical) neuroleptics over 12 months. They found a 22% incidence among those previously unexposed to the drugs, 25% for those who received the drugs for 1–30 days, and 37% among those who had received the drugs for more than a month. Five principal tardive dyskinesia syndromes have been identified (R.J. Davis and Cummings 1988): classic tardive dyskinesia, tardive dystonia, tardive akathisia, tardive Gilles de la Tourette syndrome, and tardive complex (simultaneous presence of three or more movement disorders). Classic tardive dyskinesia consists of tic-like or choreiform movements. These movements usually begin with the tongue, facial, and neck muscles and involve arms and hands. Chewing movements of the mouth and sudden protrusions of the tongue are commonly seen. These movements, which are worsened by physical activity and emotional distress, disappear during sleep. Early facial dyskinesias are difficult to differentiate from the chewing movements that occur in elders who have not been exposed to antipsychotics (Varga et al. 1982). Tardive dystonias may display as wryneck, truncal muscle spasm, blepharospasm, oromandibular dystonia, or laryngospasm; tardive Tourette’s syndrome exhibits as involuntary coprolalia, palilalia, or echolalia. These syndromes are probably due to increased numbers of postsynaptic dopamine receptors in response to chronic dopaminergic blockade. For that reason, they often appear when antipsychotic dosage is reduced.
244
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Although no effective treatment is available, discontinuing the causative medication may result in reduction of symptoms after a period of months. When the medication cannot be safely discontinued, a small increase in dosage may ameliorate symptoms by increasing the dopaminergic blockade. Despite the fact that anticholinergics may unmask some cases of tardive dyskinesia, Gardos et al. (1986) found that antiparkinson drugs were helpful in cases of tardive dyskinesia with parkinsonian symptoms such as drooling, akathisia, and tremor. They also found benzodiazepines to be frequently useful, but they did not have success with lecithin, baclofen (Lioresal), propranolol, or levodopa (Sinemet). Based on the notion that tardive dyskinesia may be partly caused by free radical damage, trials of the antioxidant vitamin E have been undertaken, with modest success. Barak et al. (1998) reviewed 12 studies and found that vitamin E, in doses ranging from 400 to 1,600 IU/day, produced modest improvement in a small subset of persons (28.3%) with tardive dyskinesia. Olanzapine (Zyprexa) has also been reported to reduce symptoms of tardive dyskinesia (Littrell et al. 1998). Most persons with dementing illnesses do not require prolonged exposure to neuroleptics, because symptoms such as agitation frequently wax and wane. Efforts should be made every few months to reduce the dose of neuroleptics and to discontinue them if patients remain asymptomatic. Patients who require long-term neuroleptics should be assessed approximately every 3 months for evidence of tardive dyskinesia by using the Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale (AIMS) (Guy 1976) (see Appendix K) and by making serial comparisons.
Atypical Antipsychotics The atypical antipsychotics differ from the so-called typical antipsychotic drugs in their decreased propensity to produce extrapyramidal symptoms. This is their major advantage in treating persons with dementia. As with the typical antipsychotics, their impact on behavioral symptoms seems unrelated to their antipsychotic effect. Their antagonism of dopamine D2 receptors is probably modulated by their blockage of 5-hydroxytryptamine (serotonin) type 2 (5-HT2) receptors. The prototypical drug in this class is clozapine (Clozaril). Clozapine exhibits modest D2 receptor blockade, somewhat greater 5-HT2 blockade, and pronounced muscarinic type 1 (M1) blockade. The drug is highly sedating, highly anticholinergic, and cardiotoxic, and it often produces leukopenia (Salzman et al. 1995). Drug administration requires weekly complete blood counts. Because it is metabolized by CYP 1A2, it may interact with theophylline; caution is also required when it is used concomitantly with digoxin and warfarin. Clozapine is a last-resort medication; dosage is discussed under hallucinations.
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
245
Risperidone (Risperdal) has higher affinity for 5-HT2 than D2 receptors but lacks affinity for muscarinic receptors. In a double-blind, placebocontrolled trial, risperidone was found to be more effective than placebo for symptoms of psychosis and verbal or physical agitated behaviors associated with dementing illnesses (Katz et al. 1999). In this multicenter study of persons with severe dementia of various etiologies in long-term-care facilities, subjects were randomized to placebo or risperidone 0.5 mg/day, 1 mg/day, or 2 mg/day. The primary outcome measure was the Behavioral Pathology in Alzheimer’s Disease (BEHAVE-AD) rating scale (Reisberg et al. 1987). At the end point (12 weeks), treatment with 1 and 2 mg produced significantly greater reductions in BEHAVE-AD total scores and scores on the psychosis and aggression subscales. The effects on aggression were independent of sedation, psychosis, or extrapyramidal symptoms. In an open-label extension of this study (average of 230 days on drug), significant improvements were noted on the psychosis and aggression subscales of the BEHAVE-AD among patients taking 0.75–1.5 mg/day. The principal side effects were falls and sedation. No case of tardive dyskinesia was reported. The incidence of extrapyramidal side effects in persons treated with 1 mg or less per day was equal to placebo. A 12-week placebo-controlled trial of risperidone, haloperidol, and placebo was performed in long-term-care residents with dementia and behavioral disturbance (DeDeyn et al. 1999). Few subjects in this study had psychotic symptoms. Subjects in the active drug arms received flexible doses (0.5–4 mg/day) of risperidone or haloperidol, with mean dosages at the end point of 1.1 mg/day (risperidone) and 1.2 mg/day (haloperidol). At the end point of the study, the percentages of patients with at least 30% reduction in overall BEHAVE-AD scores were 54% in the risperidone group, 54% in the haloperidol group, and 47% in the placebo group. Endpoint data were based on the last observation for any subject who had been randomized and had received at least one dose of study medication. Dropout rates were high in all three groups (33% of those who completed the 1-week drug washout); roughly 50% of dropouts were because of side effects, and 50% were for lack of efficacy. The need for a rescue medication (lorazepam) was the same in all three groups. In a study of 330 institutionalized persons with dementia treated with risperidone, Jeste et al. (2000) found only six cases of tardive dyskinesia (2.4%) among the 255 persons without dyskinesia at baseline. Most cases of tardive dyskinesia were associated with doses of 1.5 mg/day or more. Among the 59 persons with dyskinesia at baseline, there was significantly reduced severity over the course of the risperidone trial. Thus, it appears that risperidone behaves as an atypical antipsychotic at doses less than 1.5 mg/day. The half-life of the drug and its active metabolite is 20 hours;
246
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
once-a-day dosing is reported to be as effective as multiple doses (Jibson and Tandon 1998). To avoid adverse events, titration of the drug, beginning with 0.25 mg at bedtime and increasing slowly, appears to be the procedure of choice (DeDeyn et al. 1999). NMS has been reported with risperidone. Olanzapine (Zyprexa) produces moderate blockade of D2, 5-HT2, and M1 receptors. Its side effects include sedation, postural hypotension, and weight gain. The drug has been reported to aggravate the extrapyramidal symptoms of Parkinson’s disease and to produce extrapyramidal symptoms in elderly patients without preexisting symptoms (Granger 1999). Drugdrug interactions are minimal, and the drug does not appear to be cardiotoxic. A 6-week double-blind comparison was made of olanzapine 5, 10, or 15 mg/day with placebo in nursing home residents with dementia and psychosis or behavioral disturbance. In these patients, the sum of the Agitation, Delusions, and Hallucinations subscales of the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (Cummings et al. 1994) were significantly more improved than in patients receiving placebo, with the best results at 5 or 10 mg/day (Street et al. 2000). All patients taking olanzapine were started at 5 mg/day and were titrated to the assigned dose; those who could not tolerate the assigned dose were removed from the study. The frequency of extrapyramidal symptoms was no greater in the olanzapine group than the placebo group. Only somnolence and abnormal gait occurred more frequently in the olanzapine group. The investigators did not report dropout rates or reasons for dropout. There were 19 patients in the Street et al. study randomized to olanzapine who met criteria for dementia with Lewy bodies. There were reductions in psychotic symptoms in these individuals at doses of 5–10 mg qd, but not as 15 mg qd (Cummings et al. 2002). In an open trial of olanzapine with dosages ranging from 2.5 to 7.5 mg/day, eight patients diagnosed clinically as having dementia with Lewy bodies were treated. Of these, two patients had clear improvement, three had minimal improvement, and three could not tolerate even the lowest dose of the drug due to worsening of cognition and extrapyramidal symptoms (Walker et al. 1999). Because of its 30-hour half-life, the drug can be dosed once a day. The suggested initial dosage is 2.5 mg orally at bedtime. Quetiapine (Seroquel) is a dibenzothiazepine derivative with higher affinity for 5-HT2 receptors than for D2 receptors. It inhibits H1 and a1-adrenergic receptors, but it has minimal affinity for muscarinic receptors. The drug is largely protein bound and is metabolized by cytochrome 34A to generate inactive metabolites. It has a half-life of approximately 7 hours and requires twice-daily dosing. In an open trial of quetiapine in 151 elders with psychotic disorder (cutoff age was reduced to 50 for persons with Parkinson’s disease), analysis at 12 weeks showed the most com-
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
247
mon side effects to be sedation, dizziness, postural hypotension, and increased agitation (McManus et al. 1999). The median total daily dose was 100 mg (range, 25–800 mg). These subjects included psychiatrically hospitalized patients, residents of long-term-care facilities, and communitydwelling persons. Approximately half of the subjects had symptoms due to brain diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease; 11 subjects had Parkinson’s disease. Extrapyramidal symptoms occurred in 6% of subjects, including akathisia, tremor, and nuchal rigidity. Improvements were modest, with 52% of all subjects having at least a 20% reduction from baseline in total scores on the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (Overall and Gorham 1988); the group with psychiatric illness and the group with dementing illnesses had approximately the same percentage of responders. In the same open trial, Schneider et al. (1999) found significant improvement in psychotic symptoms and hostile behaviors in 78 persons with Alzheimer’s disease (mean age, 78 years) over a period of 52 weeks. Ziprasidone (Geodon) has higher affinity for 5-HT2 receptors than for D2 receptors and minimal anticholinergic effects (Schmidt et al. 2001). There is a greater degree of QTc prolongation than with the other newer agents (Gury et al. 2000), and the drug is contraindicated in persons with QTc greater than 500 ms and in combination with other drugs that prolong QTc or inhibit the metabolism of ziprasidone. There are no published trials of ziprasidone in elders.
Neuroleptic Malignant Syndrome, Dystonia, and Tardive Dyskinesia NMS has been reported with clozapine, risperidone (Hasan and Buckley 1998), and olanzapine (Jarventausta and Leinonen 2000). Tardive dystonia has been reported with olanzapine (Dunayevich and Strakowski 1999), and we have observed the acute onset of painful neck musculature with olanzapine. Sweet et al. (1995) studied the prevalence of tardive dyskinesia in 386 consecutive persons age 60 or older who had been admitted to a geropsychiatry inpatient unit. They found a strong relationship between duration of typical neuroleptic use and tardive dyskinesia: 16% with less than 3 months of use; 29% with 3–12 months of use; 30% with 1–10 years of use; and 41% with more than 10 years of use. Jeste et al. (2000) found the 1-year incidence of tardive dyskinesia in demented institutionalized elders to be 2.6% at a mean risperidone dose of 0.96 mg/day. Patients with dyskinesia at baseline showed significant reductions in severity. In elderly persons with chronic schizophrenia, M. Davidson et al. (2000) found a 1-year incidence of 4.3% at a mean risperidone dose of 3.7 mg/day. There is no
248
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
study of tardive dyskinesia in elders treated with olanzapine, but among 513 patients with schizophrenia, the 1-year incidence of tardive dyskinesia was 0.52% (Beasley et al. 1999). Tardive dyskinesia has also been reported with quetiapine (Ghelber and Belmaker 1999).
Neuroleptic Use in Nursing Homes There are approximately 1.8 million Americans residing in nursing homes. Half of these individuals have dementing illnesses, and most of the institutionalized persons with dementia have behavioral disturbances (Cummings and Knopman 1999). Because of concern that neuroleptic drugs were used inappropriately (Committee on Nursing Home Regulation 1986), the Omnibus Budget Reconciliation Act of 1987 mandated that the Health Care Financing Administration establish standards for the use of antipsychotic drugs in nursing homes (Health Care Financing Administration 1992). The criteria are as follows: 1) symptoms are objectively documented and quantified; 2) symptoms are persistent; 3) preventable causes have been ruled out; and 4) patients are causing danger to self or others. Or, there is continuous screaming, yelling, or pacing that interferes with functioning or psychosis that causes distress and impairs function. Physicians dealing with problem behaviors must keep these guidelines in mind when prescribing antipsychotic drugs and must document that the criteria are met, at initiation and during continuation of medication. The frequency of documentation has not been mandated, but monthly documentation seems reasonable. When patient symptoms no longer meet the criteria listed above, physicians must decide whether to reduce dosage or discontinue the antipsychotic medication. It can reasonably be argued that this is tantamount to reducing or discontinuing effective treatment and inviting recurrence of the problem behaviors. In fact, a study of neuroleptic withdrawal in a large nursing facility demonstrated that 50% of patients whose antipsychotic drugs were discontinued based on the above criteria, independent of psychiatric clinical judgment, required resumption of antipsychotic treatment (Horowitz et al. 1995). In instances when past experience with the patient suggests the strong likelihood of recurrence, a statement to this effect should justify further treatment. However, unlike the unrelenting course of schizophrenia, behavioral and psychotic symptoms in persons with dementia tend to be transient. Thus, if there is no evidence that a dosage reduction or discontinuation of antipsychotic treatment has had adverse effects, strong consideration should be given to reduction of dosage and eventual discontinuation of antipsychotic medication (American Psychiatric Association 1997).
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
249
Lithium Carbonate The antimanic agent lithium carbonate has been used to control violent behavior in several different populations, including epileptic patients with interictal aggression (Gershon 1968), mentally retarded persons, and brain-damaged and psychotic prisoners (Fava 1997). Several small openlabel series suggest that lithium may be effective in controlling aggression in elderly brain-damaged patients (Schneider and Sobin 1994), but lithium is quite toxic in elders. The side effects of lithium in elderly patients include induction or exacerbation of extrapyramidal signs, confusion and ataxia, cardiac sinus node dysfunction, hyperthyroidism or hypothyroidism, and a Creutzfeldt-Jakob–like syndrome (Smith and Kocen 1988). Because of the limited evidence of efficacy in elders without mood disorder, and because of frequent toxic effects, the use of lithium for control of behavior in dementing illness is not encouraged.
Anticonvulsants There is increasing use of anticonvulsants as antiagitation agents. The theoretical rationale is that this class of drugs reduces neuronal excitability. The anticonvulsant drug carbamazepine (Tegretol) inhibits the release of the excitatory amino acid aspartate. It was found useful for decreasing aggression in patients with functional psychotic disorders by Jenike (1985), who proposed its use for violent dementia patients without evidence of mood disorder. An open study of this medication gave positive results in a placebo-controlled crossover trial conducted in 25 nursing home residents (Tariot et al. 1999). Over a period of 5 weeks, median total scores on the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale decreased 7 points when patients were given carbamazepine compared with 3 points when patients were given placebo. One subject developed drug-induced tics. Carbamazepine side effects include nausea, vomiting, drowsiness, dizziness, and blurred vision. Hepatotoxicity and bone marrow suppression are occasional complications. Complete blood counts and liver function tests should be obtained before beginning this drug. Such tests should be repeated at 3- to 6-month intervals. Carbamazepine also acts centrally to lower serum sodium. The beginning dose is one half tablet (100 mg) once a day, taken at mealtime. Dosage may be increased by 100 mg/day in divided doses with meals to a maximum of 1,200 mg/day (300–800 mg/day in elders). The recommended plasma level is in the range of 8–12 mg/mL (4–8 mg/mL in elders). Divalproex (Depakote, Depakene, Depakote-SR) is an anticonvulsant and an antimanic agent that enhances central g-aminobutyric acid (GABA)
250
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
function. It is composed of sodium valproate and valproic acid in a 1:1 molar relationship. In an open-label study of 13 patients, there was moderate improvement in dementia-related behavioral symptoms in half the individuals treated, using doses ranging from 150–250 mg three times a day. One patient experienced drowsiness and gait disturbance (Porsteinsson et al. 1997). A later placebo-controlled study of divalproex in agitated nursing home residents with dementia showed only a trend in favor of divalproex (Porsteinsson et al. 2001). This was a 6-week randomized study involving 56 nursing home residents. Of the patients receiving divalproex, 68% were rated as improved on the Clinical Global Impression of Change (Guy 1976) compared with 52% of those receiving placebo (P=0.06). Divalproex dosage varied from 375 to 1,375 mg/day. In an open study of valproic acid in 16 patients with dementia, (dosages ranging from 750 to 200 mg/day), 50% showed some improvement; only one had to discontinue because of side effects (N. Herrmann 1998). In a retrospective chart review of 29 braininjured individuals admitted to a rehabilitation unit, divalproex was found to be effective in reducing emotional lability, impulsivity, and disinhibited behaviors in 90% of individuals within 7 days after a typical dosing of 1,250 mg/day (Showalter and Kimmel 2000). In this group of patients, dosage was titrated to effectiveness and adjusted for side effects of sedation or tremor rather than blood level. The drug has additional side effects, including peripheral edema, rash, blood dyscrasias, and hepatotoxicity and requires periodic clinical monitoring. It also interacts with other anticonvulsants, and when it is used concomitantly with phenobarbital or phenytoin (Dilantin) it requires careful monitoring of those drugs. The recommended initial dosage is 15 mg/kg daily (approximately 250 mg three times a day). Gabapentin (Neurontin) is an adjunctive therapy for partial seizures. Its mechanism of action in seizure control is unclear. The drug has a half-life of only about 6 hours, and therefore it must be administered in divided doses two or three times a day. It is eliminated directly by the kidneys. The drug was reported to have reduced behavioral disturbances in 4 demented outpatients; 3 of the 4 had their dosages titrated to 2,400 mg/day (Roane et al. 2000). A prospective open study has been reported of 12 persons whose dementia-related behavioral disturbances had not responded to neuroleptics (N. Herrmann et al. 2000). At dosages ranging from 200 to 1,200 mg/ day administered over 8 weeks, there was no improvement overall in scores on the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (Cohen-Mansfield 1986) and the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (Cummings et al. 1994). However, 5 of the 12 patients showed at least minimal improvement on the Clinical Global Impression of Change scale (Schneider et al. 1997). The drug was generally well tolerated, but it had to be discontinued in 2 patients because of drowsiness and unsteady gait.
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
251
A retrospective chart review study was performed with a series of 24 Veterans Administration nursing home residents treated with gabapentin for agitation or aggression (Hawkins et al. 2000). All met DSM-IV criteria for dementia, but their dementia was of various origins. Patient ages ranged from 50 to 99, with a mean of 71 years. Most had been treated unsuccessfully with a variety of psychotropic agents. Gabapentin dosage ranged 100 mg/day to 1,200 mg three times a day, with an average effective dosage of approximately 1,300 mg/day. Two individuals were discontinued from the drug because of excessive sedation. Of the remaining 22, 17 were rated as being much improved or greatly improved based on Clinical Global Impression of Change scores. However, 16 of these patients were continued on other psychotropics. Our own clinical experience suggests that gabapentin may be a useful as a single calming agent or as an adjunct to other medications in dosages ranging from 900 to 2,400 mg/day.
>-Adrenergic Blockers Numerous anecdotal reports support the use of propranolol in managing aggression in demented elders and in younger brain-damaged patients who are unresponsive to neuroleptics (Hales et al. 1990; Haspel 1995). Surprisingly, 2 to 4 weeks are required for an effect. Effective dosages are generally reported to range from 60 to 600 mg/day. However, Shankle et al. (1995) found that low-dose propranolol monotherapy was effective in reducing disruptive, aggressive behavior in the majority of 12 outpatients treated at dosages ranging from 10 to 80 mg/day. To date, there has been only one controlled study of b-adrenergic blockers. In a double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover study in 10 violent persons with brain damage, propranolol treatment was associated with reduction of assaultive behavior (Greendyke et al. 1986). At any dosage, the use of propranolol requires monitoring of pulse, blood pressure, and (in frail elders) creatinine level as a means to ensure the adequacy of renal blood flow. The drug is contraindicated if there is a history of asthma or if active bronchopulmonary disease is present.
Serotonin Agonists Cell loss in Alzheimer’s disease often occurs in the dorsal raphe nucleus of the brainstem, the site of serotoninergic innervation of the forebrain (Yamamoto and Hirano 1985). As a result, brain serotonin metabolites are reduced by 30%–40% in Alzheimer’s disease (Gottfries et al. 1983). Low blood levels of serotonin metabolites have been reported in aggressive chil-
252
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
dren (Greenberg and Coleman 1976) and adults (Brown et al. 1979). Adverse reactions tended to occur when such persons were treated with drugs that further lowered serotonin metabolite concentration in blood. Calming occurred with drugs that increased serotonin metabolites (Greenberg and Coleman 1976). These findings stimulated interest in the use of serotonin agonists or reuptake inhibitors in persons with aggressive behaviors. Trazodone, an antidepressant with serotoninergic effects, is widely used in the management of agitation in persons with dementia. Because of its short half-life (3–4 hours), it must be dosed three or four times a day. The usual range of dosage is 25 mg three times a day to 50 mg four times a day, but doses up to 500 mg/day have been employed (Simpson and Foster 1986). The primary side effects of the drug are sedation and postural hypotension. In a double-blind study of agitated, demented elders who were psychiatric inpatients, trazodone (50–250 mg/day) was found to be as effective as haloperidol (1–5 mg/day) over a period of 9 weeks. Symptoms of repetitiveness, verbal aggression, and oppositional behaviors appeared to respond preferentially to trazodone, whereas excessive motor activity and unwarranted accusations appeared to respond better to haloperidol (Sultzer et al. 1997). The only placebo-controlled study of trazodone in outpatients showed it to be no more effective than placebo in persons with Alzheimer’s disease with mild to moderate levels of agitation (Teri et al. 2000). Paroxetine was reported to reduce inappropriate verbalizations (screaming, repetitive questions or phrases, or constant requests for attention) in 15 persons with severe Alzheimer’s disease or vascular dementia at a dosage of 10–20 mg/day. These individuals had had no response to neuroleptics, but they were allowed to continue taking neuroleptics during the 3 months of the study. The 2 subjects who developed diarrhea responded to dosage reduction from 10 to 5 mg/day (Ramadan and Naughton 1999). Reduction in disruptive vocalizations has also been reported in 9 of 16 dementia patients (primarily with Alzheimer’s disease) with citalopram treatment over a period of 17 days while hospitalized in a psychiatric facility and taking no other psychotropic drugs (Pollock et al. 1997). Because the study was uncontrolled, it is not possible to know the contribution of citalopram to this improvement in addition to the hospital environment. In another uncontrolled study, citalopram was found to reduce pathological poststroke crying following 1–3 weeks of treatment at dosages of 10–20 mg/day (Andersen et al. 1993).
Sleep-Wake Disturbance Sleep-wake disturbances are common among persons with dementing illness. In a study comparing Alzheimer’s disease patients with age-matched
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
253
control subjects, Bliwise et al. (1989) found no difference in the total amount of sleep, but patients with Alzheimer’s disease had poorer quality of sleep as evidenced by lower mean sleep efficiency, lower percentages of stage 3 and 4 sleep, and a higher percentage of stage 1 sleep. The most common sleep complaints are nighttime wakefulness and daytime sleepiness. Persons with dementia who are not active frequently doze during the day because of inactivity and are awake and restless at night because they have already obtained their required sleep during their naps. Elderly persons’ sleep is often disturbed by pain, by the need to urinate, or by the need to take medications. Stimulants such as coffee, tea, or bronchodilators may contribute to the problem. In elderly patients with loosened pharyngeal tissue, sleep apnea due to mechanical obstruction may occur and may be responsible for frequent sleep interruption and poor quality of sleep, leading to daytime hypersomnolence (Guilleminault et al. 1973). The management of daytime sleepiness begins with reducing sedating drugs to the smallest possible dose. The most likely agents are antianxiety drugs, neuroleptics, muscle relaxants, and analgesics. The next step is maximizing out-of-bed time and finding means, if possible, to keep patients physically active or at least cognitively in touch with the environment. Stimulants such as coffee, tea, and cola drinks can be used in the mornings and afternoons, but not after the evening meal. The use of other stimulants is discussed in the section “Apathy and Withdrawal.” Nighttime sleeplessness is best combated by keeping patients active during the day and postponing bedtime until after 9 P.M. Generally speaking, it is best to have patients keep the same waking time and bedtime every day of the week. Once depression, sleep apnea, and other medical causes of insomnia have been ruled out, various medications may be prescribed. Small doses of trazodone may be used as a sleep aid. It is not habit-forming, tolerance does not develop, and withdrawal symptoms do not occur. The most common dose is 25–100 mg. Barbiturates are contraindicated because they cloud sensorium. Antihistamines may be relatively contraindicated in Alzheimer’s disease because of their potential for causing anticholinergic delirium. The benzodiazepine hypnotics temazepam (Restoril) and triazolam (Halcion) may also be employed. Temazepam is administered in doses of 15–30 mg 1 hour before bedtime because of its slow absorption. Triazolam, in doses of 0.125–0.25 mg, can cause amnesia and rebound insomnia (Grad 1995). Although it is usually administered as 0.125–0.25 mg at bedtime, the lower dose was found ineffective in 7 subjects with Alzheimer’s disease who were reported by caregivers to be frequently up at night (McCarten et al. 1995). Flurazepam (Dalmane) has a half-life of more than 24 hours in
254
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
healthy adults and of 100–200 hours in elders, and it is therefore contraindicated in elders or physically debilitated persons because of its potential for accumulation and clouding of sensorium. Short-acting benzodiazepines such as oxazepam may be used in doses of 10–15 mg for nighttime sedation, but these should be administered 1 hour before bedtime because of slow absorption. Benzodiazepine hypnotics should be avoided in persons with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (George 2000) or obstructive sleep apnea who are not being treated with continuous positive air pressure (Dolly and Block 1982). These individuals may use one of the agents described below. Zolpidem (Ambien) is a rapid-acting nonbenzodiazepine hypnotic. Using polysomnography in 14 elderly psychiatric inpatients, Kummer et al. (1993) found evidence for improved continuity of sleep and total sleep duration. In 2 elderly patients with dementia and severe nighttime wandering, normal sleep patterns were established with doses of 10 and 15 mg at bedtime (Shelton and Hocking 1997). These individuals had not responded to earlier treatment with bedtime benzodiazepines, trazodone, and neuroleptics. Tolerance, withdrawal symptoms, and amnestic episodes have been reported with this drug. The dose in elders is 5–10 mg at bedtime (Holm and Goa 2000). Zaleplon (Sonata) is a pyrazolopyrimidine compound that interacts with the GABA-BZ receptor. It is rapidly absorbed and extensively metabolized by CYP 3A4 with no active metabolites. Dosage for elders is the same as for younger adults. In elders, sleep latency is reduced, but sleep duration is not increased (Hedner et al. 2000). The recommended dose is 5–10 mg. Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep behavior disorder has been treated successfully with small doses of clonazepam (Klonopin) (Ferrini-Strambi and Zucconi 2000), but this drug might increase confusion in cognitively impaired persons. Ringman and Simmons (2000) reported successful treatment of 3 cases with donepezil.
Suspiciousness and Delusions Most suspicions and delusions in dementing illness are transient and do not require drug treatment. After hearing impairment, aggravation of sensorial impairment by drugs, and the development of a major depressive episode have been ruled out, the drugs of choice are antipsychotic medications in the dosages suggested in Table 7–1. Should increased confusion follow use of these drugs, delirium should be suspected and the drug withheld or the dose lowered. Benzodiazepines are not generally useful in the management of suspiciousness and delusions.
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
255
Even when treated with drugs, suspiciousness and the formation of delusions often do not abate completely, but their interference with patient management and with the patient’s daily living needs is usually reduced. For example, suspicions may be voiced less often, or patients who formerly refused to eat because they feared poisoning may afterward refuse to eat only certain foods or only in certain circumstances.
Hallucinations Many persons with brain damage experience hallucinations. These hallucinations are usually visual but may be auditory, tactile, gustatory, or olfactory. Typical hallucinations of dementing illness were described previously. The presence of hallucinations is an indication for psychotropic drugs only when they are disturbing to the beholder. On the other hand, the development of hallucinations demands an investigation of etiology, which may include delirium due to prescribed or unprescribed drugs; infectious, metabolic, or toxic factors; severe depression; eye or optic tract pathology; an irritable brain focus; or a new environment. The primary treatment of hallucinations (usually visual, but sometimes tactile) due to delirium consists of treating the underlying condition. The use of drugs in delirium was discussed previously. Auditory hallucinations commanding a severely depressed person to commit suicide are best treated by electroconvulsive therapy. Less threatening auditory hallucinations may remit with the use of a combined antipsychotic-antidepressant drug regimen. Hallucinations in other nondelirious dementia patients are often not greatly ameliorated by psychotropic drugs. If patients become greatly disturbed by their hallucinations, low-dose antipsychotic drugs may be useful in reducing emotional distress. The visual hallucinations that frequently accompany dementia with Lewy bodies may respond to the anticholinesterase cognitive enhancers. McKeith et al. (2000b) showed reduction of visual hallucinations in 11 such patients following treatment with a cholinesterase inhibitor, fitting the observation by Perry et al. (1990) that choline acetyltransferase activity in brain was significantly lower in dementia-with-Lewy-body patients with visual hallucinations. Visual hallucinations associated with the use of dopamine agonists for the treatment of Parkinson’s disease are best treated by reduction of the antiparkinson medication. If that is not possible, the preferred drug is quetiapine in dosages beginning at 25 mg/day with gradual escalation to twice-daily dosage; the total daily dosage depends on tolerance of side effects. Wolters et al. (1996) reported success with olanzapine. Although clozapine (Clozaril) has
256
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
some efficacy in this situation, its side effects (Wolters et al. 1990) and toxicity preclude its use as a first-line drug (Salzman et al. 1995). If used, a beginning dosage of 6.25 mg/day is suggested, with gradual titration upward.
Apathy and Withdrawal After major depression, stroke, and the exacerbation of a chronic illness or the occurrence of an acute illness have been ruled out, various kinds of stimulation can be used to combat apathy and withdrawal. The first is social stimulation, as discussed in Chapter 6. The psychostimulants dextroamphetamine (Dexedrine) and methylphenidate (Ritalin) have been used successfully to treat dementing disorders characterized by psychomotor and cognitive slowing (Woods et al. 1986). Several reports have appeared on the treatment of human immunodeficiency virus type 1 (HIV-1)–associated dementia with both drugs (Fernandez et al. 1988a, 1988b). These patients responded, whether symptoms of depression were due to affective disorder or to HIV-1–associated dementia. The improvement was found without the usual delay associated with tricyclic antidepressants. Side effects normally attributed to psychostimulants— such as appetite suppression, hypertension, palpitations, cardiac arrhythmia, headache, paranoid psychosis, insomnia, drug tolerance, and abuse—were not found. In fact, appetite stimulation was observed in lower dosage ranges. One problem noted was the tendency to uncover subclinical dyskinesias or to worsen preexisting dyskinesia. Experience suggests that psychostimulants also should be avoided in persons with a history of seizures. In a trial of psychostimulants for cognitive impairment in HIV-1– associated dementia, Fernandez et al. (1988b) treated 10 subjects with methylphenidate three times a day and crossed over to dextroamphetamine on a twice-daily schedule if there was no response. Only one patient required crossover to dextroamphetamine to achieve a response. Cognitive improvement was significant for the following tests: Trails A and B, verbal memory at 5 and 30 minutes, Mini-Mental State Exam, and the verbal selective reminding test (long-term storage and consistent long-term retrieval). Frontal lobe dysfunction appeared to respond preferentially to methylphenidate, and affective symptoms appeared to respond preferentially to dextroamphetamine. Methylphenidate has the advantage of relatively minimal appetite suppression and a half-life of 2–6 hours, allowing more sensitive titration. It is initiated at 5–10 mg per day, with increases in total daily dose every 2–3 days. The average response range in HIV-1–associated dementia is 30–45 mg/day; as much as 120 mg/day has been required. Dextroamphetamine is
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
257
a second-line drug because of its 18-hour half-life. It is started at 5–10 mg twice a day, with 5–10 mg adjustments in total daily dose every 2–3 days to a maximum of 60 mg/day.
Hypersexuality Hypersexuality occurs occasionally in nursing home residents (Levitsky and Owens 1999), where it may be disturbing to residents, staff, and family members of residents who are unable to defend themselves against sexual approach. Antiandrogens, estrogen, progesterone, and luteinizing hormone–releasing hormone (LHRH) blockers have all been employed for reducing sexual drive in men (Hashmi et al. 2000). A number of case reports indicate that the antiandrogen medroxyprogesterone is effective in reducing sexual drive and sexually aggressive acts in both cognitively intact and brain-damaged men, including Alzheimer’s disease patients (Cooper 1988). With daily or weekly dosing, side effects are minimal and include blood pressure elevation and pedal edema. We found in two cases that doses of medroxyprogesterone acetate (Depo-Provera) 150–200 mg intramuscularly every other week eliminated hypersexual behavior within a month (Weiner et al. 1992). Our unreported experience (M.F.W.) is that medroxyprogesterone acetate does not reduce hypersexuality in demented women. Lothstein et al. (1997) reported positive results in a small group of cognitively impaired elderly men with sexual disinhibition with low-dose SSRIs and also with estrogen patches; the latter at doses of 0.05, 0.1, or 0.625 mg. Estrogen therapy carries the danger of deep vein thrombosis and thromboembolism. There is a single case report of the use of leuprolide, an LHRH antagonist, in the treatment of sexual aggression in a patient with dementia and features of Klüver-Bucy syndrome (Ott 1995). There are no reports of the treatment of hypersexual behavior in women with dementia.
Impaired Judgment Judgment is a complex psychological function influenced by many factors that can be modified through the use of psychotropic drugs. Delirium reduces concentration, alters sleep-wake patterns, impairs cognition, and gives rise to illusions and hallucinations. Depressed mood affects judgment by making concentration difficult, by the sheer effort of thinking, by preoccupation with real or delusional worries, and by interfering with sleep. Anxiety impairs judgment by interfering with concentration. Appropriate treatment of delirium, depression, and anxiety can improve judgment by improving attention and concentration. In the frontotemporal dementias,
258
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
impairment of judgment may be the first symptom. Based on the finding of low serotonin receptor binding in Pick’s disease (Sparks and Marksbery 1991), there have been efforts to treat behavior problems in persons with frontotemporal dementias with SSRIs. In a report on the use of SSRIs (fluoxetine, sertraline, and paroxetine) to treat behavior problems in persons with frontotemporal dementias, Swartz et al. (1997) found positive responses in certain behaviors for approximately 50% of the 11 subjects treated. Positive responses at 3 months were in disinhibition, depressive symptoms, carbohydrate craving, and compulsions.
Real-World Use of Drugs for Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms In general, we prefer to use psychotropic drugs on a regular basis rather than as needed for the management of symptoms that are frequent or dangerous to self or others. Our rationale is that the disturbances being treated by these drugs are episodic and short lived. The symptoms frequently subside before the as-needed drug can take effect. When symptoms are infrequent, such as less than once a week, as-needed medication is a reasonable approach because it lowers the risk of long-term exposure to the drug without greatly increasing the chance of failing to treat the behavior. Another indication for as-needed medication is when disturbed behaviors arise at predictable intervals such as bath time. In that instance, a prophylactic dose of a short-acting drug is rational treatment. In the management of dementia-related behaviors, monotherapy is more the exception than the rule. Combined use of neuroleptics and antimanic drugs is common, as is the combined use of neuroleptics and SSRIs. These are often augmented with as-needed doses of a short-acting benzodiazepine and the regular use of trazodone for sleep. The advantage of drug combinations is avoiding the side effects of high doses of individual drugs; the disadvantage is that of drug-drug interaction. With the exception of major depression, which should probably be treated for a long term (e.g., years), the use of psychotropic drugs for behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementing illness should be short term (months). Periodic attempts at reduction of dosage are warranted unless there is good evidence that dangerous or severely disturbing symptoms will arise as a result of decreasing the dosage or frequency of medication administration. The drugs that are discussed in this section are those approved by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease and the prevention and treatment of cerebrovascular disease.
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
259
Drugs for Alzheimer’s Disease This section covers drugs or substances that are FDA approved or are in wide current use in the United States. Promising and experimental drugs or substances are discussed in Chapter 15.
Ginkgo Biloba Flavonoid extracts of the leaves of the Ginkgo biloba (maidenhair) tree are available on a nonprescription basis throughout the world for a variety of nonspecific indications, including the implicit claim of improving memory or depression in healthy persons as well as those with problems. The substance may have antioxidant and anti-inflammatory effects (Oken et al. 1998). However, a 6-week, randomized, placebo-controlled study in healthy cognitively intact elders showed an effect on cognitive measures at a dose of 40 mg 3 times per day (Solomon et al. 2002). A 52-week randomized, double-blind clinical trial has been performed in the United States involving persons with Alzheimer’s disease and with vascular dementia (LeBars et al. 1997). Patients were dosed at 40 mg three times a day of E Gb 761, a pharmaceutical-grade, standardized extract of ginkgo flavonoids. Of 309 persons enrolled, 52-week data were available for 202 persons. In an intentto-treat analysis, the ginkgo-treated group had scores on the Cognitive subscale of the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS-Cog) (Rosen et al. 1984) that were 1.4 points better than those of the placebo group (P=0.004). In the Alzheimer’s disease subgroup (n=236), the difference was 1.5 points. No difference was detected using the Clinical Global Impression of Change. The drug did not differ significantly from placebo in side effects. An analysis by Oken et al. (1998) of Ginkgo biloba studies concluded that only four (including that of LeBars et al. 1997) met criteria for metaanalysis. In general, there was a small but significant effect in Alzheimer’s disease patients at 3 and 6 months with 120–240 mg/day, which amounted to a 3% difference in the ADAS-Cog scores between drug and placebo. Based on these findings, funding has been awarded by the National Institutes of Health for further investigation of Gingko biloba in Alzheimer’s disease.
Drugs Affecting the Cholinergic System Precursors of acetylcholine, cholinomimetics, and anticholinesterases have been used in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Their use is based on the discovery of reduced cholinergic function in Alzheimer’s disease (P. Davies and Maloney 1976) and on the effects of anticholinergic and cholinomi-
260
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
metic drugs on memory. Drachman and Leavitt (1974) showed that scopolamine impaired the storage of information in long-term memory with relative sparing of recall and retrieval from long-term memory—a finding that resembles the impairment of normal aging. The effect of scopolamine on memory is reversed by physostigmine (Granacher and Baldessarini 1976). Furthermore, physostigmine has direct memory-enhancing effects when carefully titrated in humans (K.L. Davis et al. 1978). Based on these findings, three main strategies have been employed to increase cholinergic transmission: increasing substrate available for the biosynthesis of acetylcholine, using cholinomimetics to augment acetylcholine activity, and blocking the degradation of acetylcholine to prolong its activity at the receptor site. However, K.L. Davis et al. (1999) showed that activity of the cholinergic marker enzymes choline acetyltransferase and acetyl cholinesterase in nine cortical regions in subjects with dementia did not differ significantly from control subjects until affected persons had clinically severe Alzheimer’s disease as indicated by premortem Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (Hughes et al. 1982) scores of 4–5. Thus, K.L. Davis et al. (1999) suggest that cholinesterase inhibitors may be more effective in more advanced disease, a suggestion that is not borne out by clinical experience. Precursors. Acetylcholine is produced in the brain by the acetylation of choline through the action of the enzyme choline acetyltransferase and the cofactor coenzyme A, but there is little evidence that lecithin or phosphatidylcholine is useful in ameliorating the cognitive deficits in Alzheimer’s disease. Acetyl-L-carnitine, marketed in Italy as Nicetile for treatment of cognitive impairment, is available in the United States as a food supplement. It is a naturally occurring substance that may help in the formation of acetyl coenzyme A or acetylcholine. It has been used in Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. In a 1-year placebo-controlled study in persons with mild to moderate probable Alzheimer’s disease, the drug did not differ from placebo in a dose of 3 g/day (Thal et al. 1996). In a subanalysis, patients with early-onset disease appeared to show greater improvement than those with late onset. A subsequent study of the substance in early-onset cases was abandoned because of recruitment difficulties. Cholinomimetics. The cholinomimetic drugs, which must be regarded as experimental, are discussed in Chapter 15. Anticholinesterases. The anticholinesterases (Table 7–6) have been the most successful cognitive enhancers in Alzheimer’s disease. Of these, tacrine (Cognex) was introduced in 1993, donepezil (Aricept) in 1997, rivastigmine (Exelon) in 2000, and galantamine (Reminyl) in 2001. All four
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
TABLE 7–6.
261
Dosages of cholinesterase inhibitorsa
Generic name
Trade name
Tacrine Donepezil Rivastigmine Galantamine
Cognex Aricept Exelon Reminyl
Initial dose (mg/day)
Optimal individual dose (mg)
40 5 3 8
40 10 6 8–12
Frequency of dosage qid qd (in am) bid (with food) bid
Note. qid=four times a day; qam=every morning; qd=every day; bid=twice a day. a Dosages should be increased no sooner than 4 weeks.
drugs are reversible inhibitors of acetylcholinesterase; rivastigmine also reversibly inhibits butyrylcholinesterase; galantamine has nicotinic effects. The therapeutic and side effects of all of these drugs are virtually identical, except for the hepatotoxicity of tacrine. Tacrine, an aminoacridine derivative, has been largely abandoned because of its four-times-a-day dose schedule, the need for titration from 40 mg/day to 160 mg/day to reach the most effective dosage, and a high incidence of hepatic toxicity. It has been reported that patients who continued taking dosages of tacrine of 80 mg/day or more for 2 years were less likely to be admitted to nursing facilities than those taking lower doses (odds ratio, 2.7 for >80 mg/day; 2.8 for >120 mg/day) (Knopman et al. 1996). This finding is difficult to interpret because of the bias introduced by attrition (Knopman et al. 1998). Donepezil is a piperidine-based reversible acetylcholinesterase inhibitor. Peak plasma concentrations occur 2–4 hours after an oral dose. It is 100% bioavailable, and food has no effect on its absorption. The drug is more than 90% protein bound. Plasma concentrations increase linearly with dosage increase. The half-life of the drug is approximately 70 hours. At 5 mg/day, donepezil produces 64% red blood cell cholinesterase inhibition (Crismon 1998). A relationship has been shown between the cognitive effect of donepezil and the degree of plasma acetylcholinesterase inhibition; however, a plateau of acetylcholinesterase inhibition was reached at plasma concentrations greater than 50 ng/mL (Rogers and Friedhoff 1996). In a 24-week study, roughly 80% of patients receiving donepezil 5 or 10 mg showed no cognitive worsening on the ADAS-Cog compared with 42% of the placebo group (Rogers et al. 1998). On a measure of global function, approximately 25% of the donepezil-treated group (5 or 10 mg) improved compared with 11% of the placebo group. Thus, the overall effect of the drug was to maintain patients at their baseline level of function for approximately 24 weeks, whereas those receiving placebo decline below baseline.
262
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
In a report on donepezil administered for as long as 144 weeks it was found that, on average, cognitive scores for individuals who had received 10 mg/day remained above baseline for 51 weeks (Doody et al. 2001). However, this finding is of limited value because it only included patients who were benefiting from the medication. Side effects of donepezil include muscarinic cholinomimetic effects such as decreased pulse, increased gastrointestinal motility, urinary urgency, and sweating. Common side effects reported by patients are nausea, vomiting, diarrhea, muscle cramps (due to nicotinic effects), mild stimulation, and vivid dreams that are sometimes frightening. The gastrointestinal side effects are usually transient and respond to short-term abstinence from the drug or lowering of the dose. The medication is available in 5- and 10-mg tablets. Although there is slightly greater efficacy at 10 mg/day, there are significantly greater side effects at that dose. Titration from 5 to 10 mg is recommended after 4–6 weeks; earlier increases in dosage are associated with more frequent side effects. Donepezil has low affinity in vitro for CYP 2D6 and 3A4, making drug-drug interactions unlikely. Although the manufacturer recommends dosing the medication at bedtime, the stimulating effects of the drug (including vivid dreams) may be better tolerated if it is administered in the morning. The carbamate rivastigmine is a reversible inhibitor of both acetylcholinesterase and butyrylcholinesterase. It is rapidly absorbed orally, has a bioavailability of 0.36, and is only 40% protein bound. Elimination halflife is approximately 2 hours. It is converted to an inactive metabolite at the site of action and is not metabolized by the liver (Jann 2000). Its effects are dose dependent. In a double, blind, placebo-controlled study, rivastigmine in dosages of 1–4 mg/day and 6–12 mg/day were compared with placebo over a 26-week period (Corey-Bloom et al. 1998). Dosage was titrated upward on a weekly basis until the maximum tolerable dosage was reached. In the placebo and low-dosage groups, the dropout rate was 15%, whereas in the high-dosage group the dropout rate was 35%. Only 55% of the persons in the high-dosage group achieved the maximum rivastigmine dosage of 12 mg/day. At 26 weeks, the high-dosage rivastigmine group differed from the placebo group by nearly 5 points on the ADASCog, remaining 1 point above baseline. At 26 weeks, the high-dosage rivastigmine group had improved 0.3 point over baseline on the Mini-Mental State Exam (Folstein et al. 1975), whereas the placebo-treated group had declined by 0.79 points. On an activities of daily living scale, 25% of highdosage rivastigmine-treated patients showed clinically meaningful improvement (10%) compared with 15% of persons in the placebo group. There was no difference in outcome between low-dosage rivastigmine and placebo. Rösler et al. (1999) performed a parallel study showing a similar
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
263
dropout rate for high-dosage rivastigmine. Using an intent-to-treat analysis, the proportion of high-dosage treated subjects who achieved a meaningful improvement (4 points on the ADAS-Cog) was higher than in the placebo group (24% versus 16%). (Among those who completed the 26 weeks of study, the proportion was 29% versus 19%.) The incidence of treatment-related side effects increased with rivastigmine dosage and decreased with dosage reduction in both studies. The most common side effects were cholinergic, including nausea, vomiting, diarrhea, and anorexia. Nausea and vomiting occurred most commonly in the dosage titration phase. Rivastigmine is available in tablets of 1.5, 3, 4.5, and 6 mg. The drug is administered twice a day with food. The initial recommendation of the manufacturer was titration of the drug at a minimum 2-week interval to 6 mg twice a day. However, clinical experience indicates that fewer side effects will be encountered with dose titration at 4-week intervals, and it is now suggested that the medication be taken after a full meal. Galantamine is an alkaloid derived from the bulbs of the daffodil. It reversibly and competitively inhibits acetylcholinesterase (Bores et al. 1996). It also allosterically modulates the response of nicotinic receptors to acetylcholine. The enhancement of cholinergic nicotinic neurotransmission may increase the release of acetylcholine (Albuquerque et al. 1997). This is of potential importance for the therapy of Alzheimer’s disease, in which there is a significant loss of cortical nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (Court et al. 2001). However, there is no evidence that this is related to the observed therapeutic actions of galantamine. Galantamine is well absorbed and has absolute bioavailability of about 90%. It has a terminal elimination half-life of 7 hours and has linear pharmacokinetics in dosages from 8 to 32 mg/day. It is 18% protein bound. The drug is metabolized by the hepatic P450 isoenzymes and by glucuronidation, and it is also excreted unchanged in the urine. A 6-month randomized, placebo-controlled trial was undertaken in 636 persons with mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease by Raskind et al. (2000). Patients were randomized to placebo or to escalating dosages of galantamine up to 24 or 32 mg/day. (Eligible subjects also entered a 6-month open-label study of the 24-mg/day dosage.) Patients receiving either dosage of drug had significantly better ADAS-Cog scores than did those receiving placebo (patients receiving 24 mg/day scored 3.9 points higher on average than the placebo group; patients receiving 32 mg/day scored 3.6 points higher), and both groups showed significantly better scores than the placebo group on the Clinician’s Interview-Based Impression of Change plus Caregiver Input) (Schneider et al. 1997). At 12 months on the 24-mg/ day dosage, ADAS-Cog scores and daily function showed no deterioration
264
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
from baseline. The dosage of galantamine was increased weekly in this study; dropout rates for adverse events were 7.5% for placebo, 23% for 24 mg/day, and 32% for 32 mg/day. Wilcock et al. (2000) reported on a double-blind, placebo-controlled study of 653 persons with mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease whose daily dose of galantamine was escalated weekly over 3–4 weeks to 24 or 32 mg/ day. The dropout rates for adverse events were 13.5% for the placebo group, 20% for the 24-mg group, and 22% for the 32-mg group. ADASCog scores differed significantly from placebo at 6 months (2.9 points difference for 24 mg; 3.1 points difference for 32 mg) based on an intent-totreat analysis. Apolipoprotein E genotype had no effect on outcome. Titration at monthly intervals in a third study (Tariot et al. 2000) reduced the dropout rate for adverse events to 7% for 16 mg/day and 10% for 32 mg/ day. In this study, there were significant differences in scores on a global measure (the Clinician’s Interview-Based Impression of Change plus Caregiver Input), on activities of daily living, and on behavioral symptoms, using the Neuropsychiatric Inventory as the measure. Adverse events in all studies were primarily gastrointestinal symptoms. On the basis of these studies, the manufacturer recommends titration to 16 or 24 mg/day (8 or 12 mg twice a day) at 1-month intervals. It is available in 4-, 8-, and 12-mg tablets and is administered twice a day. The cholinesterase inhibitors are used with caution in persons with complete heart block or sinus bradycardia and in the presence of active peptic ulcer disease or asthma. Drug-drug interactions are uncommon. Patients and their families need to be counseled about the concomitant use of highly anticholinergic drugs such as diphenhydramine and oxybutynin. In our practice, we offer cholinesterase inhibitors to persons whom we believe to be in the prodromal stage of Alzheimer’s disease and have not reached the stage of full-blown dementia. We generally attempt to reach the maximum dosage suggested by the manufacturer, because the effects of these drugs are dose related. Our hope is to maintain the patient’s level of function. In dealing with families, we emphasize that temporary stabilization is the optimal result with the cholinesterase inhibitors. It is difficult to know how long treatment should be continued, because with many patients discontinuance of these drugs is associated with worsening of cognitive symptoms. In general, we do not recommend initiation of treatment with cholinesterase inhibitors in persons who require institutional care (as opposed to assisted living). When the decision is made to discontinue a cholinesterase inhibitor, gradual downward dosage titration seems appropriate; our experience is that rapid withdrawal is often followed by an exacerbation of cognitive and behavioral symptoms. If symptoms worsen with slow withdrawal, we continue the medication.
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
265
Anticholinesterases for Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms On the hypothesis that the delusions of Alzheimer’s disease might be due to cholinergic deficit in the limbic system, Cummings et al. (1993) administered oral physostigmine to two Alzheimer’s disease patients. The researchers found delusions to be markedly diminished, dosing the drug at 1 mg approximately every 2 hours (due to the short half-life of physostigmine). Later, Cummings and Kaufer (1996) and Cummings and Back (1998) postulated that a variety of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease might be related to cholinergic deficit and might respond to cholinergic treatment. Cummings (2000) suggested that acetylcholinesterase inhibitors should be considered psychotropic drugs as well as cognitive enhancers, because his literature review suggested that anticholinesterases have an effect on apathy and visual hallucinations. He speculated that the connection of behavioral and psychological symptoms to the cholinergic system might be loss of input from cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain to the limbic and paralimbic regions and to the cerebral cortex. Raskind et al. (1997) reviewed the data from a 30-week tacrine trial using last observation carried forward. They compared outcomes for persons receiving placebo (n=181) and those receiving tacrine (n=234) in dosages ranging from 40 to 160 mg/day. They found that on 3 of the 10 ADAS noncognitive items (cooperation, delusions, and pacing), there were significant improvements with tacrine over placebo. In an open-label study of tacrine in outpatients with Alzheimer’s disease, there was significant reduction in total Neuropsychiatric Inventory scores, with the group classified by MiniMental State Exam score as moderately impaired showing greater improvement than the mildly or severely impaired groups (Kaufer et al. 1998). Mega et al. (1999) studied 86 community-dwelling persons with Alzheimer’s disease who were treated with donepezil 5 mg/day for 4 weeks, followed by treatment with 10 mg/day for 4 weeks. Using global Neuropsychiatric Inventory scores as the criterion, behavioral improvement was seen in 41% and behavioral worsening in 28%. Persons who showed behavioral responses had worse initial scores on the following Neuropsychiatric Inventory subscales: Delusions, Agitation, Depression, Anxiety, Apathy, Disinhibition, and Irritability. Cummings et al. (2000) reported on 84 persons with Alzheimer’s disease treated with donepezil for 6 months and contrasted them with a group of Alzheimer’s disease patients (n=248) who were not taking the drug. Patients taking donepezil were significantly less likely to be threatening, destroy property, or talk loudly, and fewer were receiving sedatives. Weiner et al. (2000) performed a prospective
266
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
12-month study of donepezil in 25 Alzheimer’s disease patients using a reference group for comparison. The reference group was of communitydwelling persons with Alzheimer’s disease who were not receiving anticholinesterase treatment (n = 153) and were enrolled in a 1-year study of assessment instruments for Alzheimer’s disease patients (Patterson et al. 1997). Using the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) Behavioral Rating Scale for Dementia (CBRSD) (Tariot et al. 1995), Weiner et al. (2000) found that the donepezil-treated group showed improvement in CBRSD total scores at 3 months and in Depression and Behavioral Dysregulation scores at 4 months. In this group, CBRSD total, Depression, and Behavioral Dysregulation scores returned to baseline at 12 months, in contrast with the reference group, whose CBRSD scores worsened minimally over the 12 months. In an open-label study of rivastigmine in 11 persons diagnosed clinically with dementia with Lewy bodies, Neuropsychiatric Inventory scores fell after 12 weeks of treatment by 73% for delusions, 63% for apathy, 45% for agitation, and 27% for hallucinations. Of the 11 persons treated, 5 had not responded to prior treatment including low-dose neuroleptics. Furthermore, parkinsonian symptoms tended to improve. The mean dosage of rivastigmine was 9.6 mg/day (range, 3–12 mg/day) (McKeith et al. 2000a). A larger (n=120) double-blind, placebo-controlled study was conducted over 20 weeks in patients with dementia with Lewy bodies. Of the 120 participants, 18 taking rivastigmine and 10 taking placebo dropped out. The mean dosage of rivastigmine of 9.4 mg/day at the end of the 8-week titration period declined slightly afterward. The maximum daily dosage of 12 mg was achieved by only 56% of the 48 completers; most (92%) were able to tolerate at least 6 mg/day. There was no change in extrapyramidal symptoms when patients were taking rivastigmine. Despite the lack of significant differences at 20 weeks between rivastigmine and placebo in MiniMental State Exam and Clinical Global Change scores, patients receiving rivastigmine were significantly less anxious and apathetic and had fewer delusions and hallucinations than control subjects, based on observed cases (McKeith et al. 2000b). A later report from this same study indicated that therapeutic benefits were lost when the drug was discontinued after 20 weeks (Wesnes et al. 2002), underscoring the need to maintain treatment with cholinesterase inhibitors. Although there appears to be much reporting of salutary behavioral effects from cholinesterase inhibitors, these are generally uncontrolled case series. These patients often had little behavioral disturbance at baseline, and the significance of any apparent differences is not clear. The responsiveness of the persons with greater symptoms may be a function of having scores high enough on behavioral instruments to be detected.
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
267
Antioxidants Evidence of possible central nervous system damage due to lipid peroxidation and oxidative injury (Lethem and Orrell 1997; Sano et al. 1996) led to a trial of vitamin E (a-tocopherol) and the MAOI selegiline in 341 persons with Alzheimer’s disease (Sano et al. 1997). The trial, conducted over 2 years, compared a-tocopherol (2,000 IU/day), selegiline (10 mg/day), and a-tocopherol plus selegiline with placebo. The primary outcome measure (end point) was time to death, institutionalization, loss of ability to perform basic activities of daily living, or severe dementia (Clinical Dementia Rating Scale score of 3). When adjusted for severity of illness at the beginning of the study, selegiline was associated with a delay of 655 days to end point; a-tocopherol, with 670 days; and selegiline and a-tocopherol combined, 585 days. There was no difference between groups in cognitive measures or in overall side effects. The most important contraindication to high-dose vitamin E is the use of warfarin (Coumadin) as an anticoagulant.
Drugs for Prevention and Treatment of Cerebrovascular Disease In theory, vasodilators such as papaverine might be useful in cases of cerebrovascular insufficiency. However, studies using the vasodilator acetazolamide (Diamox) have shown that blood flow actually diminishes in ischemic areas after drug administration, because it reduces perfusion pressure throughout the brain and results in a “steal” of blood from areas of high resistance (Bonte et al. 1986).
Blood Viscosity Reducers and Antiplatelet Drugs The blood viscosity reducers and antiplatelet drugs seem to be a logical means to slow or arrest the progress of dementia due to cerebrovascular disease. Prevention of sludging and platelet aggregation should prevent the formation of clots on arterial atherosclerotic plaques and possibly help maintain arteriolar patency. Aspirin is the most widely used drug of this class. In low doses, aspirin inhibits the enzyme cyclooxygenase, which is responsible for the synthesis by platelets of thromboxane, a substance that promotes platelet aggregation. The effectiveness of aspirin in preventing transient ischemic attacks and reducing thrombotic strokes and myocardial infarction is well known (Fields et al. 1978; Swedish Cooperative Study Group 1987). Its principal adverse effects are gastric irritation and gastrointestinal bleeding. Rarely, aspirin will cause thrombocytopenia. Reducing the dose from the conven-
268
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
tional 325 mg and employing an 81-mg dose that is enterically coated reduces the gastrointestinal complications. Ticlopidine hydrochloride (Ticlid) is a platelet antiaggregant that inhibits the adenosine diphosphate pathway of platelet aggregation. It does not inhibit the cyclooxygenase pathway or block the production of thromboxane by platelets or the production of prostacyclin by endothelial cells (Hass et al. 1989). When compared with placebo in 1,072 subjects with completed strokes, ticlopidine (250 mg twice a day) reduced the rate of stroke or stroke death by 33.5% over 3 years (Gent et al. 1989). Compared with aspirin at a dose of 325 mg four times a day, ticlopidine at 250 mg twice a day reduced the rate of strokes in more than 3,000 persons with prior cerebrovascular symptoms (transient ischemic attacks, amaurosis fugax, mild stroke) 21% more frequently than did aspirin. It was equally effective in men and women (Hass et al. 1989). The chief side effects of ticlopidine are diarrhea and skin rash. An uncommon but important side effect is severe reversible neutropenia, which occurs within the first 3 months of treatment. Clopidogrel (Plavix), an adenosine diphosphate receptor antagonist, was slightly more effective than aspirin in reducing the combined risk of ischemic stroke, myocardial infarction, or vascular death in 19,185 patients treated an average of 1.9 years with clopidogrel 75 mg once a day versus aspirin 325 mg/day (Jarvis and Simpson 2000). For aspirin, the overall annual rate was 5.83%; for clopidogrel, it was 5.33%. Drug interactions are rare with this medication (Easton 1999), but like aspirin, it may cause severe thrombocytopenia. Gastrointestinal hemorrhage was slightly less frequent with this drug than with aspirin. It shares with ticlopidine the occasional development of diarrhea or skin rash, but it is less likely to cause neutropenia. The dosage is 75 mg once a day. Dipyridamole (Persantine) is a platelet adhesion inhibitor whose mechanism of action is unknown but may be related to red cell update of adenosine. In a large randomized clinical trial, persons who had survived a stroke or transient ischemic attack were randomized to placebo, aspirin 50 mg/ day, sustained-release dipyridamole (400 mg/day), or a combination of aspirin and dipyridamole (Forbes 1998). Dipyridamole and aspirin were equally effective in reducing the recurrence of stroke (16% for dipyridamole versus 18% for aspirin) compared with placebo; when they were combined, there was a 37% reduction in stroke recurrence compared with placebo. Aspirin was associated with enhanced bleeding and gastrointestinal side effects; dipyridamole was associated with headache. Dipyridamole is administered in doses ranging from 25 to 100 mg four times a day. The use of warfarin (Coumadin) to prevent stroke in persons with atrial fibrillation is well documented (Lodwick 1999) but is beyond the scope of this review.
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
269
Hypolipidemic Agents Based on evidence accumulated in the 1990s, it is evident that 3-hydroxy3-methylglutaryl coenzyme A reductase inhibitors (so-called statin drugs) significantly reduce cardiovascular events, including stroke (Amarenco 2001). The literature is vast and is beyond the scope of this discussion. Of note, it has been recommended that persons with established atherosclerosis be treated with a statin to achieve a low-density lipoprotein cholesterol level less than 100 mg/dL (Ansell 2000).
Drugs to Reduce Hyperhomocysteinemia Epidemiologic studies have shown hyperhomocysteinemia (serum concentration greater than 12 mM/L) to be associated with increased risk for atherosclerotic cardiovascular disease (W. Herrmann 2001). Research is now under way to determine the impact of reducing homocysteine levels in blood by use of high-dose folic acid and B vitamins.
Summary Many drugs have been employed to treat emotional, behavioral, and cognitive symptoms in cognitively impaired persons. Most of these drugs have very modest efficacy. Their dosage depends in part on patients’ general physical state and in part on the specific dementing illness. Almost all of the potent drugs that are available have important side effects and toxicity. NMS, paradoxical hypertension from MAOIs, withdrawal from minor tranquilizers, and bradycardia from anticholinergics can be life threatening. Parkinsonian symptoms and tardive dyskinesia from neuroleptics can be disabling. Patients often fall when sedated. Therefore, although drugs do much to enhance the quality of life of some patients, they may also do harm if prescribed inappropriately or not monitored adequately. In progressive dementing disease, different medication regimens may be required at different stages of the illness. Early on, an antidepressant may be indicated to treat mood disturbance or irritability; later, an antipsychotic or anticonvulsant may be needed to deal with agitated behaviors with and without psychotic symptoms; and ultimately, stimulants may be used to combat apathy and withdrawal. Drugs are available that produce transient improvement of global function and that appear to slow progression and improve behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease, but they do not yet address the underlying disease process. On the other hand, the use of statins addresses an important aspect of the pathophysiology of vascular dementia, and they, along with anticoagulants, are reducing the occurrence of stroke.
270
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
References Adams RD, Victor M: Principles of Neurology, 4th Edition. New York, McGrawHill, 1989, p 827 Adams RD, Victor M, Mancall EL: Central pontine myelinolysis. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 81:154–172, 1959 Albuquerque EX, Alkondon M, Periera EFR, et al: Properties of neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors: pharmacological characterization and modulation of synaptic function. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 280:1117–1136, 1997 Alexopoulos GS, Salzman C: Treatment of depression with heterocyclic antidepressants, monoamine oxidase inhibitors, and psychomotor stimulants, in Clinical Geriatric Psychopharmacology. Edited by Salzman C. Baltimore, MD, Williams & Wilkins, 1998, pp 184–244 Alexopoulos GS, Meyers BS, Young RC, et al: “Vascular depression” hypothesis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54:915–922, 1997 Amarenco P: Hypercholesterolemia, lipid-lowering agents, and the risk for brain infarction. Neurology 57 (5 suppl 2):S35–S44, 2001 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd Edition, Revised. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 1987 American Psychiatric Association: Practice guideline for the treatment of patients with Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias of late life. Am J Psychiatry 154 (suppl):1–39, 1997 Andersen G, Vestergaard K, Riis JO: Citalopram for post-stroke pathological crying. Lancet 342:837–838, 1993 Ansell BJ: Cholesterol, stroke risk, and stroke prevention. Curr Atheroscler Rep 2:92–96, 2000 Baldessarini RJ: Chemotherapy in Psychiatry. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press, 1977 Barak Y, Swartz M, Shamir E, et al: Vitamin E (alpha-tocopherol) in the treatment of tardive dyskinesia: a statistical meta-analysis. Ann Clin Psychiatry 10:101– 105, 1998 Beasley CM, Dellva MA, Tamura RN, et al: Randomized double-blind comparison of the incidence of tardive dyskinesia in patients with schizophrenia during longterm treatment with olanzapine or haloperidol. Br J Psychiatry 174:23–30, 1999 Bender AD, Post A, Meier JP, et al: Plasma protein binding of drugs as a function of age in adult human subjects. J Pharm Sci 64:1711–1713, 1975 Bliwise DL, Tinklenberg J, Yesavage JA, et al: REM latency in Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 25:320–328, 1989 Bohm C, Robinson DS, Gammans RE, et al: Buspirone therapy in anxious elderly patients: a controlled clinical trial. J Clin Psychopharmacol 10 (suppl 3):S47– S51, 1990 Bondareff W, Alpert M, Friedhof AJ, et al: Comparison of sertraline and nortriptyline in the treatment of major depressive disorder in late life. Am J Psychiatry 157:729–736, 2000
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
271
Bonte FJ, Devous MD, Stokely EM, et al: Testing vasoreactivity with SPECT study of regional cerebral blood flow in patients with Alzheimer or cerebrovascular disease. J Comput Assist Tomogr 10:579–583, 1986 Bores GM, Huger FP, Petko W, et al: Pharmacological evaluation of novel Alzheimer’s disease therapeutics: acetylcholinesterase inhibitors related to galantamine. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 277:728–738, 1996 Breitbart W, Marotta R, Platt MM, et al: A double-blind trial of haloperidol, chlorpromazine, and lorazepam in the treatment of delirium in hospitalized AIDS patients. Am J Psychiatry 153:231–237, 1996 Bremner JD: Mirtazapine in the treatment of elderly patients with depression. Clinical Geriatrics 7:62–72, 1999 Brown GL, Ballenger C, Minichiello MD, et al: Human aggression and its relation to cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindol acetic acid, 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol, and homovanillic acid, in Psychopharmacology of Aggression. Edited by Sandler M. New York, Raven, 1979, pp 131–148 Brymer C, Winograd C: Fluoxetine in elderly patients: is there cause for concern? J Am Geriatr Soc 40:902–905, 1992 Buchan H, Johnstone E, McPherson K, et al: Who benefits from electroconvulsive therapy? combined results of the Leicester and Northwick Park Trials. Br J Psychiatry 160:355–359, 1992 Butters MA, Becker JT, Nebes RD, et al: Changes in cognitive functioning following treatment of late-life depression. Am J Psychiatry 157:1949–1954, 2000 Calkin PA, Kunik ME, Orengo CA, et al: Tolerability of clonazepam in demented and non-demented geropsychiatric patients. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 12:745– 749, 1997 Calkin PA, Hamilton JV, Orengo CA, et al: Trazodone for insomnia and agitation in the elderly. Clinical Geriatrics 7:34–40, 1999 Carlyle W, Ancill RJ, Sheldon L: Aggression in the demented patient: a doubleblind study of loxapine versus haloperidol. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 8:103– 108, 1993 Caroff SN: The neuroleptic malignant syndrome. J Clin Psychiatry 41:79–83, 1980 Chesrow EJ, Kaplitz SE, Vetra H, et al: Double-blind study of oxazepam in the management of geriatric patients with behavioral problems. Clin Med 72: 1001–1005, 1965 Chouinard G, de Montigny C, Annable L: Tardive dyskinesia and antiparkinson medication. Am J Psychiatry 136:228–229, 1979 Christensen DB, Benfield WR: Alprazolam as an alternative to low-dose haloperidol in older, cognitively impaired nursing facility residents. J Am Geriatr Soc 46:620–625, 1998 Cohen LJ: Managing depression in the elderly patient: avoiding serious adverse effects and drug-drug interactions. Primary Psychiatry 6:69–80, 1999 Cohen-Mansfield J: Agitated behaviors in the elderly, II: preliminary results in the cognitively deteriorated. J Am Geriatr Soc 34:722–727, 1986 Cohen-Mansfield J, Billig N: Agitated behaviors in the elderly, I: a conceptual review. J Am Geriatr Soc 34:711–721, 1986
272
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Cohn CK, Shrivastava R, Mendels J, et al: Double-blind multicenter comparison of sertraline and amitryptyline in elderly depressed patients. J Clin Psychiatry 51:28–33, 1990 Committee on Nursing Home Regulation: Improving the Quality of Care in Nursing Homes. Washington, DC, National Academy of Sciences, Institute of Medicine, 1986 Cooper AJ: Medroxyprogesterone acetate as a treatment for sexual acting out in organic brain syndrome. Am J Psychiatry 145:1179–1180, 1988 Corey-Bloom J, Anand R, Veach J: A randomized trial evaluating the efficacy and safety of ENA 713 (rivastigmine tartrate), a new acetylcholinesterase inhibitor, in patients with mild to moderately severe Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychopharmacol 1:55–65, 1998 Coupland NJ, Bell CJ, Potokar JP: Serotonin reuptake inhibitor withdrawal. J Clin Psychopharmacol 16:356–362, 1996 Court J, Martin-Ruiz C, Piggott M, et al: Nicotinic receptor abnormalities in Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 49:175–184, 2001 Crismon ML: Pharmacokinetics and drug interactions of cholinesterase inhibitors administered in Alzheimer’s disease. Pharmacotherapy 18 (suppl 2):47S–54S, 1998 Cummings JL: Cholinesterase inhibitors: a new class of psychotropic compounds. Am J Psychiatry 157:4–15, 2000 Cummings JL, Back C: The cholinergic hypothesis of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 6 (suppl 1):S64–S78, 1998 Cummings JL, Kaufer D: Neuropsychiatric aspects of Alzheimer’s disease: the cholinergic hypothesis revisited. Neurology 47:876–883, 1996 Cummings JL, Knopman D: Advances in the treatment of behavioral disturbances in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 53:899–901, 1999 Cummings JL, Gorman DG, Shapira J: Physostigmine alleviates the delusions of Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 33:536–541, 1993 Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, et al: The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: an efficient tool for comprehensively assessing psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 44:2308–2314, 1994 Cummings JL, Donohue JA, Brooks RL: The relationship between donepezil and behavioral disturbances in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 8:134–140, 2000 Cummings JL, Street J, Masterman D, et al: Efficacy of olanzapine in the treatment of psychosis in dementia with lewy bodies. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 13:67–73, 2002 Davidson J: Seizures and bupropion: a review. J Clin Psychiatry 50:256–261, 1989 Davidson M, Harvey PD, Vervarcke J, et al: A long-term multicenter, open-label study of risperidone in elderly patients with psychosis. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 15:506–514, 2000 Davies DF, Shock N: Age changes in glomerular filtration rate, effective renal plasma flow, and the tubular excretory capacity in adult males. J Clin Invest 29:496–506, 1950
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
273
Davies P, Maloney AJF: Selective loss of central cholinergic neurons in Alzheimer’s disease (letter). Lancet 2:1403, 1976 Davis KL, Mohs RC, Tinklenberg JR, et al: Physostigmine: improvement of longterm memory processes in normal humans. Science 201:272–274, 1978 Davis KL, Mohs RC, Marin D, et al: Cholinergic markers in elderly patients with early signs of Alzheimer disease. JAMA 281:1401–1406, 1999 Davis RJ, Cummings JL: Clinical variants of tardive dyskinesia. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology 1:31–38,1988 de Boer T: The pharmacological profile of mirtazapine. J Clin Psychiatry 57 (suppl 4):19–25, 1996 DeLamos GP, Clements WR, Nickels E: Effect of diazepam suspension in geriatric patients hospitalized for psychiatric illness. J Am Geriatr Soc 13:355–359, 1965 DeDeyn PP, Rabheru K, Rasmussen A, et al: A randomized trial of risperidone, placebo, and haloperidol for behavioral symptoms of dementia. Neurology 53: 946–955, 1999 Dierick M: An open-label evaluation of the long-term safety of oral venlafaxine in depressed elderly patients. Ann Clin Psychiatry 8:169–178, 1996 Dolly FR, Block AJ: Effects of flurazepam on sleep-disordered breathing and nocturnal oxygen desaturation in asymptomatic subjects. Am J Med 73:239–243, 1982 Doody R, Geldmacher DS, Gordon B, et al: Open-label, multicenter, phase 3 extension study of the safety and efficacy of donepezil in patients with Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 58:427–433, 2001 Drachman DA, Leavitt J: Human memory and the cholinergic system. Arch Neurol 30:113–121, 1974 Druckenbrod R, Mulsant BH: Fluoxetine-induced syndrome of inappropriate antidiuretic hormone secretion: a geriatric case report and review of the literature. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 7:255–258, 1994 Dunayevich E, Strakowski SM: Olanzapine-induced tardive dystonia (letter). Am J Psychiatry 156:1662, 1999 Easton JD: Clinical aspects of the use of clopidogrel, a new antiplatelet agent. Semin Thromb Hemost 25 (suppl 2):77–82, 1999 Fava M: Psychopharmacologic treatment of pathologic aggression. Psychiatr Clin North Am 20:427–451, 1997 Fernandez F, Adams F, Levy JK: Cognitive impairment of AIDS-related complex and its response to psychostimulants. Psychosomatics 28:38–46, 1988a Fernandez F, Levy JK, Galizzi H: Response of HIV-related depression to psychostimulants: case reports. Hosp Community Psychiatry 39:628–631, 1988b Ferrini-Strambi L, Zucconi M: REM sleep behavior disorder. Clin Neurophysiol 111 (suppl 2):S136–S140, 2000 Fields WS, Lemak NA, Frankowski RF, et al: Controlled trial of aspirin in cerebral ischemia, II: surgical group. Stroke 9:309–319, 1978 Finkel SI, Burns A: Introduction. Behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD): a clinical and research update. Int Psychogeriatr 12 (suppl 1):9–12, 2000
274
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Finkel SI, Lyons JS, Anderson RL, et al: A randomized, placebo-controlled trial of thiothixene in agitated, demented nursing home patients. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 10:129–136, 1995 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189– 198, 1975 Forbes CD: Secondary stroke prevention with low-dose aspirin, sustained release dipyridamole alone and in combination. Thromb Res 92 (suppl 1):S1–S6, 1998 Francis J: Delirium in older patients. J Am Geriatr Soc 40:829–838, 1992 Frenchman IB, Capo C, Kass H: Effect of treatment with divalproex sodium and lorazepam in residents of long-term care facilities with dementia-related anxiety or agitation: retrospective chart review. Curr Ther Res 61:621–629, 2000 Gardos G, Cole JO, Salomon M, et al: Clinical forms of severe tardive dyskinesia. Am J Psychiatry 144:895–902, 1986 Gelenberg AJ, Van Putten T, Lavori PW, et al: Anticholinergic effects on memory: benztropine versus amantadine. J Clin Psychopharmacol 9:180–185, 1989 Gent M, Blakely JA, Easton JD: The Canadian American Ticlopidine Study (CATS) in thromboembolic stroke. Lancet 1:1215–1220, 1989 George CFP: Perspectives on the management of insomnia in patients with chronic respiratory disorders. Sleep 23 (suppl 1):S31–S35, 2000 Georgotas A, McCue RF, Friedman E, et al: Response of depressive symptoms to nortriptyline, phenelzine and placebo. Br J Psychiatry 151:102–106, 1987 Gershon S: The use of lithium salts in psychiatric disorders. Dis Nerv Syst 29:51– 62, 1968 Gerson SC, Plotkin DA, Jarvik LF: Antidepressant drug studies, 1964 to 1985: empirical evidence for aging patients. J Clin Psychopharmacol 8:311–322, 1988 Ghelber D, Bellmaker RH: Tardive dyskinesia with quetiapine. Am J Psychiatry 156:796–797, 1999 Gottfries CG, Brane G, Gullberg B, et al: A new rating scale for dementia syndromes. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 1:311–330, 1982 Gottfries CG, Adolfsson R, Aquilonius SM, et al: Biochemical changes in dementia disorders of the Alzheimer type (AD/SDAT). Neurobiol Aging 4:261–271, 1983 Grad RM: Benzodiazepines for insomnia in community-dwelling elderly: a review of benefit and risk. J Fam Pract 41:473–481, 1995 Granacher RP, Baldessarini RJ: The usefulness of physostigmine in neurology and psychiatry, in Clinical Neuropharmacology, Vol 1. Edited by Klawans HL. New York, Raven, 1976, pp. 63–79 Granato JE, Stern BJ, Ringel A, et al: Neuroleptic malignant syndrome: successful treatment with dantrolene and bromocriptine. Ann Neurol 14:89–90, 1983 Granger A: Olanzapine: extrapyramidal side effects in the elderly. Aust N Z J Med 29:371–372, 1999 Greenberg AS, Coleman M: Depressed 5-hydroxyindol associated with hyperactive and aggressive behavior. Arch Gen Psychiatry 33:331–336, 1976
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
275
Greendyke RM, Kanter DR, Schuster DB, et al: Propranolol treatment of assaultive patients with organic brain disease. A double-blind, crossover, placebocontrolled study. J Nerv Ment Dis 174:290–294, 1986 Greenwald BS, Kramer-Ginsberg E, Marin DB, et al: Dementia with coexistent major depression. Am J Psychiatry 146:1472–1478, 1989 Guilleminault C, Eldridge FL, Dement WC: Insomnia with sleep apnea: a new syndrome. Science 181:856–858, 1973 Gury C, Canceil O, Iaria P: Antipsychotic drugs and cardiovascular safety: current studies of prolonged QT interval and risk of ventricular arrhythmia. Encephale 26:62–72, 2000 Guy W: ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology. Rockville, MD, U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1976, pp 218–222 Hales RE, Silver JM, Yudofsky SC: Beta-blocking agents and the treatment of aggression, in Alzheimer’s Disease: Treatment and Long-Term Management. Edited by Cummings JL, Miller BH. New York, Marcel Dekker, 1990, pp 109–121 Hamilton M: Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. Br J Soc Clin Psychol 6:278–296, 1967 Harris MG, Benfield P: Fluoxetine: a review of its pharmacodynamic and pharmacokinetic properties, and therapeutic use in older adults. Drugs Aging 6:64–84, 1995 Hasan S, Buckley P: Novel antipsychotics and the neuroleptic malignant syndrome: a review and critique. Am J Psychiatry 155:1113–1116, 1998 Hashmi FH, Krady AI, Qayum F, et al: Sexually disinhibited behavior in the cognitively impaired elderly. Clinical Geriatrics 8:61–68, 2000 Haspel T: Beta-blockers and the treatment of aggression. Harv Rev Psychiatry 2:274–281, 1995 Hass WK, Easton JD, Adams HP Jr: A randomized trial comparing ticlopidine hydrochloride with aspirin for the prevention of strokes in high-risk patients. N Engl J Med 321:501–507, 1989 Hawkins JW, Tinklenberg J, Sheikh JI, et al: A retrospective chart review of gabapentin for the treatment of aggressive and agitated behavior in patients with dementias. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 8:221–225, 2000 Health Care Financing Administration: Medicare and Medicaid: resident assessment in long- term care facilities. Federal Register 57:61614–61733, 1992 Hedner J, Yaeche R, Emilien G, et al: Zaleplon shortens sleep latency and improves subjective sleep quality in elderly patients with insomnia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 15:704–712, 2000 Herrmann N: Valproic acid treatment of agitation in dementia. Can J Psychiatry 43:69–72, 1998 Herrmann N, Lanctot K, Myszak M: Effectiveness of gabapentin for the treatment of behavioral disorders in dementia. J Clin Psychpharmacol 20:90–93, 2000 Herrmann W: The importance of hyperhomocysteinemia as a risk factor for diseases: an overview. Clin Chem Lab Med 39:666–674, 2001 Holm KJ, Goa KL: Zolpidem: an update of its pharmacology, therapeutic efficacy and tolerability in the treatment of insomnia. Drugs 59:865–889, 2000
276
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Horowitz GJ, Tariot PN, Mead K, et al: Discontinuation of antipsychotics in nursing home patients with dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 3:290–299, 1995 Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL: A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry 140:566–572, 1982 Hyttel J: Pharmacological characterization of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 9 (suppl 1):19–26, 1994 Insel TR, Roy BF, Cohen RM, et al: Possible development of the serotonin syndrome in man. Am J Psychiatry 139:954–955, 1982 Jann MW: Rivastigmine: a new-generation cholinesterase inhibitor for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Pharmacotherapy 20:1–12, 2000 Jarventausta K, Leinonen E: Neuroleptic malignant syndrome during olanzapine and levopromazine treatment. Acta Psychiatr Scand 102:231–233, 2000 Jarvis B, Simpson K: Clopidogrel: a review of its use in the prevention of atherothrombosis. Drugs 60:347–377, 2000 Jenike MA: Handbook of Geriatric Psychopharmacology. Littleton, MA, PSG Publishing, 1985 Jeste DV, Eastham JH, Lohr JB, et al: Treatment of disordered behavior and psychosis, in Clinical Geriatric Psychopharmacology. Edited by Salzman C. Baltimore, MD, Williams & Wilkins, 1998, pp 106–149 Jeste DV, Lacro JP, Palmer B, et al: Incidence of tardive dyskinesia in early stages of low-dose treatment with typical neuroleptics in older persons. Am J Psychiatry 156:309–311, 1999 Jeste DV, Okamoto A, Napolitano J, et al: Low frequency of tardive dyskinesia in elderly patients with dementia treated with risperidone. Am J Psychiatry 157:1150–1155, 2000 Jibson MD, Tandon R: New atypical antipsychotic medications. J Psychiatr Res 32:215–228, 1998 Katz IR, Simpson GM, Jethanandani V, et al: Steady state pharmacokinetics of nortriptyline in the frail elderly. Neuropsychopharmacology 2:229–236, 1989 Katz IR, Sands LP, Bilker W, et al: Identification of medications that cause cognitive impairment in older people: the case of oxybutynin chloride. J Am Geriatr Soc 46:8–13, 1998 Katz IR, Jeste DV, Mintzer JE, et al: Comparison of risperidone and placebo for psychosis and behavior disturbances associated with dementia: a randomized, double-blind trial. J Clin Psychiatry 60:107–115, 1999 Kaufer D, Cummings JL, Christine D: Differential neuropsychiatric symptom responses to tacrine in Alzheimer’s disease: relationship to dementia severity. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 10:55–63, 1998 Keck PE Jr, Arnold LM: The serotonin syndrome. Psychiatr Ann 30:333–343, 2000 Keck PE Jr, Pope HG Jr, McElroy SL: Frequency and presentation of neuroleptic malignant syndrome: a prospective study. Am J Psychiatry 144:1344–1346, 1987 Knopman D, Schneider L, Davis K, et al: Long-term tacrine (Cognex) treatment: effects on nursing home placement and mortality. Neurology 47:166–177, 1996
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
277
Knopman D, Schneider L, Davis K, et al: Long-term tacrine treatment effects. Neurology 50:567–568, 1998 Kummer J, Guendel L, Linden J, et al: Long-term polysomnographic study of the efficacy and safety of zolpidem in elderly psychiatric inpatients with insomnia. J Int Med Res 21:171–184, 1993 Lanctot KL, Best TS, Mittman N, et al: Efficacy and safety of neuroleptics in behavioral disorders associated with dementia. J Clin Psychiatry 59:550–561, 2000 LaRue A, Spar J, Hill CD: Cognitive impairment in late-life depression: clinical correlates and treatment implications. J Affect Disord 11:179–184, 1986 Learoyd BM: Psychotropic drugs and the elderly patient. Med J Aust 1:1131–1133, 1972 LeBars PL, Katz MM, Berman N, et al: A placebo-controlled, double-blind, randomized trial of an extract of Ginkgo biloba in dementia. JAMA 278:1327– 1332, 1997 Lethem R, Orrell, M: Antioxidants and dementia. Lancet 349:1189–1190, 1997 Levitsky AM, Owens NJ: Pharmacologic treatment of hypersexuality and paraphilias in nursing home residents. J Am Geriatr Soc 47:231–234, 1999 Lipinski JF, Zubenko GS, Cohen BM, et al: Propranolol and the treatment of neuroleptic-induced akathisia. Am J Psychiatry 141:412–415, 1984 Littrell KH, Johnson CG, Littrell S, et al: Marked reduction of tardive dyskinesia with olanzapine. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:279–280, 1998 Liu DD, Raji M, Twersky J, et al: Case report: disruptive vocalization due to gout in an elderly nursing home resident with dementia. Annals of Long-Term Care 8:66–70, 2000 Lodwick A: Warfarin therapy: a review of the literature since the Fifth American College of Chest Physicians’ Consensus Conference on Antithrombotic Therapy. Clin Appl Thromb Hemost 5:208–215, 1999 Lothstein LM, Fogg-Waberski J, Reynolds P: Risk management and treatment of sexual disinhibition in geriatric patients. Conn Med 61:609–618, 1997 Lyketsos CG, Sheppard JM, Steele CD, et al: Randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind clinical trial of sertraline in the treatment of depression complicating Alzheimer’s disease: initial results from the Depression in Alzheimer’s Disease Study. Am J Psychiatry 157:1686–1689, 2000 Maes M, Meltzer HY: The serotonin hypothesis of major depression, in Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress. Edited by Bloom FE, Kupfer DJ. New York, Raven, 1995, pp 933–944 Magnuson TM, Roccaforte WH, Wengel SP, et al: Medication-induced dystonias in nine patients with dementia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 12:219–225, 2000 Marsden CD, Harrison MJG: Outcome of investigation of patients with presenile dementia. Br Med J 2:249–252, 1972 McCarten JR, Kovera C, Maddox MK, et al: Triazolam in Alzheimer’s disease: pilot study on sleep and memory effects. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 52:447–452, 1995
278
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
McEvoy JP, McCue M, Spring B, et al: Effect of amantadine and trihexyphenidyl on memory in elderly normal volunteers. Am J Psychiatry 144:573–577, 1987 McKeith I, Del Ser T, Spano P, et al: Efficacy of rivastigmine in dementia with Lewy bodies: a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled international study. Lancet 356:2031–2036, 2000a McKeith IG, Grace JB, Walker Z, et al: Rivastigmine in the treatment of dementia with Lewy bodies: preliminary findings from an open trial. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 15:387–392, 2000b McManus DQ, Arvanitis LA, Kowalcyk BB, et al: Quetiapine, a novel antipsychotic: experience with elderly patients with psychotic disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 60:292–298, 1999 Mega MS, Masterman DM, O’Connor SM, et al: The spectrum of responses to cholinesterase inhibitor therapy in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 56:1388– 1393, 1999 Mourilhe P, Stokes PE: Risks and benefits of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in the treatment of depression. Drug Safety 18:57–82, 1998 Mulsant BH, Pollock BG, Rosen J: Newer antidepressants and antipsychotics in the nursing home. Nursing Home Medicine 5:240–247, 1997 Muth EA, Haskins JT, Moyer JA, et al: Antidepressant chemical profile of the novel bicyclic compound Wy-45,030, an ethyl cyclohexanol derivative. Biochem Pharmacol 35:4493–4497, 1986 Nelson JP, Rosenberg DR: ECT treatment of demented elderly patients with major depression: a retrospective study of efficacy and safety. Convuls Ther 7:157– 165, 1991 Nemeroff CB, DeVane CL, Pollock BG: Newer antidepressants and the cytochrome P450 system. Am J Psychiatry 153:311–430, 1996 Newhouse PA, Krishnan KR, Doraiswamy PM, et al: A double-blind comparison of sertraline and fluoxetine in depressed elderly outpatients. J Clin Psychiatry 61:559–568, 2000 Novartis Pharmaceuticals: Important drug warning (letter). July 7, 2000. Available at http://www.fda.gov/medwatch/safety/2000/mellar.htm Nyth AL, Gottfries GC: The clinical efficacy of citalopram in treatment of emotional disturbances in dementia disorders: a Nordic multicentre study. Br J Psychiatry 157:894–901, 1990 Nyth AL, Gottfries CG, Lyby K, et al: A controlled multicenter clinical study of citalopram and placebo in elderly depressed patients with and without dementia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 86:138–145, 1992 Oken BS, Storzbach DM, Kaye JA: The efficacy of Ginkgo biloba on cognitive function in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 55:1409–1415, 1998 Ott BR: Leuprolide treatment of sexual aggression in a patient with dementia and the Klüver-Bucy syndrome. Clin Neuropharmacol 18:443–447, 1995 Overall JE, Gorham DR: The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS): recent developments in ascertainment and scaling. Psychopharmacol Bull 24:97–98, 1988
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
279
Patterson MB, Mack JL, Mackell JA, et al: A longitudinal study of behavioral pathology across five levels of dementia severity in Alzheimer’s disease: the CERAD Behavior Rating Scale for Dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 11 (suppl 2):S40–S44, 1997 Pearlman CA: Neuroleptic malignant syndrome: a review of the literature. J Clin Psychopharmacol 6:257–273, 1986 Perry EK, Perry RH, Blessed G, et al: Necropsy evidence of central cholinergic deficits in senile dementia (letter). Lancet 1:189, 1977 Perry EK, Kerwin J, Perry RH, et al: Cerebral cholinergic activity is related to the incidence of visual hallucinations in senile dementia of Lewy body type. Dementia 1:2–4, 1990 Petracca G, Tesón A, Chemerinski E, et al: A double-blind placebo-controlled study of clomipramine in depressed patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 8:270–275, 1996 Pollock BG, Mulsant BH, Sweet R, et al: An open pilot study of citalopram for behavioral disturbances in dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 5:70–78, 1997 Pollock BG, Mulsant BH, Nebes R, et al: Serum anticholinergicity in elderly depressed patients treated with paroxetine or nortriptyline. Am J Psychiatry 155:1110–1112, 1998 Porsteinsson AP, Tariot PN, Erb R, et al: An open trial of valproate for agitation in geriatric neuropsychiatric disorders. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 5:344–351, 1997 Porsteinsson AP, Tariot PN, Erb R, et al: Placebo-controlled study of divalproex sodium for agitation in dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 9:1–9, 2001 Ramadan FH, Naughton BJ: Paroxetine for verbal agitation. Annals of Long-Term Care 7:282–286, 1999 Raskind MA, Sadowsky CH, Sigmund WR, et al: Effect of tacrine on language, praxis, and noncognitive behavioral problems in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 54:836–840, 1997 Raskind MA, Peskind ER, Wessel T, et al: Galantamine in AD: a 6-month randomized, placebo-controlled trial with a 6-month extension. Neurology 54:2261– 2268, 2000 Reifler BV, Teri L, Raskind M, et al: Double-blind trial of imipramine in Alzheimer’s disease patients with and without depression. Am J Psychiatry 146:45– 49, 1989 Reisberg B, Borenstein J, Salob SP, et al: Behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease: phenomenology and treatment. J Clin Psychiatry 48 (suppl 5):9–15, 1987 Reynolds CF, Perel JM, Kupfer DJ, et al: Open-trial response to antidepressant treatment in elderly patients with mixed depression and cognitive impairment. Psychiatry Res 21:111–122, 1987 Ringman JM, Simmons JH: Treatment of REM sleep behavior disorder with donepezil: a report of three cases. Neurology 55:870–871, 2000 Roane DM, Feinberg TE, Meckler L, et al: Treatment of dementia-associated agitation with gabapentin. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 12:40–43, 2000 Robinson DS: Changes in monoamine oxidase and monoamines in human development and aging. Fed Proc 34:103–107, 1975
280
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Robinson DS, Davis JN, Nies A, et al: Aging, monoamines and MAO levels. Lancet 1:290–291, 1972 Robinson D, Napoliello MJ, Schenk J: The safety and usefulness of buspirone as an anxiolytic drug in the elderly versus young patients. Clin Ther 10:740–746, 1990 Rogers SL, Friedhoff LT: The efficacy and safety of donepezil in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: results of a U.S. multicentre, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Dementia 7:293–303, 1996 Rogers SL, Farlow MR, Doody RS, et al: A 24-week, double-blind, placebocontrolled trial of donepezil in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 50:136–145, 1998 Rosen WG, Mohs RC, Davis KL: A new rating scale for Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 141:1356–1364, 1984 Rosenblatt A, Leroi I: Neuropsychiatry of Huntington’s disease and other basal ganglia disorders. Psychosomatics 41:24–30, 2000 Rösler M, Anand R, Cicin-Sain A, et al: Efficacy and safety of rivastigmine in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: international randomised controlled trial. Br Med J 318:633–640, 1999 Rupniak NMJ, Jenner P, Marsden CD: Acute dystonia caused by neuroleptic drugs. Psychopharmacology 88:403–419, 1986 Sackeim HA: Electroconvulsive therapy in late-life depression, in Clinical Geriatric Psychopharmacology. Edited by Salzman C. Baltimore, MD, Williams & Wilkins, 1998, pp 262–309 Salzman C (ed): Clinical Geriatric Psychopharmacology, 3rd Edition. Baltimore, MD, Williams & Wilkins, 1998 Salzman C, Vaccaro B, Lieff J, et al: Clozapine in older patients with psychosis and behavioral disruption. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 3:26–33, 1995 Sano M, Ernesto C, Klauber MR, et al: Rationale and design of a multicenter study of selegiline and alpha-tocopherol in the treatment of Alzheimer disease using novel outcome measures. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 10:132– 140, 1996 Sano M, Ernesto C, Thomas RG, et al: A controlled trial of selegiline, alpha-tocopherol, or both as treatment for Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 336:1216– 1222, 1997 Schmidt AW, Lebel LA, Howard HR, et al: Ziprasidone: a novel antipsychotic agent with a unique human binding profile. Eur J Pharmacol 425:197–201, 2001 Schneider LS, Sobin P: Treatments for psychiatric symptoms and behavioral disturbances in dementia, in Dementia. Edited by Burns A, Levy R. London, Chapman & Hall, 1994, pp 517–538 Schneider LS, Pollock VE, Lyness SA: A metaanalysis of controlled trials of neuroleptic treatment of dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 38:553–563, 1990 Schneider LS, Olin JT, Doody RS, et al: Validity and reliability of the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study—clinical global impression of change. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 11 (suppl 2):S22–S32, 1997
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
281
Schneider LS, Yeung PP, Sweitzer DE, et al: Quetiapine may reduce hostility in patients with psychosis related to Alzheimer’s disease. Schizophr Res 36:295– 296, 1999 Schone W, Ludwig M: A double-blind study of paroxetine compared with fluoxetine in geriatric patients with major depression. J Clin Psychopharmacol 13:34S–39S, 1993 Schwartz RS, Shuman WP, Bradbury VL, et al: Body fat distribution in healthy young and older men. J Gerontol 45:M181–M185, 1990 Schweizer E, Case WG, Rickels K: Benzodiazepine dependence and withdrawal in elderly patients. Am J Psychiatry 146:529–531, 1989 Segraves RT, Kavoussi R, Hughes AR, et al: Evaluation of sexual functioning in depressed outpatients: a double-blind comparison of sustained-release bupropion and sertraline treatment. J Clin Psychopharmacol 20:122–128, 2000 Shankle WR, Nielson KA, Cotman CW: Low-dose propranolol reduces aggression and agitation resembling that associated with orbitofrontal dysfunction in elderly demented patients. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 9:233–237, 1995 Shelton PS, Hocking LB: Zolpidem for dementia-related insomnia and nighttime wandering. Ann Pharmacother 31:319–322, 1997 Showalter PEC, Kimmel DN: Agitated symptom response to divalproex following brain injury. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 12:395–397, 2000 Simpson DM, Foster D: Improvement in organically disturbed behavior with trazodone treatment. J Clin Psychiatry 47:191–193, 1986 Small GW, Salzman C: Treatment of depression with new and atypical antidepressants, in Clinical Geriatric Psychopharmacology. Edited by Salzman C. Baltimore, MD, Williams & Wilkins, 1998, pp 245–261 Smith JM, Kocen RS: A Creutzfeldt-Jakob–like syndrome due to lithium toxicity. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 51:120–123, 1988 Solomon PR, Adams F, Silver A, et al: Ginkgo for memory enhancement: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 288:835–840, 2002 Sparks DL, Marksbery WR: Altered serotonergic and cholinergic synaptic markers in Pick’s disease. Arch Neurol 48:796–799, 1991 Street JS, Clark WS, Gannon KS, et al: Olanzapine treatment of psychotic and behavioral symptoms in patients with Alzheimer disease in nursing care facilities: a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57:968–976, 2000 Sultzer DL, Gray KF, Gunay I, et al: A double-blind comparison of trazodone and haloperidol for treatment of agitation in patients with dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 5:60–69, 1997 Swartz JR, Miller BL, Lesser IM, et al: Frontotemporal dementia: treatment response to serotonin selective reuptake inhibitors. J Clin Psychiatry 58:212– 216, 1997 Swedish Cooperative Study Group: High dose acetylsalicylic acid after cerebral infarction. Stroke 18:325–334, 1987 Sweet RA, Mulsant BH, Gupta B, et al: Duration of treatment and prevalence of tardive dyskinesia in late life. Arch Gen Psychiatry 52:478–486, 1995
282
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Swift CG, Swift MR, Ankover SI, et al: Single dose pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of loprazolam in the elderly. Br J Clin Pharmacol 20:119–128, 1985 Taragano FE, Lyketsos CG, Mangone CA, et al: A double-blind, randomized, fixed-dose trial of fluoxetine vs. amitriptyline in the treatment of major depression complicating Alzheimer’s disease. Psychosomatics 38:246–252, 1997 Tariot PN, Mack JL, Patterson MB, et al: The Behavior Rating Scale for Dementia of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease. The Behavioral Pathology Committee of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease. Am J Psychiatry 152:1349–1357, 1995 Tariot PN, Jakimovich IJ, Erb R, et al: Withdrawal from controlled carbamazepine therapy followed by further carbamazepine treatment in patients with dementia. J Clin Psychiatry 60:684–689, 1999 Tariot PN, Solomon PR, Morris JC, et al: A 5-month, randomized, placebocontrolled trial of galantamine in AD. Neurology 54:2269–2276, 2000 Teri L, Logsdon RG, Peskind E, et al: Treatment of agitation in Alzheimer’s disease patients: a randomized placebo-controlled clinical trial. Neurology 55:1271– 1278, 2000 Thal LJ, Carta A, Clarke WR, et al: A 1-year multicenter placebo-controlled study of acetyl-L-carnitine in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 47:705– 711, 1996 Trollor JN, Sachdev PS: Electroconvulsive treatment of neuroleptic malignant syndrome: review and report of cases. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 33:650–659, 1999 Varga E, Sugerman AA, Varga V, et al: Prevalence of spontaneous oral dyskinesia in the elderly. Am J Psychiatry 139:329–331, 1982 Vestal RE, Wood AJ: Influence of age and smoking on drug kinetics in man: studies using model compounds. Clin Pharmacokinet 5:309–319, 1980 Volicer L, Rheaume Y, Cyr D: Treatment of depression in advanced Alzheimer’s disease using sertraline. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 7:227–229, 1994 von Moltke LL, Abernethy DR, Greenblatt DJ: Kinetics and dynamics of psychotropic drugs in the elderly, in Clinical Geriatric Psychopharmacology. Edited by Salzman C. Baltimore, MD, Williams & Wilkins, 1998, pp 70–93 Walker A, Grace J, Overshot R, et al: Olanzapine in dementia with Lewy bodies: a clinical study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 14:459–466, 1999 Weihs KL, Settle EC Jr, Batey SR, et al: Bupropion sustained release versus paroxetine for the treatment of depression in the elderly. J Clin Psychiatry 61:196– 202, 2000 Weiner MF, Davis KL: Anticholinergic drugs, in Drugs in Psychiatry, Vol 4. Edited by Burrows GD, Norman TR, Davies B. Amsterdam, Elsevier, 1986, pp 191– 205 Weiner MF, Denke M, Williams K, et al: Intramuscular medroxyprogesterone acetate for sexual aggression in elderly men. Lancet 339:1121–1122, 1992 Weiner MF, Edland SD, Luszczynska H: Prevalence and incidence of major depression in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 151:1006–1009, 1993
Behavioral, Psychological, and Cognitive Symptoms
283
Weiner MF, Martin-Cook K, Foster BM, et al: Effects of donepezil on emotional/ behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease patients. J Clin Psychiatry 61:487–492, 2000 Wesnes KA, McKeith IG, Ferrara R et al: Effects of rivastigmine on cognitive function in dementia with Lewy bodies: a randomized placebo-controlled international study using the Cognitive Drug Research Computerized Assessment System. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 13:183–193, 2002 Wilcock GK, Lillienfeld S, Gaens E: Efficacy and safety of galantamine in patients with mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease: multicentre randomised controlled trial. BMJ 321:1–7, 2000 Wolters EC, Hurwitz TA, Mak E: Clozapine in the treatment of parkinsonian patients with dopaminomimetic psychosis. Neurology 40:832–840, 1990 Wolters EC, Jansen ENH, Tuynman-Qua HG, et al: Olanzapine in the treatment of dopaminomimetic psychosis in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 47:1085–1087, 1996 Woods SW, Tesar GE, Murray GB, et al: Psychostimulant treatment of depressive disorders secondary to medical illness. J Clin Psychiatry 47:12–15, 1986 Yamamoto T, Hirano A: Nucleus raphe dorsalis in Alzheimer’s disease: neurofibrillary tangles and loss of large neurons. Ann Neurol 17:573–577, 1985
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER
8
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions C. Munro Cullum, Ph.D. Ronald G. Paulman, Ph.D. Elisabeth Koss, Ph.D. Sandra Bond Chapman, Ph.D. Laura Lacritz, Ph.D.
The clinical neuropsychological evaluation plays important roles in detecting and diagnosing dementing illness and in characterizing patients’ cognitive strengths and weaknesses. The information obtained in the evaluation is used in making the differential diagnosis, in planning patient management and disposition, and in providing education and support to patients’ families. Neuropsychological data provide an objective, quantitative means to establish levels of cognitive functioning and produce profiles that can be used to assist in differential diagnosis and in understanding individuals’ functional impairments (Naugle et al. 1998; Zakzanis et al. 1999). Neuropsychological evaluation techniques are recognized and accepted neurodiagnostic procedures (American Academy of Neurology Task Force 1996) and are the most sensitive existing means of evaluating human brain function and cognitive abilities. Because the oldest age groups are the most rapidly growing segment of society and because of the association between age and dementing illness, the use of neuropsychological testing in persons with known or suspected dementing illness is one of the most important roles for modern clinical neuropsychology. The inclusion of cognitive screening instruments and neuropsychological test batteries is considered routine to establish cognitive criteria for Alzheimer’s disease (McKhann et al. 1984) and was recently included as a recommended practice guideline for the early detection of dementing illness (Petersen et al. 2001). In fact, detailed neuropsychometric testing has shown high sensitivity and specificity in detecting dementing illness (Becker et al. 1994). 285
286
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Goals of Neuropsychological Assessment Characterization of Cognitive Functioning The neuropsychological evaluation provides objective information that relates to a variety of everyday functional tasks and can help address important practical questions such as the patient’s ability to understand procedures, make independent decisions, manage finances, function on a job, and drive a car. Neuropsychological testing places individuals in a structured situation requiring effortful processing of both new and previously acquired information. By examining performance on standardized tasks, clinicians may use neuropsychological data to help assess how individuals may be expected to perform in occupational or psychosocial settings. For example, this can help to determine if a business executive continues to possess the higher-order organizational abilities required to manage a department with multiple simultaneous needs. At the other end of the spectrum, it may also help predict whether a widowed elderly woman will be able to meet her needs in an independent living situation, or whether she needs assistance or requires a more structured placement. Thus, the neuropsychological evaluation provides a quantified, comprehensive characterization of neurobehavioral abilities that may confirm and extend observations and impressions derived from physicians, family, and other caregivers.
Role of Cognitive Assessment: Neurodiagnosis An understanding of the cognitive patterns and changes associated with normal aging is an important prerequisite to identifying dementing illnesses in older adults. Although improved norms for many neuropsychological tests have been published over the past two decades, additional knowledge regarding what constitutes “normal” cognition among the oldest old is needed. Cognitive decline generally does not become apparent until after age 70, and this decline is not uniform across cognitive abilities. Decreases in activities requiring executive function, visuospatial processing, mental speed, and psychomotor speed are most notable, whereas relative stability is observed for language-related skills (Albert 1988). Memory decline associated with aging has been described as “benign senescent forgetfulness” (Kral 1978) and “age-associated memory impairment” (Crook et al. 1986). However, criteria for applying these terms are not uniform, are too all-encompassing, and do not adequately consider the high degree of individual variability in memory functions among elderly individuals (see Larrabee and McEntee 1995). Whereas the ability to acquire and retain certain types of new information (e.g., visual
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
287
memory) declines with aging, other aspects of memory (e.g., paragraph retention) shows little change (e.g., Cullum et al. 1990; see Verhaeghen et al. 1993 for a review). Cognitive changes observed as part of normal aging are trivial compared with those observed in persons with dementing illness (Koss et al. 1991). Memory loss is by definition in DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association 2000) the common abnormality in all the dementing illnesses. Also present are varying degrees of disturbances in awareness, orientation, insight, general behavior, ability to efficiently access stored information, language function, praxis, visuospatial function, topographical orientation, problem-solving ability, judgment, calculation, and emotion. These nonspecific dysfunctions cover all cognitive domains. It should be noted that changes in some of these areas can be associated with normal aging as well. The intensity, qualitative differences, unique symptom constellation, and progressive nature seen in dementing illness serve to distinguish them from normal aging. As noted in Chapter 1, DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for dementia include impairment in short- and long-term memory and at least one of the following: aphasia, apraxia, agnosia, and impairment in executive functioning. These deficits must also represent a decline from premorbid status and be associated with impairments in everyday activities. As such, an evaluation of several cognitive domains is a necessary part of the assessment of dementing illness. Multilevel assessment of cognitive changes in an individual offers the best opportunity for discriminating between age-related and diseaserelated changes in cognitive status (Albert 1988). A person’s performance on neuropsychological tests is combined with historical, developmental, educational, and medical information (including neurological, psychiatric, and neuroimaging) to assist in the diagnosis of dementing illness. Both the age and education of patients must be taken into account. For example, low performance by an individual of limited premorbid intelligence, education, or occupational attainment may not suggest a dementing condition. Conversely, average performance on cognitive tasks by a university professor may signal the onset of a dementing illness. Thus, the diagnostic process necessitates integration of all information relevant to a patient’s past and present functioning. Furthermore, the pattern of cognitive test performance, including specific pathognomonic signs and error types, may be illuminating in terms of differential diagnosis.
Recommendations for Interventions In addition to describing cognitive function and assisting in diagnosis, neuropsychological evaluation may lead to specific recommendations con-
288
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
cerning further evaluation and treatment. These might include considering neuroimaging studies (if they have not yet been performed); giving behavioral prescriptions to help compensate for specific cognitive deficits such as memory, language, or visuospatial impairments; or designing strategies for management of problem behaviors in the living environment. Results of a neuropsychological evaluation may also be useful to family members in terms of guidance in setting appropriate expectations for their loved one and restructuring their environment to help capitalize on the patient’s cognitive strengths.
Comprehensive Neuropsychological Evaluation A comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation provides an assessment of multiple cognitive domains and abilities. This requires an integration of multiple sources of information, including 1) the patient’s developmental, medical, social, and educational history; 2) collateral information from others familiar with the patient’s recent and past functional status; 3) quantitative and qualitative testing of a range of neurocognitive skills; and 4) assessment of the patient’s emotional status. Elements of these examinations are described in more detail in the following sections.
History The past and present context of a person’s life influences the interpretation of neurocognitive evaluation results. Although an objective score on a particular test may be the same for two individuals, the implications of that score may be different based on individual differences such as cultural background and educational or professional achievement. Neuropsychological assessment therefore begins with a systematic inquiry into early medical and developmental history; language acquisition; school performance, including any special needs; military record (if relevant); occupational experience; and acquired insults to the central nervous system such as head trauma, toxin exposures, or other neurological conditions. Current medical history is also important in that conditions such as diabetes, hypertension, vitamin deficiencies, hormonal imbalance, cardiac problems, kidney or liver disease, infections, and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease can influence neuropsychological function (see Chapter 5). Past psychiatric history and treatment are also important, in addition to current medication regimen. Functions such as attention and memory are particularly susceptible to disruption in the elderly by medications such as anxiolytics or antidepressants with anticholinergic side effects (Weiner and Davis 1986). Finally, family history of dementing
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
289
illness should be queried, because some dementing illnesses have a genetic component.
Interviewing Clinical interviewing is covered in Chapters 1 and 2. The availability of external sources of information is important in corroborating information gathered during a patient interview. Observational data from physical appearance, motor function, level of awareness, sensory impairments, and speech are easily obtained in interview. By contrast, a person’s selfreport can be unreliable. Persons presenting for cognitive evaluation may provide erroneous or confabulated answers to questions or may have difficulty finding words or remembering what was asked of them. As a result, it is advisable to have a close relative or caregiver available when conducting an initial mental status examination or clinical interview. Although patients should be questioned directly, it is often advantageous to also interview caregivers separately so that they may feel free to disclose information about the patient that might be upsetting to him or her. It is important to note that caregiver appraisals concerning memory deficits in persons with Alzheimer’s disease frequently correlate well with objective test measures (Koss et al. 1993). However, the interviewer must be aware that families or caregivers present information from their perspective, and therefore may exaggerate the problems or may minimize or deny them.
Neurocognitive Testing Many tests and measures of cognitive functioning have been developed, and many of the more popular tools in clinical use are summarized in texts such as those by Lezak (1995), Snyder and Nussbaum (1998), and Spreen and Strauss (1998). Although several standard batteries of tests have been developed, the important factor in the comprehensive cognitive evaluation of dementia is the inclusion of tests that tap specific cognitive domains. Particular attention must be paid to the areas that are most susceptible to the effects of different types of dementing illness. This generally means an emphasis on memory assessment, in addition to other ability areas commonly involved in different dementing illnesses. As noted, interpretation of the level as well as the pattern of performance, is important, as qualitative features of different tests can be useful in diagnosis (see Cahn et al. 1997; Rosenstein 1998). A list of some common measures used in the comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation of cognitive dysfunction is presented in Table 8–1.
290
TABLE 8–1.
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Typical neuropsychological test battery for dementia with tests listed by cognitive domaina
Global cognitive functioning Mini-Mental State Exam Dementia Rating Scale Intellectual ability Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd Edition (WAIS-III) or Revised (WAIS-R)b Premorbid intellectual level National Adult Reading Test–Revised (NART-R) or Wide Range Achievement Test, 3rd Edition (WRAT-3) Reading test Executive function Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Trail Making Test—Part B Attention Trail Making Test—Part A Digit Span (WAIS-III/R) Language Boston Naming Test Verbal fluency (letter and category fluency) Vocabulary (WAIS-III/R) Visuospatial skills Block Design (WAIS-III/R) Clock drawing test Cross, cube drawings Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Learning and memory California Verbal Learning Test (or other list learning tests such as the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Buschke Selective Reminding Test) Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd Edition (WMS-III) or Revised (WMS-R) Logical Memory, Visual Reproduction subtests Psychomotor abilities Finger tapping test Hand dynamometer Grooved pegboard a
References for tests can be found in Lezak (1995), Snyder and Nussbaum (1998), and Spreen and Strauss (1998). b Selected subtests are often used to provide an estimate of current intellectual status (e.g., Vocabulary, Information, Block Design, Digit Symbol/Coding)
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
291
Cognitive Domains Typically Assessed in the Neuropsychological Evaluation Intellectual Ability and Global Cognitive Status Intellectual assessment is a frequent component of the comprehensive cognitive workup for several reasons. A determination of global cognitive or intellectual status can be used to estimate the extent to which current intellectual functioning likely reflects a decline from presumed premorbid abilities. Furthermore, having an estimate of overall intellectual ability is useful in interpreting the results from other neurocognitive measures. For example, are memory test results lower than expected given an individual’s overall mental ability level? In addition, because clinical measures of intellectual function are composed of multiple subtests, the pattern of subtest scores can be useful to infer cognitive decline in addition to providing information relevant to specific cognitive strengths and weaknesses. For example, the ability to define words tends to maintain its integrity relatively well into the course of Alzheimer’s disease, in contrast to early compromise in the ability to interpret proverbs or assemble three-dimensional blocks to replicate patterns. The Wechsler scales—including the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd Edition (WAIS-III), and the earlier revised version (WAIS-R)— are the most popular means of assessing intellectual capacity in adults and may be useful for detecting cognitive deficits. These measures evaluate academically acquired knowledge in addition to many global verbal and visuomotor problem-solving skills important in daily life. As such, these measures are well-standardized indices of overall cognitive function, comparing an individual’s responses with those of same-age peers. Verbal and nonverbal intellectual abilities are assessed, and composite scores can be derived that provide general descriptors of overall intellectual capacity. The WAIS-III includes a Verbal Comprehension Index and a Perceptual Motor Index, which reflect verbal and nonverbal cognitive skills. Results of intellectual assessment may suggest a decline in intelligence but are not sufficient to make a diagnosis of dementing illness, because many other areas of cognitive functioning are not evaluated by these measures. In fact, intellectual testing is often much less sensitive to acquired cognitive impairment secondary to dementing illness than are many other neurocognitive measures. The estimation of premorbid cognitive functioning is also useful in interpreting current neurocognitive test scores and inferring the presence and extent of cognitive decline. There are a number of ways to obtain such estimates (Kareken 1997), including 1) clinical judgment (given the patient’s education, age, social and occupational background, was he or she
292
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
likely average, above average, or below average premorbidly?); 2) regression-based approaches using demographic data (e.g., Barona et al. 1984); 3) examination of patterns of neuropsychological performance on tests that are generally less sensitive to cognitive decline (vocabulary or word definitions, general fund of information) in comparison with other measures; and 4) use of tests that hold up better in the presence of dementing illness and correlate well with premorbid intelligence. These include sight-word reading tests such as the National Adult Reading Test–Revised (NART-R) (Blair and Spreen 1989) and its variants and the Reading subtest from the Wide Range Achievement Test, 3rd Edition (WRAT-3) (G.S. Wilkinson 1993). The NART-R, for example, has shown reasonably good stability relative to the WAIS-R in Alzheimer’s disease, even as the disease progresses (Maddrey et al. 1996; Paolo et al. 1997). The use of such procedures (often in combination) can provide a reasonable estimate of premorbid cognitive ability, although predictive limitations at the upper and lower ends of the intellectual spectrum restrict the accuracy of these approaches in individual cases. Global cognitive status is typically assessed by using cognitive screening tools, which provide a cursory examination of several cognitive domains yet yield a global score that is useful in the overall characterization of cognitive impairment. Among the assessment instruments commonly used in persons with dementia are the Dementia Rating Scale (Mattis 1988) and the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) (Folstein et al. 1975). Such instruments are often included in drug treatment studies to examine the effects of medications on overall cognitive status and in clinical and research settings to provide a quick, quantitative index of the level of cognitive impairment. Despite the utility of some of these brief screening instruments in distinguishing between demented and nondemented groups (van Gorp et al. 1999), the limitations of such tools are numerous as a result of their brief nature, insensitivity to some forms of dementing illness, and poor sensitivity at both ends of the cognitive continuum (subtle or mild cognitive impairment and severe dementia). In the Alzheimer’s Disease Center at the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, for example, 14% of 673 consecutive patients with a clinical diagnosis of possible or probable Alzheimer’s disease obtained scores in the normal range (higher than 23) on the MMSE (Cullum and Rosenberg 1998). As with more detailed neurocognitive measures, these measures are influenced by demographic factors such as age, education, and ethnicity; as a result, careful interpretation and use of appropriate norms are important when evaluating findings. Despite their limitations, cognitive screening tools can provide quantitative scores that aid not only in diagnosis, but in tracking the progression of dementing illness in a standardized fashion. They are often included in
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
293
neuropsychological test batteries for the reasons just mentioned; furthermore, they may be particularly useful as dementing illness progresses and as patients become less amenable to more detailed cognitive testing. As an intermediate approach to cognitive screening, a brief battery of tests was assembled by the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). This group identified a basic neuropsychological battery (Morris et al. 1989) evaluating memory, language functions, and constructional praxis, with level of difficulty allowing for the evaluation of persons with mild to relatively severe cognitive impairment. The CERAD battery evaluates global cognitive function (MMSE), acquisition of new information (a 10-word list learning task), memory (delayed recall and recognition of the word list plus recall of drawings), language functions (category fluency plus a 15-item version of the Boston Naming Test), and constructional praxis (copying simple geometric figures). The CERAD battery typically requires 30–45 minutes to complete, has available norms to age 90 (Welsh et al. 1994a), and has shown positive correlations with regional cerebral function in Alzheimer’s disease (Welsh et al. 1994b). With progression of dementing illness, scores on many standard tests quickly decline, and this may result in a floor effect. Patients eventually may become untestable using traditional cognitive assessment techniques, and eventually it becomes necessary to rely on clinical observation and informant ratings to obtain information about disease progression. Nevertheless, several tests have been developed to provide quantitative neurobehavioral data in cases of more profound cognitive impairment. For example, the Severe Impairment Battery (Saxton et al. 1990) was developed to evaluate cognitive functioning in persons who may not be amenable to standard neuropsychological assessment. It includes a variety of subtests to evaluate various basic areas and has shown evidence of convergent validity in relation to other formal measures and behavioral observations. Similarly, the Texas Functional Living Scale (TFLS) (Cullum et al. 2001) was designed to provide a brief index of daily functional abilities in persons with dementing illness, with an emphasis on cognitive tasks (e.g., making change, looking up and dialing a telephone number, remembering simple instructions). In preliminary studies, the TFLS has shown good convergent and discriminant validity, with promise as a cognitively oriented measure of independent activities of daily living in persons at various stages of dementing illness.
Attention and Concentration Attention and concentration abilities underlie all higher cognitive processes. Although it is often characterized as a specific ability, attention is a
294
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
highly complex set of functions involving many brain regions, including reticular, thalamic, and frontal systems. In dementing conditions, the finetuned balance between awareness of novel stimuli and the focus on those with greatest relevance is frequently disrupted. Therefore, impaired selective attention in cognitively impaired persons leads them to be either highly distractible or excessively focused, thereby missing important environmental cues. Accompanying problems in sustaining or dividing attention are also usually present (Butters et al. 1987; Lines et al. 1991), even though simple attention may remain grossly intact. Simple attention is often assessed by having patients repeat strings of single-digit numbers (e.g., WAIS-III digit span forward), in addition to observing the degree to which they are able to follow instructions, track conversation, and respond to questions. Such basic attentional skills are often intact in the early stages of dementing illness, and it is not uncommon for patients with Alzheimer’s disease or Korsakoff’s syndrome to demonstrate normal performance on digit span forward tasks, despite the presence of amnesia. Selective attention may be assessed in numerous ways. The ability to track digit sequences backward and perform mental arithmetic problems on the WAIS-III, for example, requires more working memory and selective attention. Selective attention is also required in the discrimination of visual stimuli in the presence of a competing cognitive set, as in the Stroop Color Word Test. Sustained attention may be evaluated by requiring patients to be vigilant for recurrent target stimuli in a long auditory or visual sequence (e.g., Digit Vigilance Test; Lewis and Rennick 1979). The Verbal Series Attention Test (VSAT; Mahurin and Cooke 1997) assesses simple and selective attention using a variety of brief, repeatable tasks, as does the Mental Control subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd Edition (WMS-III; Wechsler 1997) and its precursors. Divided attention tasks may require the patient to perform two tasks simultaneously (Paulman and Kennelly 1984) or to respond alternately on a number-letter sequencing test. Because the construct of attention represents a hierarchical set of abilities, persons with mild dementing illness may demonstrate little or no impairment in simple or selective attention, although they may be unable to divide or alternate their attentional focus. Conversely, persons with moderate to severe levels of cognitive impairment have difficulty with all levels of attentional processing.
Executive Functions Loss of executive processing abilities, such as organizing, planning, and monitoring one’s problem-solving behaviors, is common in dementing ill-
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
295
ness. Executive functions are thought to be primarily mediated by anterior brain systems, although damage to other brain systems may produce deficits on the complex tests used to assess this construct. As this set of abilities represents the highest level of cognitive control and planning, executive functions rely heavily on a variety of supportive cognitive skills. Similarly, executive functions may mediate other ability areas such as memory (Troyer et al. 1994). Difficulties with planning, reasoning, and inhibition may be manifested in many ways, and standardized assessment of this complex set of skills has proved challenging. Most commonly, however, these abilities are examined by tests that rely heavily on novel problem-solving skills. Commonly used measures include the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Heaton 1981), the Category Test (Reitan and Wolfson 1985), and the recently published Delis-Kaplan Executive Function Test (2001) and its component tests. Most of these instruments require patients to generate and test hypotheses while using success-failure feedback to flexibly alter responses with changing task demands. Other tasks that rely on some degree of flexible generation (e.g., word fluency) or inhibition and the switching of mental sets (e.g., Trail Making Test Part B [Reitan and Wolfson 1985], Stroop Test [Trenerry et al. 1989], go–no-go tasks) are also commonly used to evaluate persons with known or suspected dementing illness. Tests of verbal reasoning and judgment can also be used to assess aspects of executive function, but as with other tasks, these tests may show impairment resulting from a variety of reasons besides impairment of executive function.
Memory Memory impairment is a sine qua non of dementia in DSM-IV-TR, and it is typically the earliest and most severe symptom in Alzheimer’s disease. However, memory complaints are also frequently reported by depressed individuals and healthy elderly adults. The clinician has the task of determining whether a patient’s memory problem is due to a neuropathological process, a psychiatric disturbance, or a normal variation. Memory is a higher-order set of abilities that includes the acquisition, storage, and retrieval of information. As such, memory is another complex construct that requires the integration of many functional systems of the brain. Some inconsistency exists in the terminology used to describe memory tasks and abilities, and a number of functional dichotomies have been posited (see Squire 1986 and Schacter and Tulving 1994 for reviews). Another important distinction related to clinical memory assessment is the concept of recall versus recognition. Free recall of information is required on an essay type of examination, wherein subjects are asked to
296
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
remember information presented earlier. In contrast, recognition memory is evaluated in a multiple-choice type of test, wherein subjects must choose the correct response from an array of stimuli. Problems in recognition memory are often related to faulty retrieval rather than encoding: information may be learned, but problems may occur with the active retrieval of the stored material. This distinction can be useful in differentiating between cortical and subcortical disorders, between cognitive impairment associated with depression and dementia, or between Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia (FTD). Persons with a cortical dementia such as Alzheimer’s disease often exhibit difficulties in encoding, free recall, and recognition memory, whereas depressed persons and those with subcortical disorders more frequently show only deficits in free recall and show significant improvement on recognition testing. One important factor in clinical memory assessment pertains to the nature and complexity of the test. For example, although very brief tasks such as three-word recall (as included in the MMSE) may distinguish large groups of demented subjects from nondemented subjects, the results of such tests may vary widely within the normal population and in individual cases (Cullum et al. 1993). Other related factors that may influence results include the instructional set provided at the time of stimulus presentation (explicit versus implicit recall), the nature of the stimuli (simple versus complex words or ideas), the provision of cues during recall, and the number of pieces of information to be recalled. For example, longer memory tests may be needed to better elicit some of the characteristic errors during recall that typify some dementing illnesses (Kaltreider et al. 2000). No single test of memory is capable of tapping all of the dimensions of memory. Most assess episodic memory, the ability to recall information within a temporal context, such as remembering recently presented information. Along these lines, formal memory tests, particularly those assessing delayed verbal recall, are the most efficient means of discriminating dementing illnesses from normal aging (Welsh et al. 1991). Some of the verbal memory measures used in cognitive assessment are the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT), and the Buschke Selective Reminding Test. Such instruments have the advantage of multiple learning trials (versus one-time exposure to stimuli), adequate length to require secondary (long-term) memory, a delayed recall condition, and a recognition trial. The CVLT, for example, provides a quantitative assessment of multiple qualitative aspects of verbal learning and memory, including learning style, errors during recall, ability to benefit from cueing during recall, and errors made during recall and recognition trials. Figure 8–1 shows typical learning scores by trial in normal elders.
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
297
FIGURE 8–1. California Verbal Learning Test learning scores by trial (T1–T5) in normal subjects ages 50–89 (N=174). This wealth of information has proved useful in the differential diagnosis of various amnestic and dementing disorders (Bondi et al. 1994; Delis et al. 1991), particularly in the early stages (Cullum et al. 1995) (Figure 8–2). In addition to the word list–learning tasks listed above, the Logical Memory subtest of the WMS-III (Wechsler 1997), assesses recall for paragraph-length information (stories). Two paragraph-long stories are read to the patient, one at a time. After each presentation, patients are asked to recall as much information from the story as possible. Delayed recall is also assessed after 30 minutes to examine how much of the initially learned information is retained. The inclusion of delayed-recall trials in such tests is critical, because some amnestic persons are able to perform relatively well on immediate recall trials, showing the severe decay of memory only after a period of time has passed. Visual memory is commonly evaluated by measures such as the Visual Reproduction subtest of the WMS-III and by the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test (Visser 1985). In these tasks, subjects are presented with printed geometric designs of varying complexity. After the figures are removed from view, patients are asked to reproduce the designs from memory. Typically, recall after a 15–30 minute delay is also assessed. Although such tasks are susceptible to the confound of visuoconstructional and motor impairments, comparison of patients’ direct copies of the test stimuli versus recalled reproductions can yield useful information in terms of visual memory ability.
298
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
FIGURE 8–2. California Verbal Learning Test performance scores in subjects with mild Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and advanced normal aging. Note. %Retent = percent retention (delayed free recall/last learning trial ´ 100); Recency% = percentage of words recalled from recency region of the word list; Intrus%= percentage of total words recalled that were intrusions; Discrim%=recognition hits adjusted for false-positive errors. Source. Adapted from Cullum et al. 1995.
Visuospatial Functioning Visuospatial processes are complex and involve a variety of functional systems in the brain, particularly in the right hemisphere. Visuoconstructional functioning is frequently evaluated through use of various standard figure drawings and the Block Design subtest from the WAIS-III and WAIS-R. Some of the more common drawing tasks include clocks (to command and copy, with hands set at a specified time), crosses, cubes, and intersecting lines and pentagons. Various scoring methods have been developed for such tasks, most notably the clock drawing test (see Freedman et al. 1994; Kozora and Cullum 1994). Having patients render drawings of common but visually complex objects such as houses and bicycles can also be useful, even within the context of cognitive screening examinations. Another standard visuospatial drawing test is the copy of the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure, which can also be used later as a visual memory task. Drawings by patients with Alzheimer’s disease often exhibit oversimplification, poor angulation, and impaired perspective (Kirk and Kertesz
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
299
1991). They may also display poor planning in their layout, organization, and presentation. Perseverative features may also be observed, particularly in drawings by patients whose disease includes greater involvement of anterior brain systems. In addition to central brain mechanisms, primary visual impairments and motor deficits may complicate the interpretation of constructional dyspraxia in the elderly. As a result, the use of multiple measures of visuospatial functioning may be in order to preclude the rendering of conclusions on the basis of too little information. Figure 8–3 presents an example of perseveration on the copy of the alternating ramparts from the Dementia Rating Scale.
FIGURE 8–3. Perseveration in frontotemporal dementia as depicted by the copying task from the Dementia Rating Scale.
Language Receptive and expressive language abilities can be disrupted by many forms of dementing illness. Receptive language involves the comprehension of oral and written communication. An individual may lose the ability to attach semantic meaning to words or to understand their syntactical meaning within a sentence, such as “lost woman’s purse” versus “woman’s lost purse.” Visual comprehension of written material is based on auditory mastery and is also often disturbed when an individual has auditory comprehension deficits. In dementing illness, the appreciation of nonverbal components of language, such as prosody or body movements, may also be disrupted. Expressive language disturbances (disorders of production) can take numerous forms. These include disturbances of articulation and word finding and the loss of grammar, syntax, repetition, verbal fluency, and writing. To speak a word, it is necessary to locate it in the extensive repository or
300
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
semantic lexicon of words previously learned. As noted, knowledge of word definitions—and particularly the ability to read single words—tends to remain relatively better preserved in cognitively impaired persons without frank aphasia. Accordingly, performance on the Vocabulary subtest of the WAIS-III can often be used as a gross index of overall intellectual functioning, and in early to middle-stage Alzheimer’s disease, sight-word reading may correlate well with premorbid IQ (Maddrey et al. 1996). A number of standard tests of comprehension exist (e.g., the Token Test; Van Dongen and van Harskamp 1972), although a great deal of information along these lines can be obtained during the clinical interview and by examining patients’ responses to instructions. Cognitively impaired persons can often describe the use of an object but cannot retrieve the name of it (dysnomia). Although bedside testing of confrontation naming (e.g., having the patient name a pen, watch, door, etc.) may elicit the deficit, standard psychometric instruments such as the Boston Naming Test and its derivations (Lansing et al. 1999; Morris et al. 1989), which include line drawings of objects, are more sensitive to dysnomia. Verbal fluency, the ability to produce words and sentences in uninterrupted strings, is affected by numerous factors, including dysnomia associated with frontal and temporal lobe lesions. Common fluency tasks include the rapid generation of words within a minute that begin with a specified letter (letter or phonemic fluency [e.g., “F, A, S” or “C, F, L”]) or that belong to a specific category (category or semantic fluency, e.g., “animals” or “fruits and vegetables”). Whereas some age-related changes on such tests are seen (Abwender et al. 2001; Kozora and Cullum 1995), individuals with Alzheimer’s disease commonly show deficits on the verbal fluency tasks. Some studies have suggested that individuals with dementing illness have greater difficulty with category fluency than with phonemic fluency (Monsch et al. 1994), although this finding is not universally seen in Alzheimer’s disease (Epker et al. 1999; Sherman and Massman 1999). Salmon and Chan (1994) present a detailed discussion of the hypothesized breakdown in semantic knowledge that leads to verbal fluency deficits. Phonemic fluency has been linked with frontal lobe function, whereas category or semantic fluency has been associated more with temporal lobe activation (Moscovitch 1994). Therefore, performance patterns on these tasks in the context of a comprehensive neuropsychological profile may be useful in identifying cerebral regions having greatest dysfunction, which may have implications for diagnosis (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease versus FTD). In addition to the traditional neuropsychological evaluation of basic language abilities, neurolinguistic analysis of discourse can be useful in assessing the communication abilities of cognitively impaired persons
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
301
(Joanette and Brownell 1990). Discourse refers to the linguistic expression of ideas, knowledge, and opinions, typically conveyed as a sequence of sentences that has coherent organization and meaning. The capacity to produce coherent discourse entails a complex interplay of basic linguistic skills, cognitive processes, and information-handling abilities. Discourse abilities are assessed using a variety of procedures, including descriptive, conversational, narrative, procedural, and expository procedures. Early discourse disturbances in Alzheimer’s disease may include reduced elaboration or embedding of ideas (Snowdon et al. 1996), impaired gist-level processing (Chapman et al. 1998), impaired coherence of ideas (Chapman et al. 1995), and interrupted flow of information during verbal expression (Bayles et al. 1989; Ripich and Terrell 1998). A disrupted flow of information often occurs in the Alzheimer’s disease process as the speaker has difficulty fluently translating his or her thoughts into language, resulting in fragmentation of ideas. Discourse gist (e.g., the ability to abstract the overall theme of a story) is also vulnerable in the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease, and deficits are found in persons with mild cognitive impairment (Chapman et al. 1998). In contrast, cognitively healthy adults typically demonstrate a remarkable capacity to derive the gist (central or synthesized meaning) well into the tenth decade of life (Radvansky 1999; Ulatowska et al. 1998). Procedural discourse, a genre that requires an individual to provide the props and sequence of actions to complete an activity, reveals differential breakdowns in component skills involved in generating procedures. Persons with FTD show reduced verbal fluency and also show a noticeable reduction in the specificity of verbs used to describe actions or steps in a task sequence compared with cognitively healthy control subjects and persons with Alzheimer’s disease.
Motor Abilities Motor abilities range from strength and fine motor speed to more complex skilled movements (praxis). Basic motor deficits occur commonly in vascular and subcortical dementias. Simple motor strength is often assessed through use of a hand dynamometer, whereas manual speed may be tested through procedures requiring finger tapping or fine motor dexterity. Decreases in motor strength and/or fine motor speed beyond the effects of normal aging deserve further follow-up (examining for vascular or other focal lesions), particularly when lateralized. If such lateralized findings occur within the context of other lateralized cognitive results (e.g., decreased tapping and dexterity on the right hand in conjunction with impaired verbal versus nonverbal memory and dysnomia), the case for predominant involvement of the left hemisphere can be made.
302
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Disorders of complex skilled movements that do not arise from basic motor difficulties are known as apraxias. Persons may be unable to carry out a motor response to command that is easily performed spontaneously (ideomotor apraxia). Alternatively, they may be unable to sequence known motor acts toward a specific goal (ideational apraxia). These impairments in the production and organization of skilled behaviors are common in the later stages of Alzheimer’s disease. Evaluation of praxis involves instructing the patient to pantomime a series of common activities (e.g., waving goodbye, brushing teeth, preparing a cup of coffee) through both verbal command and imitation.
Sensory-Perceptual Abilities The interaction between performance on cognitive tests and perceptual deficits must be evaluated carefully. A substantial number of cognitively impaired persons complain of visual problems. Although these complaints are often dismissed by clinicians and family members, several studies have reported visual distortions in persons with dementing illnesses in one or more of the following domains: color vision, depth perception, detection of motion, eye movements, low-contrast sensitivity, and higher-order visual perception (Cronin-Colomb et al. 1991; Mendez et al. 1990). Such complications, which can occur at any stage of the disease process, may affect reading and performance on tasks that rely on visual input. It is essential that primary visual dysfunction be ruled out before interpretation of visuospatial processing deficits are posited. Individuals may be asked to read sample text or identify small objects to help ensure adequate basic visual acuity, and this can often be incorporated as a routine examination procedure. Similarly, because most neurocognitive tasks require verbal instruction, decreased hearing thresholds must be considered as a potential confound to the interpretation of cognitive test results. Clinical assessment of hearing level during interview may suffice along these lines, in addition to direct inquiry of the patient and family about hearing problems. Also, olfactory dysfunction may occur early in the course of Alzheimer’s disease (Koss et al. 1988). Formal assessment of sensory-perceptual functions may be undertaken to provide information concerning lateralized somatosensory dysfunction, potentially useful in the identification of focal neuropathological conditions. Several standardized sensorimotor assessment tasks include measures of basic sensory-perceptual skills. The Sensory Perceptual Examination (Reitan and Wolfson 1993) includes measures of simple touch, response to double simultaneous stimulation, finger graphesthesia, gnosis,
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
303
and stereognosis in a standardized and quantifiable fashion. A word of caution is necessary when interpreting sensory-perceptual testing results in cognitively impaired persons, however. Impairments in attention, memory, or confusion about instructions may, for example, predispose cognitively impaired persons to make errors in responding to sensory-perceptual testing. Thus, overinterpretation of sensory-perceptual deficits, particularly on tasks that require more complex responses, may occur in such cases. Consideration of the person’s entire neuropsychological profile is therefore of critical importance.
Personality and Emotional Assessment Clinical interviewing of individuals is sometimes difficult because of their memory impairment, lack of insight into internal mood states, or limited ability to cooperate. Instruments such as the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al. 1961) and the Geriatric Depression Scale (Yesavage et al. 1983) provide brief, reliable indices about depression and mood symptoms. When a more detailed assessment of mood and personality is desirable and feasible, the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory–2 (MMPI-2; Hathaway and McKinley 1989) may be considered. However, the length of this measure and its requirement of a higher level of cognitive integrity (e.g., sustained concentration, reading comprehension at or above the eighth grade level) must be considered. Nevertheless, the findings from such self-report mood and psychological measures can provide important information that may not be immediately available from either the patient or other informants (see Rush et al. 2000 for a listing of many popular psychiatric measures). In any case, some assessment of psychological functioning is an integral part of the neuropsychological evaluation, particularly because psychiatric disorders can influence performance on tests of cognitive ability.
Reporting Cognitive Evaluation Results Results of a comprehensive neurocognitive evaluation are presented in several ways. A verbal report is frequently provided to the referral source on completion of the assessment, followed by a written report containing the patient’s relevant history, clinical interview data, and neuropsychological test results. Conclusions are made concerning the presence, severity, and likely type of dementia, if possible. A description of adaptive functioning in a variety of areas is also commonly made, and prognostic statements are offered concerning relationships with personal, occupational, and psychosocial functioning. Specific recommendations can also be made for further
304
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
neurodiagnostic testing, treatment, disposition, or future reevaluation. Finally, a reporting session may be held with the patient and/or primary caregiver. At this session, findings are presented in everyday language with frequent use of real-life examples to illustrate an individual’s neurobehavioral strengths and deficits. Family members or other caregivers are also given help in devising strategies for patient management in the home, along with support and recommendations for their well-being, in addition to information regarding community resources and referrals as indicated.
Cognitive Patterns of Various Types of Dementia Alzheimer’s Disease Alzheimer’s disease is the most common form of dementing illness. Although the heterogeneity in the behavioral manifestations and the slow progression of Alzheimer’s disease sometimes complicate its early clinical identification, neurocognitive test results and profiles tend to show common characteristics. For example, whereas early or mild Alzheimer’s disease is associated with memory impairment, detailed psychometric assessment often reveals prototypical neuropsychological findings, including limited acquisition and impoverished association abilities, rapid forgetting of newly learned information, and faulty retrieval. Persons with Alzheimer’s disease also tend to make intrusion errors (i.e., injecting incorrect but often related material into information they are trying to remember) during learning and recall attempts on some, but not all, memory tasks. Similarly, when their recognition for previously presented material is assessed, as in a yes-no identification of stimuli, they sometimes make a proportionally large number of false-positive or false alarm errors on tasks such as the CVLT. Such characteristics of episodic memory breakdown often distinguish cortical dementias such as Alzheimer’s disease from subcortical dementias (discussed below). In addition to amnesia (see Bondi et al. 1994), symptoms that characteristically develop in Alzheimer’s disease include degradations in confrontation naming and word-finding, abstract reasoning, linguistic discourse processing (discussed earlier), and visual-constructional skills. Early in the disease course, general intellectual skills may remain relatively intact, and individuals with higher premorbid cognitive abilities may produce normal results on cognitive screening tests, with their impairments only manifesting on detailed psychometric analysis of their memory skills. It is also important to note that although the overall pattern of impairments in Alzheimer’s disease usually suggests greatest involvement of temporal, and to a lesser extent, frontal systems (Chen et al. 2001), heterogeneity of the
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
305
overall neurocognitive profile is common, with some individuals showing greater deficits in certain areas (e.g., primary verbal versus visuospatial). This may relate to long-standing cognitive patterns as well as different neurobehavioral phenotypes of the illness (Johnson et al. 1999; Mayeux et al. 1985; Naugle et al. 1985). Figure 8–4 depicts common patterns as seen in Alzheimer’s disease.
FIGURE 8–4. Typical neuropsychological performance pattern in Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Note. Attn = attention; ExFx = executive function; Verbal = verbal/language skills; Visuo=visuospatial skills; Learn=learning; Mem=memory. Source. Adapted from the meta-analysis by Zakzanis et al. 1999.
Vascular Dementia Vascular dementia accounts for approximately 10% of all cases of dementing illness, although some vascular pathology was present in 30%–40% of autopsied dementia patients in recent population-based studies (Holmes et al. 1999). Vascular dementia often includes both cortical and subcortical manifestations, frequently including the presence of neurological signs (e.g., focal weakness, sensory changes, or gait disturbance). Several series of standard diagnostic criteria for vascular dementia have been developed (see Román et al. 1993), although it remains a challenging clinical diagnosis (see Knopman et al. 2001). Neuropsychologically, there is considerable overlap between the cognitive impairments of Alzheimer’s disease and those of vascular dementia, and the fact that the disorders may coexist further complicates the situation.
306
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Nevertheless, neuropsychological evaluation may assist the diagnosis of vascular dementia through the identification of focal cerebral dysfunction or asymmetries in sensorimotor performance, which are more common in vascular dementia. Some studies have suggested that specific aspects of the neuropsychological profile can be used to distinguish vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. For example, better performance on verbal recognition memory testing, yet worse performance on phonemic fluency was recently reported in patients with vascular dementia compared with patients with Alzheimer’s disease (Tierney et al. 2001). Finally, serial neuropsychological testing can help to characterize the pattern and severity of decline over time, and the extent to which an individual patient varies from the typical progression and/or pattern associated with Alzheimer’s disease can be particularly useful diagnostically.
Dementia With Lewy Bodies Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), including the Lewy body variant of Alzheimer’s disease, is now recognized as one of the more common forms of dementing illness, probably second only to Alzheimer’s disease. It may account for 15%–20% of late-onset dementia cases (Campbell et al. 2001). Whereas early studies of DLB suggested a greater frequency of visual hallucinations, heightened sensitivity to antipsychotic medications, fluctuations in alertness, and gait disturbance (McKeith et al. 1996), other reports have indicated a great deal more overlap between DLB and Alzheimer’s disease (Weiner et al. 1996). Increasingly, evidence is suggesting that it may not be possible to reliably distinguish between DLB and Alzheimer’s disease using existing clinical means (Knopman et al. 2001). There have been reports that, unlike the presentation of Alzheimer’s disease, DLB tends to be associated with more prominent attention deficits, marked fluctuations in attention, impairments in visuoconstructional skills, and less involvement of memory (Heyman et al. 1999). However, other investigations suggest that DLB and Alzheimer’s disease are not reliably distinguishable using neurobehavioral or neuropsychological techniques (Ballard et al. 1999). For reviews of DLB, the reader is referred to Perry et al. (1996) and Cercy and Bylsma (1997).
Frontotemporal Dementias FTD includes a variety of disorders such as Pick’s disease (with or without Pick bodies), corticobasal ganglionic degeneration, nonspecific frontal gliosis, semantic dementia, and various tauopathies. Characteristic symptoms include deficits in executive function and self-awareness and changes
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
307
in personality and behavior. These may include decreased emotion and insight, selfishness, neglect of self-care and grooming, increased preference for sweets, wanting, motor and verbal stereotypes, echolalia, and mutism (reviewed in Hodges 2001). Behavioral disturbance and personality change are often early signs, with disinhibition and inappropriate behaviors being common features. Clinical criteria for the diagnosis of FTD have been put forth, emphasizing these points (McKhann et al. 2001; Miller et al. 1997; Neary et al. 1998). Neuropsychological evaluation of these patients early in the disease course typically reveals prominent deficits in executive function (Filley and Cullum 1993). Lateralized patterns of deficits are also seen with some frequency (Boone et al. 1999; Razani et al. 2001) and have shown relationships to underlying neuropathology (Lipton et al. 2001). Perseverative behaviors are common in frontal lobe dementias and are often documented on a variety of neurocognitive tasks (see Figure 8–3). Impaired performance on executive function tasks is often a prominent feature of the neuropsychological record, outstripping deficits in other functional domains (including memory) in many cases, which can assist in differential diagnosis (Duke and Kaszniak 2000). Striking impairments on measures such as phonemic fluency (Controlled Word Association Test [FAS]; Borkowski et al. 1987), the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Heaton 1981), and the Trail Making Test Part B (relative to Trail Making Part A) are commonplace, in addition to the aforementioned perseverative features, which may be manifested on a variety of tasks (e.g., tests of visuoconstructional skills, memory, and word definitions). A typical cognitive profile seen in cases of FTD is presented in Figure 8–5.
Subcortical Dementias Subcortical dementias are caused by disorders that primarily involve subcortical structures and systems and have associated cognitive impairments (Cummings and Benson 1984). These include Huntington’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, and progressive supranuclear palsy. Other disorders that fall under the heading of subcortical or white matter dementias include progressive multifocal leukoencephalopathy, Binswanger’s disease, multiple sclerosis, acquired immunodeficiency syndrome–related dementia, and normal-pressure hydrocephalus (see Filley 1995). Similar to subcortical disorders such as Parkinson’s disease without dementia, mild cognitive difficulties have also recently been reported in patients with benign essential tremor (Lacritz et al. 2002). Whereas neuropsychological findings in subcortical dementias overlap considerably with those seen in the cortical dementias, several common distinctions are worth noting, with qualitative
308
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
FIGURE 8–5. Neuropsychological performance pattern in frontotemporal dementia (FTD). Note. Attn = attention; ExFx = executive function; Verbal = verbal/language skills; Visuo=visuospatial skills; Learn=learning; Mem=memory. Source. Adapted from the meta-analysis by Zakzanis et al. 1999.
performance features often serving to distinguish these groups (Butters et al. 1987, 1998; Troster et al. 1993). Subcortical dementias frequently involve a pronounced slowing in general information processing in addition to problems with attentional, executive, and visuospatial functions. Marked aphasia and amnesia are characteristically absent in earlier stages. The pattern of deficits can mimic frontal lobe dysfunction in some cases and may be manifested as impairment in the organization of problem-solving behavior (Brandt and Bylsma 1993; Mahurin et al. 1993). Depression is also a common feature of subcortical dementias (Cummings 1990; Rosenstein 1998).
Alcohol-Related Dementia Chronic abuse of alcohol is associated with documented changes in brain morphology, blood flow, electrophysiology, and neuropsychological function (Parsons et al. 1987). Although the amnestic Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome is relatively well understood, there is still debate surrounding the concept of alcohol-related dementia. Some have suggested that many years (typically several decades) of chronic, heavy alcohol abuse can result in a generalized cognitive deterioration characterized as alcoholic dementia (see Salmon et al. 1993; Saxton et al. 2000), whereas others believe that dementia in individuals with long-term alcoholism is attributable to a
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
309
coexisting dementing illness such as Alzheimer’s disease. Nevertheless, the presence of a long, heavy history of alcohol abuse should raise suspicion that this may be a contributing factor, if not the sole factor, in causing cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologically, marked visuospatial deficits (relative to more intact verbal skills) are commonly seen in alcohol-related dementia, in addition to impairments in executive function and memory. In fact, it often presents with cortical as well as subcortical features (Munro et al. 2001). Although cognitive deterioration may slow with a cessation of drinking in some alcoholic persons (D.A. Wilkinson 1987), irreversible brain changes occur in both alcoholic dementia and Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome (Butters 1985; Salmon et al. 1993).
Mild Cognitive Impairment The term mild cognitive impairment (MCI) has been used in recent years to describe patients with impaired memory who are otherwise functioning reasonably well and do not meet criteria for dementia (Petersen et al. 2001). Memory impairment is typically defined as performance on a standard neuropsychological test of memory that falls more than one standard deviation below average for a given age. General clinical criteria for MCI are presented in Table 8–2; see Honig and Mayeux (2001) for a review. MCI is often viewed as a transitional or prodromal stage of Alzheimer’s disease, as a high percentage of individuals who meet criteria for MCI eventually develop dementia, with conversion rates ranging from 6% to 25% per year (Petersen et al. 2001). Furthermore, a great deal of overlap between the qualitative features of memory and cognitive performances in MCI and Alzheimer’s disease has been noted. TABLE 8–2.
Criteria for mild cognitive impairment
1. Presence of memory complaint, preferably corroborated by informant 2. Evidence of objective (i.e., neuropsychological) memory impairment 3. Other cognitive abilities generally within normal limits for age and background 4. Unimpaired activities of daily living 5. Clinically nondemented (i.e., does not meet DSM-IV-TR criteria for dementia) Source.
Adapted from Petersen et al. 2001.
Figure 8–6 presents data from groups of patients with a clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease who are in the early stages of illness (having a score above 115 on the Dementia Rating Scale) and patients with a diagno-
310
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
FIGURE 8–6. Neuropsychological performance in patients with MCI versus mild Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Note. ExFx = executive function; Attn = attention; Lang = language skills; Visuo = visuospatial skills; Vmem=visual memory; NVMem=nonvisual memory.
sis of MCI. As depicted in the figure, both curves show very similar cognitive patterns. In addition to the qualitative commonalities shared between these groups on memory measures, such data suggest that MCI likely represents a very early stage of Alzheimer’s disease in many (if not most) cases, as has been indicated by recent neuropathological findings (Morris et al. 2001).
Depression Depression can result in symptoms and behaviors that mimic dementia (Kaszniak and Christenson 1994). Disengagement from previous activities, decreased motivation, psychomotor slowing, slowed response times, decreased attention, and apparent forgetfulness may all be symptoms of depression. When such clinical features are accompanied by the depressed patient’s cognitive complaints and tendency to overreport symptoms, this can contribute to the appearance of dementing illness. The fact that persons with dementing illness may experience depressive symptoms can complicate attempts to distinguish the cognitive impairment of depression from structural brain diseases (Gilley 1993). Furthermore, because neuropsychological tests depend on adequate effort on the part of the patient, suboptimal or variable effort must be carefully considered lest it be interpreted as neurologically based cognitive impairment. It has been noted that individuals with the cognitive impairment of depression sometimes resemble persons with subcortical dysfunction in their slowed information processing and failure to spontaneously employ active
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
311
learning and memory strategies (King and Caine 1990). When cognitive deficits on formal neuropsychological testing occur in these patients, several characteristic features or trends have been observed (Cummings 1990; Naugle et al. 1998, p. 263). Most commonly, the cognitive difficulties, when present, are very mild (Zakzanis et al. 1999) and often resemble subcortical dysfunction (e.g., see Massman et al. 1992) with evidence of frontal system disturbance (Veiel 1997). Attention and concentration may be reduced or variable across tasks. In terms of memory, depressed persons often show intact storage ability even though learning may be slow and retrieval problems may occur. This may be demonstrated on memory tasks that include cueing or recognition testing procedures, wherein the additional prompts often facilitate considerable improvement in the depressed patient’s performance. Along these lines, comparing recognition to free recall can be particularly illuminating, because more information is encoded than can be easily retrieved by some depressed patients. Greater difficulty with the recall of nonverbal information can also be seen; thus, verbal memory tasks may be more useful in differentiating depression from dementing illness. Whereas the results of cognitive testing in persons with depression are often normal, the performance of these persons may be characterized by variability within and across tests. For example, depressed persons may sporadically miss easier items and get more difficult ones correct. In addition, some tests may show depressed persons to have intact function, whereas other measures that tap similar abilities indicate impairment. Moreover, rapid onset of cognitive symptoms and the presence of patient complaints beyond those expressed by family members are also more common in depressed patients. Test results that do not fit any known neuropsychological profiles or reflect functioning that is widely discrepant from patient complaints should also be suspect. Nevertheless, persistence of deficits after recovery from depression may signal a concurrent dementing illness (Nussbaum et al. 1995). Neuropsychological patterns of performance can often be used to differentiate between Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive difficulties secondary to depression. However, the disorders may coexist, and differential diagnosis may be challenging in a small proportion of cases. Accordingly, serial neuropsychological evaluations may be very useful in monitoring patients over time, because the cognitive problems in persons with uncomplicated depression would not be expected to progress (although as noted, variability in performance can be seen). In contrast, persons with Alzheimer’s disease typically produce rather characteristic patterns of learning, forgetting, and making errors during recall and recognition that help distinguish them from persons with depression alone.
312
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Summary The value of the neuropsychological examination is several-fold: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6.
To detect subtle cognitive impairment To quantitatively document level of impairment To outline cognitive strengths as well as weaknesses To aid in differential diagnosis To monitor changes over time (including responses to treatment) To assist in designing intervention strategies to help optimize patient functioning
Neuropsychological evaluation can contribute important diagnostic information in the following conditions: 1) when there is a discrepancy between the patient’s self-report of difficulties and the family’s assessment; 2) when the physician desires additional confirmation of a tentative diagnosis before recommending other neurodiagnostic procedures; 3) when emotional factors such as depression or anxiety may contribute to the symptoms; and 4) when a more thorough evaluation of cognitive functions is necessary. Several examples illustrate these points. Family members sometimes become concerned about an elderly parent’s behavior. However, in a short office visit, elderly persons may deny or minimize difficulties; if they possess intact vocabulary, conversational skills, and social comportment, cognitive deficits may be underappreciated. A neuropsychological evaluation provides valuable diagnostic information as well as useful practical recommendations to the physician and family regarding specific cognitive limitations and degree of independence. Initial neuropsychological presentation may also have implications for predicting future course of illness (Becker et al. 1988; Rasmusson et al. 1996). Or the physician may suspect that a patient is declining cognitively but also appears to be withdrawn and slightly depressed. The neuropsychological evaluation can often help to identify the various factors contributing to the apparent decline and quantify them for future reference. For example, distinguishing depression from dementing illness can often be accomplished using neuropsychological techniques and profile analysis. A brief neuropsychological screening examination can also be useful in the office or inpatient setting to help identify persons who may warrant further evaluation for dementing illness or other emotional disorders in a quick and cost-effective fashion. The neuropsychological evaluation can also help to assess the cognitive effects of various medical treatments. These evaluations can be employed in a pre-post design to assess the effects of treatments such as cognition-
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
313
enhancing medications, electroconvulsive therapy, shunts for normalpressure hydrocephalus, antidepressant or antianxiety medications, and endarterectomy. Results may yield valuable objective data concerning patients’ cognitive response, allowing the physician to make decisions about whether to continue, change, or discontinue a particular form of therapy. Finally, the detailed analysis of a patient’s cognitive abilities may be used to plan interventions that focus on patients’ strengths and weaknesses. This can be done in conjunction with or following termination of medical therapy and includes attempts to minimize the effects of cognitive deficits on everyday functional abilities and overall quality of life. This often involves continuing education and support of the family to maximize the use of the available support network to help maintain safe, independent functioning for as long as possible. Such interventions can be particularly useful in progressive illnesses, in which planning and structuring an individual’s environment and working with the family to enhance the quality of life of all parties involved is the most useful intervention.
References Abwender DA, Swan JG, Bowerman JT, et al: Qualitative analysis of verbal fluency output: review and comparison of scoring methods. Assessment 1:8:323–336, 2001 Albert MS: Cognitive function, in Geriatric Neuropsychology. Edited by Albert MS, Moss MB. New York, Guilford, 1988, pp 33–53 American Academy of Neurology Task Force: Assessment: neuropsychological testing of adults. Neurology 47:592–599, 1996 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 2000 Ballard CG, Ayre G, O’Brien J, et al: Simple standardised neuropsychological assessments aid in the differential diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies from Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 10: 104–108, 1999 Barona A, Reynolds CR, Chastain R: A demographically based index of premorbid intelligence for the WAIS-R. J Consult Clin Psychol 52:885–887, 1984 Bayles K, Boone D, Tomoeda C, et al: Differentiating Alzheimer’s patients from the normal elderly and stroke patients with aphasia. J Speech Hear Disord 54:74– 87, 1989 Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, et al: An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 4:561–571, 1961 Becker JT, Huff FJ, Nebes RD, et al: Neuropsychological function in Alzheimer’s disease: pattern of impairment and rates of progression. Arch Neurol 45:263– 268, 1988
314
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Becker JT, Boller F, Lopez OL, et al: The natural history of Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 51:585–594, 1994 Blair JR, Spreen O: Predicting premorbid IQ: a revision of the National Adult Reading Test. Clin Neuropsychol 3:129–136, 1989 Bondi MW, Salmon DP, Butters N: Neuropsychological features of memory disorders in Alzheimer disease, in Alzheimer Disease. Edited by Terry RD, Katzman R. New York, Raven, 1994, pp 41–63 Boone KB, Miller BL, Lee A, et al: Neuropsychological patterns in right versus left frontotemporal dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 5:616–622, 1999 Brandt J: The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test: development of a new memory test with six equivalent forms. Clinical Neuropsychologist 5:125–142, 1991 Brandt JA, Bylsma FW: The dementia of Huntington’s disease, in Neuropsychology of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias. Edited by Parks RW, Zec RF, Wilson RS. New York, Oxford University Press, 1993, pp 265–282 Borkowski JG, Benton AL, Spreen O: Word fluency and brain damage. Neuropsychologia 5:135–140, 1967 Buschke H, Fuld PA: Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology 24:1019–1025, 1974 Butters N: Alcoholic Korsakoff’s syndrome: some unresolved issues concerning etiology, neuropathology, and cognitive deficits. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 7:181– 210, 1985 Butters N, Granholm E, Salmon D, et al: Episodic and semantic memory: a comparison of amnestic and dementia patients. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 9:479– 497, 1987 Butters M, Goldstein G, Allen DN, et al: Neuropsychological similarities and differences among Huntington’s disease, multiple sclerosis, and cortical dementia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 13:721–735, 1998 Cahn DA, Salmon DP, Bondi MW, et al: A population-based analysis of qualitative features of the neuropsychological test performance of individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer type: implications for individuals with questionable dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 3:387–393, 1997 Campbell S, Stephens S, Ballard C: Dementia with Lewy bodies: clinical features and treatment. Drugs Aging 18:397–407, 2001 Cercy SP, Bylsma FW: Lewy bodies and progressive dementia: a critical review and meta-analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 3:179–194, 1997 Chapman SB, Ulatowska HK, King L, et al: Discourse in early Alzheimer’s disease versus normal advanced aging. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 4:122–127, 1995 Chapman SB, Highley AP, Thompson JL: Discourse in fluent aphasia and Alzheimer’s disease: linguistic and pragmatic considerations. J Neurol 11:55–78, 1998 Chen P, Tarcliff G, Belle SH, et al: Patterns of cognitive decline in presymptomatic Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58:853–858, 2001 Cronin-Colomb A, Corkin S, Rizzo JF, et al: Visual dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: relation to normal aging. Ann Neurol 29:41–52, 1991
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
315
Crook T, Bartus RT, Ferris SH, et al: Age-associated memory impairment. Proposed diagnostic criteria and measures of clinical change: report of a National Institute of Mental Health Work Group. Dev Neuropsychol 2:261–276, 1986 Cullum CM, Rosenberg RN: Memory loss in aging: when is it Alzheimer’s disease? JAMA 279:1–3, 1998 Cullum CM, Butters N, Troster AI, et al: Normal aging and forgetting rates on the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 5:23–30, 1990 Cullum CM, Thompson LL, Smernoff EN: Three word recall as a measure of memory. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 15:321–329, 1993 Cullum CM, Filley CM, Kozora E: Episodic memory function in advanced aging and early Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1:100–103, 1995 Cullum CM, Saine K, Chan LD, et al: A performance-based instrument to assess functional capacity in dementia: the Texas Functional Living Scale. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 14:103–108, 2001 Cummings JL: Subcortical Dementia. New York, Oxford University Press, 1990 Cummings JL, Benson F: Subcortical dementia: review of an emerging concept. Arch Neurol 41:874–879, 1984 Delis DC, Massman PJ, Butters N, et al: Profiles of demented and amnesic patients on the California Verbal Learning Test: implications for the assessment of memory disorders. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 3:19–26, 1991 Delis-Kaplan Executive Function Test, San Antonio, TX, Psychological Corporation, 2001 Duke LM, Kaszniak AW: Executive control functions in degenerative dementias: a comparative review. Neuropsychol Rev 10:75–99, 2000 Epker MO, Lacritz LH, Cullum CM: Comparative analysis of qualitative verbal fluency in normal elderly and demented populations. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 21:425–434, 1999 Filley CM: Neurobehavioral anatomy. Boulder, CO, University of Colorado Press, 1995 Filley CM, Cullum CM: Early detection of frontotemporal degeneration by clinical evaluation. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 8:359–367, 1993 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189– 198, 1975 Freedman M, Leach L, Kaplan E, et al: Clock drawing: a neuropsychological analysis. New York, Oxford University Press, 1994 Gilley DW: Behavioral and affective disturbances in Alzheimer’s disease, in Neuropsychology of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias. Edited by Parks RW, Zec RF, Wilson RS. New York, Oxford University Press, 1993, pp 112–137 Hathaway SR, McKinley JC: Manual for Administration and Scoring the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory—2. Minneapolis, MN, The University of Minnesota Press, 1989 Heaton RK: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual. Odessa, FL, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1981
316
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Heyman A, Fillenbaum GG, Gearing M, et al: Comparison of Lewy body variant of Alzheimer’s disease with pure Alzheimer’s disease: Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease, Part XIX. Neurology 52:1839–1844, 1999 Hodges JR: Frontotemporal dementia (Pick’s disease): clinical features and assessment. Neurology 56 (11 suppl 4):S6–S10, 2001 Holmes C, Cairns N, Lantos P, et al: Validity of current clinical criteria for Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Br J Psychiatry 174:45–50, 1999 Honig L, Mayeux R: Natural history of Alzheimer’s disease. Aging 13:171–182, 2001 Joanette Y, Brownell HH: Discourse Ability and Brain Damage: Theoretical and Empirical Perspectives. New York, Springer-Verlag, 1990 Johnson JK, Head E, Kim R, et al: Clinical and pathological evidence for a frontal variant of Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 56:1233–1239, 1999 Kaltreider LB, Cicerello AR, Lacritz LH, et al: A comparison of the CERAD and CVLT list-learning tasks in Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Neuropsychol 14:269– 274, 2000 Kareken DA: Judgement pitfalls in estimating premorbid intellectual function. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2:701–709, 1997 Kaszniak AW, Christenson GD: Differential diagnosis of dementia and depression, in Neuropsychological Assessment of Dementia and Depression in Older Adults: A Clinician’s Guide. Edited by Storandt M, VandenBos GR, Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 1994, pp 81–117 King DA, Caine DA: Depression, in Subcortical Dementia. Edited by Cummings JL. New York, Oxford University Press, 1990, pp 218–230 Kirk A, Kertesz A: On drawing impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 48:73–77. 1991 Knopman DS, DeKosky ST, Cummings JL, et al: Practice parameter: diagnosis of dementia (an evidence-based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 56:1143–1153, 2001 Koss E, Weiffenbach JM, Haxby JV, et al: Olfactory detection and identification performance are dissociated in early Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 38:1228– 1232, 1988 Koss E, Haxby JV, DeCarli C, et al: Patterns of performance preservation and loss in healthy aging. Dev Neuropsychol 7:99–113, 1991 Koss E, Patterson MB, Ownby R, et al: Memory evaluation in Alzheimer’s disease: caregivers’ appraisals and objective testing. Arch Neurol 50:92–97, 1993 Kozora E, Cullum CM: Qualitative features of clock drawings in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Assessment 1:179–187, 1994 Kozora E, Cullum CM: Generative naming in normal aging: total output and qualitative changes using phonemic and semantic constraints. Clin Neuropsychol 9:313–320, 1995 Kral VA: Benign senile forgetfulness in Alzheimer’s disease, in Senile Dementia and Related Disorders. Edited by Katzman R, Terry RD, Bick KL. New York, Raven, 1978, pp 47–51
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
317
Lacritz LH, Dewey R, Giller C, et al: Cognitive functioning in individuals with “benign” essential tremor. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 8:125–129, 2002 Lansing AE, Ivnik RJ, Cullum CM, et al: An empirically derived short form of the Boston Naming Test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 14:481–487, 1999 Larrabee GJ, McEntee WJ: Age-associated memory impairment: sorting out the controversies. Neurology 45:611–614, 1995 Lewis RF, Rennick PM: Manual for the Repeatable Cognitive-Perceptual-Motor Battery. Clinton Township, MI, Ronald F. lewis, 1979 Lezak MD: Neuropsychological assessment, 3rd edition. New York, Oxford University Press, 1995 Lines CR, Dawson C, Preston GC, et al: Memory and attention in patients with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type and in normal elderly subjects. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 13:691–702, 1991 Lipton A, Cullum CM, Satumtira S, et al: Synapse loss is asymmetric and contributes to lateralizing clinical deficits in frontotemporal dementias. Arch Neurol 58:1233–1239, 2001 Maddrey AM, Cullum CM, Weiner MF, et al: Premorbid intelligence estimation and level of dementia in Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2:551–555, 1996 Mahurin RK, Feher EP, Nance ML, et al: Cognition in Parkinson’s disease and related disorders, in Neuropsychology of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias. Edited by Parks RW, Zec RF, Wilson RS. New York, Oxford University Press, 1993, pp 308–349 Mahurin RK, Cooke N: The Verbal Series Attention Test: normal and demented older adults. Clinical Neuropsychologist 10:43–53, 1996 Massman PJ, Delis DC, Butters N, et al: The subcortical dysfunction hypothesis of memory deficits in depression: neuropsychological validation in a subgroup of patients. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 14:687–706, 1992 Mattis S: Dementia Rating Scale. Odessa, FL, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1988 Mayeux R, Stern Y, Spanton S: Heterogeneity in dementia of the Alzheimer type: evidence of subgroups. Neurology 35:453–461, 1985 McKeith IG, Galasko D, Kosaka K, et al: Clinical and pathological diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): report of the Consortium on Dementia With Lewy Bodies International Workshop. Neurology 47:1113–1124, 1996 McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al: Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of the Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 34:939–944, 1984 McKhann GM, Albert MS, Grossman M, et al: Clinical and pathological diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia. Arch Neurol 58:1803–1809, 2001 Mendez MF, Mendez MA, Martin R, et al: Complex visual disturbances in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 40:439–443, 1990 Miller BL, Ikonte C, Ponton M, et al: A study of the Lund-Manchester research criteria for frontotemporal dementia: clinical and single-photon emission CT correlations. Neurology 48:937–942, 1997
318
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Monsch AU, Bondi MW, Butters N, et al: A comparison of category and letter fluency in Alzheimer’s disease and Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychologia 8:25– 30, 1994 Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, et al: The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD), part I: clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 39:1159–1165, 1989 Morris JC, Storandt M, Miller JP, et al: Mild cognitive impairment represents early stage Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 58:397–405, 2001 Moscovitch M: Cognitive resources and dual-task interference effects at retrieval in normal people: the role of the frontal lobes and medial temporal cortex. Neuropsychology 8:524–534, 1994 Munro CA, Saxton J, Butters MA: Alcohol dementia: “cortical” or “subcortical” dementia? Arch Clin Neuropsychol 16:523–533, 2001 Naugle RI, Cullum CM, Bigler ED, et al: Neuropsychological and computerized axial tomography volume characteristics of empirically derived dementia subgroups. J Nerv Ment Dis 173:596–604, 1985 Naugle RI, Cullum CM, Bigler ED: Introduction to Clinical Neuropsychology. Austin, TX, Pro-Ed, 1998 Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, et al: Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 51:1546–1554, 1998 Nussbaum PD, Kazniak AW, Allender J, et al: Depression and cognitive decline in the elderly: a follow-up study. Clin Neuropsychol 9:101–111, 1995 Paolo AM, Troster AI, Ryan JJ, et al: Comparison of NART and Barona demographic equation premorbid IQ estimates in Alzheimer’s disease. J Clin Psychol 53:713–722, 1997 Parsons OA, Butters N, Nathan PE (eds): Neuropsychology of Alcoholism: Implications for Diagnosis and Treatment. New York, Guilford, 1987 Paulman RG, Kennelly KJ: Test anxiety and ineffective test taking: different names, same construct? J Educ Psychol 76:279–288, 1984 Perry R, McKeith I, Perry E: Dementia With Lewy Bodies: Clinical, Pathological and Treatment Issues. Cambridge, England, Cambridge University Press, 1996 Petersen RC, Stevens JC, Ganguli M, et al: Practice parameter: early detection of dementia: mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 56:1133–1142, 2001 Radvansky GA: Aging, memory, and comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science 8:49–53, 1999 Rasmusson DX, Carson KA, Brookmeyer R, et al: Predicting rate of cognitive decline in probable Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Cogn 31:133–147, 1996 Razani J, Boone KB, Miller BL, et al: Neuropsychological performance of rightand left-frontotemporal dementia compared to Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 7:468–480, 2001 Reitan RM, Wolfson D: The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Tuscon, AZ, 1985
Evaluation of Cognitive Functions
319
Reitan RM, Wolfson D: The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: theory and clinical interpretation, 2nd Edition. Tuscon, AZ, Neuropsychology Press, 1993 Ripich D, Terrell B: Patterns of discourse cohesion and coherence in Alzheimer’s disease. J Speech Hear Disord 53:8–15, 1998 Román GC, Tetemichi TK, Erkinjuntti T, et al: Vascular dementia: diagnostic criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology 43:250–260, 1993 Rosenstein LD: Differential diagnosis of the major progressive dementias and depression in middle and late adulthood: a summary of the literature of the early 1990s. Neuropsychol Rev 8:109–167, 1998 Rush AJ, Pincus HA, First MB, et al: Handbook of Psychiatric Measures. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 2000 Salmon D, Chan A: Semantic memory deficits associated with Alzheimer’s disease, in Neuropsychological Explorations of Memory and Cognition. Edited by Cermak LS. New York, Plenum, 1994, pp 61–76 Salmon DP, Butters N, Heindel WC: Alcoholic dementia and related disorders, in Neuropsychology of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias. Edited by Parks RW, Zec RF, Wilson RS. New York, Oxford University Press, 1993, pp 186–209 Saxton J, McGonigle-Gibsons KL, Swihart AA: Description and validation of a new neuropsychological test battery; psychological assessment. J Consult Clin Psychol 2:298–303, 1990 Saxton J, Munro CA, Butters MA, et al: Alcohol, dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease: comparison of neuropsychological profiles. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 13:141–149, 2000 Schacter DL, Tulving E (eds): Memory Systems. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, 1994 Sherman AM, Massman PJ: Prevalence and correlates of category versus letter fluency discrepancies in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 14:411– 418, 1999 Snowdon DA, Kemper SJ, Mortimer JA, et al: Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer’s disease in late life. JAMA 275:528–532, 1996 Snyder PJ, Nussbaum PD: Clinical Neuropsychology: A Pocket Handbook for Assessment. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 1998 Spreen O, Strauss E: A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests, 2nd Edition. New York, Oxford University Press, 1998 Squire LR: Mechanisms of memory. Science 232:1612–1619, 1986 Tierney MC, Black SE, Szalai JP, et al: Recognition memory and verbal fluency differentiate probable Alzheimer disease from subcortical ischemic vascular dementia. Arch Neurol 58:1654–1659, 2001 Trenerry MR, Crosson B, DeBoe J, et al: The Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test. Odessa, FL, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1989 Troster AI, Butters N, Salmon DP, et al: The diagnostic utility of savings scores: differentiating Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s diseases with the logical memory and visual reproduction tests. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 15:773–788, 1993
320
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Troyer AK, Graves R, Cullum CM: Executive functioning as a mediator between episodic memory and healthy aging. Aging Cognition 1:45–53, 1994 Ulatowska HK, Chapman SB, Highley AP, et al: Discourse in healthy old-elderly adults: a longitudinal study. Aphasiology 12:619–633, 1998 Van Dongen HR, van Harskamp F: The token test: a preliminary evaluation of a method to detect aphasia. Psychiatr Neurol Neurochir 75:129–134, 1972 Van Gorp WG, Marcotte TD, Sultzer D, et al: Screening for dementia: comparison of three commonly used instruments. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 21:29–38, 1999 Veiel HOF: A preliminary profile of neuropsychological deficits associated with major depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 4:587–603, 1997 Verhaeghen P, Marcoen A, Goossens L: Facts and fiction about memory aging: a quantitative integration of research findings. J Gerontol 48:P157–P171, 1993 Visser RSH: Manual of the Complex Figure Test. Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Swets and Zeitlinger, 1985 Wecshler D: Wecshler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. New York, Psychological Corporation, 1981 Wecshler D: Wecshler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd Edition. San Antonio, TX, Psychological Corporation, 1997 Weiner MF, Davis KL: Anticholinergic drugs, in Drugs in Psychiatry, Vol 4. Edited by Burrows GD, Norman TR, Davies B. Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Elsevier, 1986, pp 191–205 Weiner MF, Risser RC, Cullum CM, et al: Alzheimer’s disease and its Lewy body variant: a clinical analysis of post-mortem verified cases. Am J Psychiatry 153:1269–1273, 1996 Welsh K, Butters N, Hughes J, et al: Detection of abnormal memory decline in mild cases of Alzheimer’s disease using CERAD neuropsychological measures. Arch Neurol 48:278–281, 1991 Welsh K, Butters N, Mohs RC, et al: The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD), part V: normative study of the neuropsychological battery. Neurology 44:609–614, 1994a Welsh K, Hoffman JM, Earl NL, et al: Neural correlates of dementia: regional brain metabolism (FDG-PET) and the CERAD neuropsychological battery. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 9:395–409, 1994b Wilkinson DA: CT scan and neuropsychological assessments of alcoholism, in Neuropsychology of Alcoholism: Implications for Diagnosis and Treatment. Edited by Parsons OA, Butters N, Nathan PA. New York, Guilford, 1987, pp 76–98 Yesavage J, Brink T, Rose T: Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychaitr Res 17:37–49, 1983 Zakzanis KK, Leach L, Kaplan E: Neuropsychological Differential Diagnosis. Lisse, Netherlands, Swets and Zeitlinger, 1999
CHAPTER
9
Supporting Family Caregivers Kristin Martin-Cook, M.S. Doris Svetlik, B.S.N., R.N., M.S. Myron F. Weiner, M.D.
In the long-term management of dementing illness, especially in progressive illnesses such as Alzheimer’s disease, the clinician’s primary relationship is with the patient’s family. Families bear the burden of understanding and managing the patient’s person and property. Therefore, after making a diagnosis and instituting appropriate medical treatment, the most important issues to address are the education and physical and emotional support of family caregivers. Education About the Illness Caregivers often need (and want) to learn about the cognitive and behavioral effects of dementing illness, its anticipated course, and the various social and financial consequences of the illness. However, more is not always better. Different caregivers require different information. Some caregivers will cope best by seeking as much information as they can obtain; other caregivers become overwhelmed by thinking about the future. It is not a universal recommendation in our clinic to read caregiver-oriented or dementia-oriented books. Rather, we offer handouts or other literature providing an overview of issues and then explain that more information can be obtained as issues arise or whenever the caregiver feels it is needed. For those who are information seekers, many lay-directed guides are available for families of dementia patients, especially those with Alzheimer’s disease. The best resource is the third edition of The 36-Hour Day (Mace et al. 1999). Other recent guides are Alzheimer’s Disease: A Handbook for Caregivers (Hamdy et al. 1998) and Alzheimer’s Early Stages: First Steps in Caring and Treatment (Kuhn and Bennett 1999). See also Appendix K for additional sources of information. 321
322
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Family members need to understand that their loved ones have a reduced capacity to encode, process, integrate, communicate, and act on information that in turn impairs the ability to act appropriately in response to certain situations or information. The use of medical jargon should be avoided, and when it is used, it needs to be explained. For example, the term dementia requires explanation. Dementing illnesses cause invisible disability, which often leaves family members puzzled about patients’ abilities and inabilities and, very often, about the diagnosis. It is sometimes useful to show families brain images (magnetic resonance imaging, single photon emission computed tomography, or even textbook pictures), because this helps caregivers to view troubling behaviors in the context of a brain disease. Also, concrete examples of how cognitive symptoms may affect daily functioning are important, such as the following examples: Your husband becomes lost when trying to find his way to the bathroom at night. This is a frequent symptom of dementing illness. Because his memory is impaired, he has to rely on his eyesight to help him make up for that loss. When he can’t see well, his memory is no longer good enough to guide him. Your mother probably burns food on the stove because she is having difficulty with her concentration and her memory. Her attention wanders easily, and she forgets what she is doing. She gives the appearance of becoming careless when she is actually performing as well as she can at the moment.
Sometimes the cognitive impairment is best conveyed by analogizing the patient’s inability to reason and understand to that of a small child—including a child’s need for simple directions and for direction by example. You told me how you dealt with your own children when they were young—showing them instead of telling them. Instead of telling your mother to stir the soup for you so that it won’t burn, show her how you want the soup stirred, help her to do it for a few moments, and then let her do it on her own. When she gets confused on the way to the bathroom, guide her to it. If you tell her where the bathroom is, she won’t remember. She’ll be less irritable if she’s helped to do what you want her to do or what she wants to do instead of being told what not to do or that what she’s doing is wrong.
Because disturbing behaviors, even more than cognitive dysfunction, lead families to seek diagnosis and treatment, it is crucial to address these behavioral symptoms and to explain the link between these symptoms and the dementing illness. Irritability, for example, requires an explanation just like disorientation. Your observation that your father is more irritable goes along with his illness. His irritability is due in part to the fact that he is no longer able to sup-
Supporting Family Caregivers
323
press minor irritation as you and I do when we get upset over something that we know is trivial. It is also partly due to his irritation with himself because he is unable to concentrate and remember.
The phenomenon of disinhibition also needs to be explained, for example, so that a cognitively impaired person who accidentally exposes his genitals is not believed to be manifesting potentially dangerous hypersexuality. Depression, too, is often a concern of families. The apathy inherent in most dementing illnesses is frequently misinterpreted as depression. In fact, many families and primary care physicians postpone diagnosis and treatment for dementing illness, believing that the patient is “just depressed.” It is important to help families understand that what is seen as withdrawal often represents positive disengagement from activities that are too challenging for the patient’s current cognitive functioning. The specific cognitive and behavioral problems in each case can be brought to light by including caregivers in the mental status examination of the patient and by reviewing that examination with them afterward. Performing the digit span test shows that defects in concentration make it difficult to remember and that information must be presented in small increments. Testing remote memory and immediate recall can demonstrate that recall is the type of memory that is most seriously affected—that what was encoded long ago may be partially intact. Understanding that there are different types of memory deficit helps caregivers deal with the logical inconsistency that a person can easily remember something from long ago but not something that happened only a few minutes ago. Failures in simple mathematical operations also point to difficulty in concentration. Impairment of abstract reasoning can be demonstrated through the use of similarities and proverbs, and impaired judgment can be demonstrated by asking simple questions involving social judgment. Observing the clinician’s management of the patient’s irritability or inappropriate social behavior can be translated by caregivers into useful management strategies. Caregivers learn how to avoid increasing patients’ irritability by observing the examiner. Families often want to know what element of cognitive functioning or behavior will be affected next and whether there is a predictable pattern of loss. In the case of progressive dementing illnesses, families can be told that there will be progressive impairment of the ability to learn new skills and ideas, so that patients will have to rely increasingly on what they already know how to do. They can also be told to expect impairment of abstract thinking and reasoning ability, as indicated in the following example: If you were to tell your wife that it is raining cats and dogs, she might be puzzled and say that she doesn’t see any cats or dogs. She also wouldn’t understand what you meant if you said that a place was only a hop, skip, and
324
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
a jump away. She’d think you were talking about hopping, skipping, and jumping. Her impaired judgment is likely to show up in driving. She will probably stop at a red traffic light and go when the light turns green but may not take into account that somebody may still be crossing the street.
Families of Alzheimer’s disease patients in particular are becoming more sophisticated and educated. Many want to discuss the pathophysiology of the illness to find out what areas of the brain and what neurotransmitters are involved. They become able to understand why anticholinergic drugs are prone to cause delirium in Alzheimer’s disease and to guard their patients from the antihistamines and other medications with significant anticholinergic properties. Educating families about the potentially toxic side effects of medications is also helpful. They need not be given a list of delirium-producing medications. Instead, they can be encouraged to always consider the possibility that a new medication or an increase in dosage of a long-standing medication may be responsible for a change in mental status. Families also need to be cautioned about the potential paradoxical excitatory effects of minor tranquilizers in all forms of dementing illness. Families can be taught that difficulty with new learning makes it unwise to introduce their loved one into new situations and that it is wiser to involve that person in familiar activities in familiar places. They can be taught not to remind patients of their deficits and to serve as memory and a social buffer for their patients.
Prognosis Prognosis is a very important issue for families, from both practical and emotional points of view. Families want and need to know the anticipated course of the illness. Most families can tolerate being told that a disease is expected to progress but that the exact course cannot be predicted. They can be told that there may be long periods of no apparent decline. They generally appreciate being told the worst as well as the best possible outcome. Generally speaking, it seems unwise to suggest that the course of the illness may at any time involve agitation or violence. Such a suggestion makes families wary of their loved ones and may cause them to overreact to trivial events. Families of patients with acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS) can be told that there may be significant remission of cognitive symptoms with treatment of the underlying disease. In cases of patients with vascular dementia, the family may be offered the hope that progression may be slowed by the use of aspirin or antiplatelet drugs, but they should also be told that there still remains a good likelihood that the dementia will progress and that plans should be made accordingly. In cases
Supporting Family Caregivers
325
of Alzheimer’s disease, families can be told that the disease will progress, that its rate of progression is uncertain, but that medications are available to slow progression. When families ask about the ultimate outcome of Alzheimer’s disease, they can be told that if the patient lives long enough, this disease may result in complete inability to communicate or to care for himself or herself. But it is also useful to state that other illnesses usually supervene long before the dementing illness has run its course. Our general policy is to suggest to families that they live one day at a time rather than live in anticipation of symptoms or behaviors that may never arise. When we are asked when nursing home care will be needed, we indicate that need for such care depends on several family factors, but that the ability of families to deal with and to tolerate behaviors such as wandering and incontinence is often the impetus, and that families generally know when their tolerance has been exceeded.
Family Risk Some dementing illnesses involve risk to family members. In the case of Huntington’s disease and Alzheimer’s disease, the risks are genetic. Because the former expresses in midlife, the pattern of dominant inheritance poses a serious threat to siblings and offspring; great sensitivity is needed in dealing with the issue of heritability. The situation in Alzheimer’s disease is similar for dominantly inherited early-onset Alzheimer’s disease, but not for late-onset disease. We respond to concern about the heritability of Alzheimer’s disease by stating that not enough is currently known to enable sound counseling but that the risk increases for siblings and children of affected persons, although we are unable to predict the exact risk for individuals. In the instances of familial Alzheimer’s disease with onset as early as the fifth decade, families can be reasonably counseled that the risk approaches 50% for offspring of an afflicted person should they live to the age of usual disease onset in the family. When presenting data concerning the heritability of illness, clinicians need to be alert to the impact of this information on family members. An open-ended discussion following the communication of this information usually brings to light fears, misgivings, and potential negative effects. When counseling about infectious disorders such as Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease or AIDS, the clinician indicates that these diseases can be communicated only from body fluid to body fluid, suggesting that appropriate precautions be taken. In the case of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, blood-to-blood transmission would only be likely in the event that a caregiver punctured himself or herself with a needle contaminated with the patient’s blood. In the latter case, contact with blood and seminal fluid is to be avoided.
326
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Other Major Issues It is important to deal with certain concrete issues. One issue is who will be the primary caregiver and conduit of information to and from the patient and family. The designated primary caregiver, in consultation with the clinician, the patient, and the rest of the family, needs to consider what supervision is necessary and how supervision is to be arranged. If the person is living independently, is independent living feasible or desirable? If so, how independent? Can the patient be allowed to continue driving? Buy groceries? Cook? Must eating and personal hygiene be supervised? In many instances, cognitively impaired persons can continue to live in their own accustomed residence, with once- or twice-a-day supervision by friends, relatives, and neighbors. If independent living is not feasible from the standpoint of safety or convenience, where can the person be served and accommodated best? Could the person live with a friend or relative? Would a day-care program ease the burden? Would assisted living be appropriate? Is a special care facility needed? Chapter 13 includes a full discussion of assisted living and special care units. Families attempt to balance these quality-of-life and safety issues. Because the awareness and insight of many patients is impaired, their families are often left to make decisions regarding the level of independence the patient will maintain—a situation that many patients resent and resist. Mandates to the family by the physician to stop patients from driving or to move patients into supervised living are rarely productive. It is usually more useful to discuss the options, barriers, and resources and a plan of action for dealing with the problem and minimizing risks in very practical and concrete terms. For patients with more advanced disease, the issue of nursing home care arises. With near-terminal patients in nursing homes, the issue is whether to institute tube feeding. Financial and legal issues must be raised (see Chapter 10). Has a will been made? If not, is the patient able to indicate his or her true wishes? Is the patient able to transact business or enter contracts, such as for the sale of property? Does another family member have the type of power of attorney that can be used when the patient is no longer able to grant power of attorney? Is guardianship necessary to ensure impartial supervision of the patient’s assets? If guardianship is needed, should it be full or partial? In anticipation of the need for Medicaid coverage of possible prolonged nursing home placement, is it best that the patient’s property be transferred to other family members? Although we typically refer to financial consultants specializing in the elderly for specific issues, it is important to provide some general information regarding financial issues. Many people mistakenly believe that Medicare will provide for the health
Supporting Family Caregivers
327
care needs of patients. It is important to explain that custodial care of persons with dementing illnesses is not covered by Medicare, so that families can make plans accordingly.
Imparting a Psychological Point of View It is also useful to impart a psychological point of view to caregivers. Family members are often confused by daily fluctuations in cognitive ability and behavior and tend to see cognitively impaired persons as arbitrarily deciding to forget and consciously manipulating others. Both may be true to a certain extent. However, it is more often true that what appear willful and arbitrary behaviors are attempts to avoid becoming overwhelmed, based on each person’s own style of coping with or adapting to stress. Caregivers often see cognitively impaired persons as unreasonably irritable, failing to recognize how frustrating it is to be unable to reason, to remember, and to perform simple activities of daily living. When caregivers’ attempts at orienting cognitively impaired persons are met with apparent indifference, the caregivers must be reminded that this appearance of indifference is both a product of the disease and a psychological protection against overwhelming anxiety, not indifference to the caring of others. Family members, out of their own needs and inability to conceive of the impact of the brain changes that are taking place, often prefer to see cognitively impaired loved ones as intact persons who are concealed behind a confused exterior. They can be told that although fragments of their loved one may exist intact, changes in cognition necessitate adjusting and readjusting to a new person with different needs, interests, perceptions, and abilities throughout the course of the dementing illness. Nevertheless, cognitively impaired persons should be shown the same respect as human beings as cognitively intact persons. Attention must also be given to certain prominent mechanisms of defense in dementia.
Dealing With Denial and Projection Two of the most important mechanisms of defense in dementing illness, as pointed out in Chapter 2, are denial and projection. They need to be explained to caregivers as normal means of dealing with the emotionally overwhelming situation of literally losing one’s mind and control over one’s behavior. Caregivers need to be told how frightening it is to be unable to maintain a sense of self, how all persons use denial to avoid dealing with their own mortality, and how blaming others helps each person avoid full awareness of his or her deficits or limitations.
328
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
It is often necessary for caregivers to respect patients’ need for denial while addressing their realistic inability to cope with certain aspects of the environment. For example, it may be better to say that the car is being repaired or has been borrowed by another family member than to tell a cognitively impaired person that he or she may no longer drive. One woman dealt with her concern about her husband’s driving by telling him that she wanted him to supervise her driving. Projection is more difficult for caregivers to tolerate than denial because they are usually the object of the projection. When a wallet or purse, house keys, or items of clothing cannot be found, caregivers are the ones usually blamed. We tell caregivers that it is not useful to argue that one has not borrowed or stolen a particular object. It is more useful to institute a search for what is missing, accompanied by the patient if he or she is willing. It is also useful to note where commonly used objects or possessions are kept and to see that they are kept in those places if at all possible (see also Chapter 6). Delusional projection is even more difficult for caregivers because it frequently ties in with their own wishes and concerns. Thus, a cognitively impaired person may express concern that caregivers are trying to steal his money. That may be true, in the sense that the family is attempting to control spending so that the person will not lose all of his or her financial resources, but it may also be true that the beneficiaries are trying to protect their inheritance. Their own emotional entanglement may lead caregivers into vigorous denials that further heighten mutual suspicion. Caregivers find it easier to cope when, instead of being blamed themselves, they are told that strange-looking little people are entering the house and stealing money. When something specific is missing, caregivers need only institute a matter-of-fact search and produce what is missing, if they can. Many times, missing items have been concealed by the patient, who can no longer remember where they are. For that reason, it may be useful to periodically search the environment as a matter of routine to find such hiding places. When accusations are made by the patient that cannot be controverted, such as “I know you are stealing my money,” it is best to give a brief assurance to the contrary and to then engage the patient in some distracting activity. A reasonable reply to the foregoing accusation would be, “No, I’m not stealing your money. Breakfast is ready. Let’s eat.” If delusional projection does not abate and results in great perturbation for the patient or family, the use of antipsychotic medication may be temporarily indicated. At times, the clinician may suspect that the patient’s suspicions of family members or other caregivers are well founded. If the family is suspected of
Supporting Family Caregivers
329
financial or other abuse, Adult Protective Services can be contacted to investigate the situation. The family or proper agencies can be notified if abuse by professional caregivers is suspected.
Maintaining Self-Esteem Individuals in the early stages of a dementing illness have difficulty maintaining their self-esteem because they can no longer live up to their own notion of who they ought to be and at what level they ought to function. They also recognize that they cannot do for themselves what they have been able to do since childhood: perform simple calculations, follow directions, maintain orientation in time and space, button garments, tie shoelaces. As cognitive impairment progresses, the capacity for self-awareness and communication diminishes, and maintenance of self-esteem becomes less of a problem, both in terms of patients’ self-assurance and their ability to communicate concern about themselves. Many persons define who they are by what they do. Loss of capacity to work or to carry out one’s usual activities leads to a sense of uselessness. It is therefore useful for families to help persons with milder cognitive impairment to find activities at which they can feel competent. These may include activities as simple as dusting the furniture or helping to fold the laundry. Persons who enjoy and are kept occupied by housekeeping chores can be encouraged to do those chores daily—it makes little difference whether the house needs cleaning again or whether the laundry really needs to be folded again. Playing simple games such as bingo can be pleasurable. Each person is unique in the activities that are meaningful to him or her. Helping to discover those activities can be a positive experience for both caregiver and patient. It is often necessary for caregivers to redefine their notion of an activity and to reevaluate their view of their loved ones’ abilities. Caregivers frequently come to the clinic saying that their care recipient “can’t do anything anymore” simply because he can no longer engage in his previous activities. It is useful to provide examples of everyday activities that may be enjoyable and esteem maintaining, and how to initiate these activities. For example, less impaired persons might organize a drawer in the kitchen or roll coins, while more impaired persons can sort items in a drawer, sharpen pencils, test pens, or sort coins. Whatever the activity, it is important to help caregivers view an activity as a series of smaller, more doable tasks and to focus on the process of doing the activity rather than on the end product. The Pleasant Events Schedule (Teri and Logsdon 1991) mentioned in Chapter 6 is useful for discovering more activities.
330
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Impact of Dementia on the Family The development of a dementing illness affects different families and family members in many different ways. It affects spouses, companions, children, grandchildren, and (as in the case of AIDS) parents. Family members experience denial, bargaining, anger, depression, and acceptance—the stages of adjustment to disability and grief—many times throughout the course of the illness. These emotional reactions are especially prominent at diagnosis, placement in long-term care, and death, but also when patients have major declines in functioning or changes in personality. This process of adjustment and readjustment is complicated by the progression of the disease, and it requires practical lifestyle changes and medical management. Generally, there seems to be a theme or focus for caregiving in different stages of dementing illness and a task that must be negotiated for each stage. Like staging dementing illness, however, staging caregiving is imprecise, and the challenges that are prominent at one stage also may exist in some way in another stage. During the stage at which patients are experiencing mild cognitive impairment, the challenge for caregivers is awareness. Once caregivers become aware that things are not right, the task is obtaining a diagnosis (identifying the problem). This is not an easy task. It often requires that the caregiver deal with conflicting views of the patient’s functioning from the patient, other family members, and sometimes the patient’s primary care physician. Caregivers often are ambivalent about receiving a diagnosis. They are validated by the acknowledgment that something is wrong; yet are terrified to face the diagnosis. Mild declines are frequently unnoticed or glossed over by spouses of elderly persons. In many elderly couples, there is an automatic compensation by one for the other’s deficits. A husband will begin cooking as the wife becomes less competent in the kitchen, or the wife will take over household repairs from the husband. In our experience, it is not unusual for one member of a spousal pair to be physically handicapped but cognitively intact, whereas the other spouse is cognitively impaired and physically intact. In this situation, the physically disabled member guides the cognitively disabled person in doing the daily chores. Often, the first person to comment on the cognitive deficit of an elderly person with dementia is a son or daughter who lives at a distance and visits infrequently. Many times, it is difficult for the adult children to convince the intact parent that the cognitively impaired parent needs a medical evaluation. The challenge or focus of caregivers in early dementing illness is grieving, which is necessary for planning to begin. This involves grieving for the loss of the person, the relationship with that person, life as it was (nor-
Supporting Family Caregivers
331
malcy), and dreams of the future. When the cognitive impairment of one spouse begins to markedly reduce a couple’s quality of life, the unaffected spouse or partner feels frustration, anger, sadness and, sometimes, frank depression. A common reaction in elders is a sense of having been robbed of one’s golden years. Many elders postponed pleasures earlier in life for the sake of their careers or their children, in the expectation that retirement would bring comfortable leisure. They become frustrated at their inability to enjoy a time when they are no longer burdened by work or family responsibilities, angry at having lost the opportunity, and sad for their spouse’s loss of quality of life. They become depressed as they look forward to their situation deteriorating further and eventually losing their spouse as a person. As cognitive function continues to deteriorate, the challenge or focus is role adjustment. Adjusting one’s role to fit the caregiving task at hand (financial manager, social coordinator, housekeeper) is crucial. Because this is a time when more fluctuation in behavior, functioning, and cognitive abilities occurs, the task is often simply life management or crisis management as it frequently seems for families. The personal challenge of the late stages of dementing illnesses is disengagement from the role of spouse or adult child and assumption of the role of physical caregiver. The work involved in this stage is feeding, maintaining hygiene, and helping with way finding and ambulating. Caregivers at this stage often make comments like “this person is not my wife,” or “my mom is already gone.” Spousal caregivers experience difficulties in many areas. Although many persons are indifferent to their loss of cognitive function, others strive to maintain their premorbid level of functioning and autonomy. A key area of conflict for many couples is driving. A wife will note that her husband’s judgment is becoming impaired. He not only becomes lost while driving but also changes lanes without looking or makes left turns from the right lane. Although she does not want her husband to feel less of a person, she fears for his and others’ safety. Furthermore, she does not wish to engage in the endless arguments that arise from intensely emotional issues such as relinquishing driving. In situations in which a spouse has difficulty saying no, we often suggest that the blame be assigned to the physician by saying, “The doctor doesn’t want you to drive right now.” Our female patients frequently insist that they can still cook, despite their forgetting how to follow simple recipes and ignoring food burning on a stovetop. Strategies employed here include turning off the gas supply or electricity to the stove unless there is someone who can directly supervise the cooking process. Another key area of difficulty for spouses is in coping with suspiciousness or outright delusional blaming. Being constantly accused of stealing
332
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
and lying is demoralizing at best. Although spouses can be educated to not challenge suspicions and delusions, it is difficult for them not to feel unappreciated. In our experience, two situations most often precipitate nursing home admission. They are the advent of total incontinence in the dementia patient and the caregiver’s inability to get adequate sleep. Spouses seem able to tolerate occasional urinary incontinence. They are often able to manage toileting their spouse frequently and restricting fluid intake after supper to prevent “accidents” or using incontinence pads. Prolonged fecal incontinence is not well tolerated. The emotional transition from spouse to nurse often cannot be accomplished. When persistent incontinence appears to be a result of the dementing illness and not a result of factors such as urinary tract infection, bladder prolapse, fecal impaction, or improper diet, we begin raising the question of institutionalization with the now-exhausted spouse. The same is true when the dementia patient’s nighttime agitation or wandering make it impossible for the caregiver to sleep. Dementing illness affects both adult children and grandchildren. The adult children who are particularly compromised are those who are still actively raising their own children. They are sandwiched between caring for the generation ahead of and behind them. Our experience and that of others is that daughters and daughters-in-law are affected more than sons or sons-in-law. Brody (1989) suggests that this is so because women have more nurturing expectations of themselves than men do, but it may also have to do with the fact that men are more often employed full-time outside the home. However, this may change as more women enter the work force. It is often painful for children when their parents become less than fully competent. For many adult children, having an active, decisive parent is so important that they want to continue following that parent’s judgment and have difficulty substituting their own judgment for that of their parents. A surprisingly large number of adults continue to be emotionally and financially dependent on their parents and experience the onset of a dementing illness in their parent as a threat to their own integrity. There are also children who want to protect parents from the consequences of dementia but whose parents’ suspiciousness or need for autonomy is so great that they reject help offered regarding adequate nutrition or proper clothing. Many of our patients are widows who have lived and managed on their own for many years and are fiercely independent. In this situation, we try to help the children achieve a reasonable balance between safeguarding their parents and interfering with their parents’ much-needed sense of autonomy. Adult children are understandably upset that meals-
Supporting Family Caregivers
333
on-wheels delivered to the home are uneaten, that ice cream and cookies are preferred to a balanced diet, that clothing goes unwashed, or that the house is not kept clean. Our general stance is that, as long as fiercely independent persons can maintain their nutrition and personal hygiene fairly well, they will have a better quality of life living on their own than in an institution, and it may be worth risking that they can continue to care for themselves. Tensions also develop between the adult children of cognitively impaired parents. Ordinarily, one adult child assumes the majority of the burden of care. This primary nonspousal caregiver is usually self-selected and is most often the child who has maintained the strongest attachment to the parents over the years. At times, the role of primary caregiver falls to the child who is geographically closest or who is not employed outside the home. The primary caregiver’s closeness to the parent is often envied by other siblings, who often deal with their envy by criticizing the primary caregiver’s quality of care. He or she is seen as too domineering, too harsh, or too easy on the affected parent. Despite their willingness to criticize, envious siblings who act out their rivalry by criticism are often difficult to engage as helpers. Grandchildren are generally not strongly affected unless the grandmother or grandfather comes to live with them. At times, there is a comfortable relationship, with the grandparent enjoying the interaction with grandchildren and grandchildren tolerating their grandparent’s deficit well. Often, however, grandparents with dementia regress significantly and feel competitive with grandchildren for their parents’ attention. Children, in turn, may have difficulty with their grandparents’ incapacity and may claim that the elders are faking. Children who feel deprived of nurturing by the presence of a grandparent in the house may also act against the older persons and attempt to discredit them or drive them out of the house. Dementing illness in an adult child also poses problems for parents. Many parents appreciate the opportunity to do what they can for a child of any age. Others feel that their freedom from child rearing has been hard won and are resentful of intrusion on their middle-age or late-life style of living. They feel a wish to care for their children, as well as guilt because they would now like to conserve some of their energy for themselves. In the case of patients with AIDS, many parents of homosexual persons or intravenous drug abusers are resentful of the child’s lifestyle, believe their child has caused his or her disease, and feel still further alienated. Having reached adulthood, many children are resentful of parents reentering their lives in a decision-making capacity. This is especially true for immature adults or adults for whom autonomy from parents has been a significant and poorly resolved struggle.
334
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Caregiver Stress It is clear that living with or coping with cognitively impaired persons can be extremely stressful. In fact, there is now a large body of literature on caregiver stress in Alzheimer’s disease (Light and Lebowitz 1989). Interventions designed to reduce caregiver burden and stress are frequently ineffective (Bourgeois et al. 1996). Although many supportive community resources exist—including support groups, respite care, elder companions, and adult day care—few caregivers use these services. It often seems that caregivers experience a type of survival guilt, in which they too “suffer” with the disease and cannot allow themselves more quality of life than their impaired loved one. Many authors suggest that caring for mentally impaired elders is more stressful than caring for the physically disabled (Brody 1989). Also, institutionalization does not totally relieve caregiver strain. George (1984) found that stress symptoms were as prevalent in caregivers whose loved ones with dementia resided in nursing homes as they were in caregivers who maintained their loved ones at home. Families maintain their supportive activities even when loved ones are institutionalized (Martin-Cook et al. 2001). They visit nursing homes frequently, interact with nursing personnel, help with feeding, feel sad about their loved one’s condition, and feel guilty because they were not able to maintain their loved one at home. They often feel helpless to complain about suboptimal care for fear that staff will retaliate against their loved ones. Cognitive impairment progresses in Alzheimer’s disease, most persons appear to lose awareness of their deficits. To the family, the deficits become increasingly apparent as they result in a heavier work and emotional load (Pratt et al. 1985; Zarit et al. 1985). Despite the heavy emotional burden and frequent signs of emotional and physical overload, several questionnaire studies show no strong relationship between duration or severity of illness and indices of caregiver burden (George and Gwyther 1986; Zarit et al. 1980). Patient and caregiver factors such as health status and problem solving seem to have greater impact on burden (Zarit and Teri 1992). Dysphoria is extremely common. In a survey of primary caregivers, Rabins et al. (1982) found that 87% reported chronic depression, fatigue, and/or anger when asked about their emotional state. Coppell et al. (1985) found that 40% of 68 Alzheimer’s disease caregivers were experiencing depressive symptoms. Only 1 caregiver, however, met criteria for major depression. Before recommending means to partially relieve the physical and emotional distress of caregivers, the particular stressors need to be identified. This requires spending time with the family. In the setting of our dementia clinic, this is done during the initial interview with the physician, in which
Supporting Family Caregivers
335
the family is seen apart from the patient by both the physician and the clinic nurse. Caregiver stress is also assessed at the reporting interview, to which all concerned family members are invited. Aside from diagnosis and direct treatment recommendations, our clinic team tries to help families arrive at a management plan that is optimal for them and the patient. We state that our aim is to maintain the highest quality of life for all members of the family and that planning for a person’s care involves balancing that person’s needs against the needs of the rest of the family. On occasions, we recommend supportive counseling or active psychiatric treatment for a family member. So far, we have not suggested family therapy, but we would not hesitate to recommend it for the cognitively intact family members if we believed it was indicated. We also support professional caregivers doing home care, usually by telephone. They are liable to intense buffeting between family members with divergent points of view and often stand between an irritable, nonunderstanding spouse and the patient. Frequently, elderly spouses are only slightly less cognitively impaired than the primary patient and therefore cannot fathom why the husband or wife cannot understand. We offer continued advice and support by telephone and through regular revisits, and we suggest active involvement with the educational programs and support groups of the local Alzheimer’s Association and other sources of community support as indicated in Chapter 11.
Sexuality An area that is rarely addressed with regard to cognitively impaired persons and their spouses is the effect of dementing illness on their sexual feelings and behaviors. The need for affection and intimacy persist well into dementing illness for many persons (Davies et al. 1992). In some instances, especially with frontotemporal dementing illnesses (Joseph 1999), individuals become hypersexual. For spouses, sexual involvement with a partner who no longer provides an intimate relationship becomes uncomfortable or aversive. In our clinical experience, these statements hold true for many couples. In most couples in which the onset of dementia is after age 70, sexual involvement has already diminished and does not present a problem; but for younger couples with active sex lives, problems do occur. Spouses report that, as they assume a caregiver role in relation to their spouse and become more of a parent than a partner in the relationship, their sexual desire decreases. Some even report feelings of revulsion because it feels as though they are committing incest with their “child.” These feelings cause great conflict, guilt, and frustration for spouses who try to deal with them alone, and they are often not comfortable bringing these issues up with
336
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
professionals. Therefore, the responsibility lies with the professional to address this aspect of the disease and its impact on the relationship Volicer et al. (1988) suggest that caregivers be taught how to distract or gently dissuade sexually persistent spouses. In our experience, some spouses cannot be dissuaded behaviorally nor is their sexual drive reduced through use of tranquilizers or antipsychotics. We have found medroxyprogesterone to be useful in reducing inappropriate sexual aggression in cognitively impaired men (see Chapter 7), but we have not found an effective treatment for our female patients. Although there are some anecdotal reports that selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) have been effective in reducing sexual disinhibition, it is too early to conclude that SSRIs should be considered the treatment of choice in the elderly (Hashmi et al. 2000).
Long-Term-Care Placement Decisions regarding placement in long-term-care facilities are usually made by the family and are implemented when the primary caregiver becomes physically and emotionally overwhelmed. Chenoweth and Spencer (1986) found that 72% of caregivers indicated that being overwhelmed by 24-hour-a-day care was the most important precipitant of nursing home placement. Less frequently cited reasons were the caregiver becoming ill (21%), behavioral problems (18%), and incontinence (18%); many reported more than one reason. In our experience, long-term-care placement is the result of a process in which the needs of caregivers and care recipients are considered. It concludes with the decision that placement is best for each. The process includes caregivers dealing with their guilt over “abandoning” their loved one (usually a spouse whom they have promised never to institutionalize). It also includes acceptance by caregivers of their physical, emotional, and financial limitations, because hiring 24-hour help is more expensive than institutional care. An important obstacle to nursing home placement of persons with frontotemporal dementias is their unawareness of their impaired judgment and behavioral dyscontrol. Families are usually counseled to follow the path that offers the greatest quality of life to those able to appreciate that quality. Here again, an analogy to early childhood may be useful. Thus, one can say that a patient with severe dementia is very similar to an infant: not able to recognize the source of caregiving but in need of being cared for. The source of the caregiving is far less important than that caregiving be provided. Family scapegoating may become a difficulty in deciding about longterm-care placement. Family members living at a distance often tell the
Supporting Family Caregivers
337
family member managing the patient how unfair it is to institutionalize the patient. The physician can help to undercut this process by adding the weight of medical opinion to the decision. Families also need to be counseled that, once a nursing home placement is made, the length and frequency of visits should be such that the patient comes to accept the long-term-care facility as a permanent place of residence. Thus, families are urged not to visit daily or to visit for hours at a time. Instead, they are encouraged to visit a few times a week and for brief periods. Once the transition to the facility is accepted, brief outings can be arranged. In our experience, the transition to full-time long-term-care placement is facilitated by enrolling patients in day care at the facility, later using the facility for respite care, and finally using it for full-time care.
Ethical Issues for Caregivers The primary ethical issues that arise in dealing with cognitively impaired persons are related to following that person’s wishes with regard to independence and maintenance of life. Many persons who eventually develop dementing illnesses express the desire to be cared for at home in the event of a debilitating illness. Their loved ones agree. However, the time comes when the patient becomes an impossible burden for the primary caregiver, often because the caregiver has become physically incapacitated. At that point, the family weighs practical necessity against the earlier expressed wishes of the patient and decides whether that person’s experienced quality of life is more important than state of health or length of life or whether the needs of other family members take precedence. The family may decide that the patient’s quality of life would not be measurably diminished by nursing home placement, whereas the quality of life of the now-disabled caregiver might be immeasurably improved. Many cognitively impaired persons who live alone do not want to leave their homes despite their inability to adequately maintain themselves. In some cases these individuals are a danger to themselves. In other cases, they are a danger to both themselves and others. If failure to institutionalize would lead to the person’s near-term death by starvation, it is unlikely that society at large would condone such inaction, and the family might find itself opposed by legally constituted authorities such as Adult Protective Services. In the eventuality that a severely impaired person living alone refuses needed institutionalization, Adult Protective Services can assist in arranging involuntary longterm-care placement. In the case of a person living alone in an apartment who has inadvertently started several fires and has flooded the building on
338
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
several occasions by stuffing inappropriate objects in the toilet, long-term care may be required based on danger to self and others if adequate supervision cannot be provided. The conflict between maintaining life and quality of life also arises when dementia progresses to the point that the person is no longer capable of self-feeding and does not swallow spoon-fed food. Some individuals will have previously expressed their preference verbally or in writing to be or not to be sustained by artificial means. If they have expressed a preference not to be so sustained, and the family agrees, it is permissible to offer food and water at appropriate intervals and to let nature take its course. This position is supported by the Supreme Court of New Jersey, which held that every person has a common-law right to determine what medical interventions can be performed on him or her. That right continues after the person is no longer competent, as long as the person’s previous competently expressed wishes can be ascertained (In re Conroy 1985). The Conroy court also held that, where there has been no expression of preference on the part of the dementia patient, the family and physician have the right to decide, based on their knowledge of that individual’s interests and preferences, what he or she would have wanted. A decision to discontinue life-extending treatment was based by another court on the spouse’s testimony concerning his wife’s independent nature and dislike of physicians (In re Colyer 1983). When there has been no clear statement of the patient’s wishes concerning artificial maintenance of life support and when that person’s probable wishes cannot be clearly inferred, decisions can be made based on what appears to be in the best interest of the patient. To continue life support using the best interests of the patient as a guide, the generally accepted formula is that the benefits the patient derives from life should outweigh the burden of the treatment (Cantor 1987). The actual weighing of these factors is highly subjective and should involve discussion between physician and family, in consultation with a legal advisor familiar with the law in that particular jurisdiction. As indicated in Chapter 10, the U.S. Supreme Court has ruled that individuals have the right to terminate life-sustaining treatment or may designate a person to represent their wishes (Cruzan v. Director, Missouri Dept. of Health 1990).
Summary With no cure currently available for most dementing illnesses, an important role for health care professionals is family support. Educating families about the disease process helps reduce stress and anxiety, as does introducing families to a dynamic view of their loved one’s behavior. Making refer-
Supporting Family Caregivers
339
rals to other agencies encourages interaction of the families with others and helps to avoid feelings of isolation. Providing concrete management suggestions for specific behaviors and help with decisions about placement is also necessary as part of family support. Addressing family issues can also be important. Being available to listen to complaints and validate emotions when there is no specific intervention or solution to a problem is also important for caregivers. Through discussion and addressing specific questions, the health care team can assess, enhance, and support family members’ knowledge, emotional comfort, and decisions.
References Bourgeois MS, Schulz R, Burgio L, et al: Interventions for caregivers of patients with Alzheimer's disease: a review and analysis of content, process, and outcomes. Int J Aging Hum Dev 43:35-92, 1996 Brody B: The family at risk, in Alzheimer’s Disease Treatment and Family Stress: Directions for Research. Edited by Light E, Lebowitz B. Rockville, MD, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1989, pp 2–49 Cantor NL: Legal Frontiers of Death and Dying. Bloomington, IN, Indiana University Press, 1987 Chenoweth B, Spencer B: Dementia: the experience of family caregivers. Gerontologist 26:267–272, 1986 Coppell DB, Burton D, Becker J, et al: Relationship of cognitions associated with coping reactions to depression in spousal caregivers of Alzheimer’s disease patients. Cognitive Therapy Research 9:253–266, 1985 Cruzan v Director, Missouri Department of Health et al, 110 SCt 2841, 1990 In re Colyer, 660 P2d 738, 748, 1983 In re Conroy, 486 A2d 1209 (NJ 1985) Davies HD, Zeiss A, Tinklenberg JR: ’Til death do us part: intimacy and sexuality in the marriages of Alzheimer’s patients. J Psychosoc Nurs Ment Health Serv 30:5–10, 1992 George LK: Dynamics of Caregiver Burden. Durham, NC: Center for the Study of Aging and Human Development, 1984 George LK, Gwyther LP: Caregiver well-being: a multidimensional examination of family caregivers of demented adults. Gerontologist 26:253–259, 1986 Hamdy RC, Turnball JM, Edwards J (eds): Alzheimer’s Disease: A Handbook for Caregivers. St. Louis, MO, Mosby, 1998 Hashmi FH, Krady AI, Qayum F, et al: Sexually disinhibited behavior in the cognitively impaired elderly. Clinical Geriatrics 8:61–68, 2000 Joseph R: Frontal lobe psychopathology: mania, depression, confabulation, catatonia, perseveration, obsessive compulsions, and schizophrenia. Psychiatry 62:138–172, 1999 Kuhn D, Bennett DA: Alzheimer’s Early Stages: First Steps in Caring and Treatment. Alameda, CA, Hunter House, 1999
340
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Light E, Lebowitz B: Alzheimer’s Disease Treatment and Family Stress: Directions for Research. Washington, DC: Department of Health and Human Services, 1989 Mace N, Rabins P, McHugh PR: The 36-Hour Day: A Family Guide to Coping for Persons With Alzheimer’s Disease, Related Dementing Illnesses, and Memory Loss in Later Life, 3rd Edition. Baltimore, MD, Johns Hopkins University Press, 1999 Martin-Cook K, Hynan L, Chaftez P, et al: Impact of family visits on agitation in residents with dementia. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 16:1–4, 2001 Pratt C, Schmall V, Wright S, et al: Burden and coping strategies of caregivers to Alzheimer’s patients. Fam Relat 34:27–33, 1985 Rabins PV, Mece NL, Lucas MJ: The impact of dementia on the family. JAMA 248: 333–335, 1982 Teri L, Logsdon R: Identifying pleasant activities for individuals with Alzheimer’s disease: the Pleasant Events Schedule–AD. Gerontologist 31:124–127, 1991 Volicer L, Fabiszewski K, Rheaume Y, et al: Clinical Management of Alzheimer’s Disease. Rockville, MD, Aspen, 1988 Zarit SH, Teri L: Interventions and services for family caregivers, in Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics. Edited by Schaie K, Lawton MP. New York, Springer, 1992, pp 287–310 Zarit SH, Reever K, Bach-Peterson J: Relatives of the impaired elderly: correlates of feelings of burden. Gerontologist 20:649–655, 1980 Zarit SH, Orr NK, Zarit JM: The Hidden Victims of Alzheimer’s Disease: Families Under Stress. New York, University Press, 1985
CHAPTER
10
Legal and Ethical Issues John Z. Sadler, M.D. Barton E. Bernstein, J.D., L.M.S.W. Daniel C. Marson, J.D., Ph.D.
In dealing with persons with dementing illness, clinicians and researchers are faced with an array of ethical and legal ambiguities, requirements, and duties. This chapter deals with the legal and ethical elements involved in caring for cognitively impaired persons and for engaging them as research subjects. In the first section, we discuss the legal issues associated with dementing illness, describing key legal terms and how they are implicated in care. In the second section we consider the ethical issues associated with dementing illness, from both the clinical and the research perspective. Legal Issues Deteriorated mentality in legal terms has to do with the capacity to function within specific legal contexts. Early in the course of a dementing illness, the functions of competency and testamentary capacity may be relatively intact. Individuals with slight cognitive impairment retain the capacity to sign a contract, to marry, to stand trial for committing a criminal offense, to remain in the public domain, and to not be committed to a mental hospital. They are sufficiently competent to be responsible for committing a crime and to be denied the plea of not guilty by reason of insanity. These individuals can continue to manage their own affairs; consent to medical treatment; and keep, be responsible for, maintain, and control all their property, as well as their civil rights and duties. Late in the course of a dementing illness it is likely that competency and testamentary capacity will be impaired. Therefore, it is important for clinicians, early in the process of treating persons with dementing illness, to call these gradually evolving issues to the attention of patients and their families. Legal issues need to be addressed, if possible, while patients have the capacity to function competently. Legal competency is situational, and cer341
342
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
tain legal obligations are binding only when and while the individual has the mental capacity to perform that particular function. To relate dementing illness to competency, it is necessary to understand the interrelationship of the medical and legal definitions. Illustrative case examples follow:
Case 1 Mrs. S is a wealthy widow, married for the third time. She has children by her first and second husbands, and her current husband is encouraging her to update her will, offering him some protection in the event she dies. At this time she is forgetful, often fails to take her medicines, trails off sentences instead of finishing them, sometimes gets her grandchildren mixed up, and occasionally drifts away in the neighborhood. Her physician has considered that she may be in the very early stage of a dementing illness. Everyone in the neighborhood knows her, including the police officers and the mail carrier, and they periodically escort her home. Her new husband takes her to his lawyer. The lawyer drafts a new will, omitting her children and bequeathing all her assets to him, except for a few token items of little financial value that she bestows to her children. But does Mrs. S have the legal capacity to execute a will?
Case 2 Mr. P, a slightly deaf elderly man, was hearing voices, resisting medications, and feeling suspicious and had become convinced the CIA was going to “get” him. He wore old army uniforms, had numerous firearms around the house, and never went to the door without a loaded weapon. He gradually developed a fear of men in uniform. One day, the postman called to deliver a package. Thinking the postman was a CIA agent, Mr. P went to the door, and when the postman abruptly pulled out the package, Mr. P killed him. He was indicted for murder, and his lawyer raised the insanity defense.
Case 3 Mr. Q was a very successful businessman. He had accumulated a large financial estate. When his wife died, he was despondent, feeling that his world had collapsed. When he met a younger woman at a church party and she lavished him with attention and then affection, he was thrilled and couldn’t do enough for her personally and financially. His children were appalled when he bought her an expensive car and then a condominium apartment. They thought the woman was greedy, and they saw their potential inheritance or expectancy disappearing. The more he lavished gifts on “her” (always said through grinding teeth), the more she lavished affection on their dad, who never failed to show enthusiastic appreciation. One day, his children took Mr. Q to a physician, who brought up the subject of a “hint” of dementia. They were shocked and surprised. Their dad had been a forceful, independent, powerful captain of industry; always in complete control of his finances, which he had managed in a cool, care-
Legal and Ethical Issues
343
ful, and competent manner. Now he was investing “peculiarly” and tossing money around. This was conduct completely inconsistent with his whole life history. Is this the time for his children to consider a guardianship (of the person?) (of the estate?)?
The Process In each of the examples above, the issue is one of competence in a specific circumstance. The measure of competence to draft, execute, and implement an enforceable will is different from competence when challenged by a lawyer proposing the insanity defense. And should a guardianship of the person or estate be desired, the yardstick is still different. The mental health of the individual is still a concern, but the legal requirements differ; when cognitive impairment is part of the equation, the question becomes, “What is the degree of cognitive impairment?” The legal question is, “Has the dementing illness progressed to the extent that legal competence is impaired and in question?” At that point the diagnosis takes on a different form, coordinating the current diagnosis, or the diagnosis at the time of the act, with the legal requirements needed to determine competence to perform the act in the particular situation.
Some Standards The question here is, “Has the dementing illness progressed to the time that a determination is needed to make the stage of dementia relevant and perhaps controlling in each specific situation?” Although standards may differ from state to state and from jurisdiction to jurisdiction, there are many general principles and rules of law that apply to clinicians when determining competence in specific situations. To relate cognitive impairment to competency, the professional must consider the following traditional standards: • Testamentary capacity. Capacity to make and execute an enforceable will means a person must have knowledge, at the time he or she signs or executes the will, of the extent of his or her property, of his or her kin, and the general nature of his or her bounty and must have sufficient memory to make a reasonable judgment concerning these elements and their relation to each other (Campbell v. Groves 1989). In other words, does the person know what he or she has and who “ought” to inherit it? • Insanity defense. If the defendant raises the insanity defense, he or she must prove by a preponderance of the evidence (the least rigorous legal standard) that he or she has a mental disease or defect and that at the
344
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
time of the act, he or she lacked the capacity to appreciate the criminality of his or her conduct or to conform his or her conduct to the requirements of the law he or she allegedly violated (see Slovenko 1999). Does the defendant know the difference between right and wrong and that what was done was wrong? Could the impulses have been controlled? • Guardianship. When considering guardianship, the court will inquire into the ability of any allegedly incapacitated adult to feed, clothe, and provide shelter for himself or herself, to care for the person’s own physical health, and to manage the person’s property or financial affairs. The issues in the three cases presented previously will be determined by two somewhat unrelated factors. Did the individual have a mental defect, and did that mental defect result in a particular situation or circumstance in which the person is not responsible for present (guardianship), past (murder or drafting a will), or future actions (danger to self or others) (commitment and also guardianship)?
Other Standards There are numerous other legal standards that control capacity and competence (see Bernstein and Weiner 1980). These include the following: • Involuntary hospitalization or commitment. To meet this standard, persons must have a mental illness and must present an immediate threat to self and/or others. • Voidable marriage. A voidable marriage can occur if one of the marriage partners did not have the competence to consent to marriage or to understand the nature of the marriage ceremony. The competence to consent to marriage includes an understanding of the obligations and responsibilities of marriage. • Capacity to contract. The capacity to contract is determined by whether or not the individual did or did not understand the nature of the transaction and its implications.
Legal Needs Following the Diagnosis of Dementing Illness If the patient or the family contemplates making a will, organizing an estate, executing any of the various powers of attorney, petitioning for a guardianship, or making or executing any contracts, or if the individual is considering marriage or divorce, the potential progression of a dementing illness must be taken into consideration. There is usually no need to prepare for an insanity defense.
Legal and Ethical Issues
345
Because each family and each patient is different, the suggestions below are options to be considered and either implemented or discarded. The final decision is up to the family and patient, but this list of choices should at least be considered. Options and tools include the following: • Creating an inventory of all assets and liabilities, including noting the whereabouts of all important papers such as deeds, insurance policies, bank and brokerage accounts, and all papers that indicate the ownership of any property and the right to property. All important papers indicates all papers of any value or containing critical information. • Drafting and executing a will and estate plan with the help of a competent attorney, financial planner, trust officer, accountant, insurance agent, and any other person with the specialized knowledge needed to put an estate (even a small one) in order. • Durable powers of attorney and medical powers of attorney address management of the patient’s affairs in particular domains. Durable powers of attorney address the management of property in general or specific properties in particular. Armed with a durable power of attorney, family members, or at least one in particular, can manage the patients’ property should the dementing illness progress and should the patient gradually lose competence to manage his or her estate. The property could then be preserved and managed by a loving and capable family member. Medical powers of attorney permit one or more family members to make medical decisions on behalf of a temporarily incompetent (e.g., delirious) or permanently incompetent (e.g., advanced irreversibly demented) patient. The legal definitions and boundaries of powers of attorney vary from jurisdiction to jurisdiction, and so the clinician should be familiar with the state or local statutes and local uses of powers of attorney. • Guardianship. When and whether a guardianship over person or property is appropriate is always a delicate issue. A guardian of the person can consent to medical treatment without the independent consent of the patient, and a guardian of the estate can control financial matters independently without consent of the ward (the patient). The powers of a guardian are far reaching. The process is judicial and must be considered carefully, beginning with the initial diagnosis and continuing throughout the treatment process. Often, cognitively impaired persons resent, resist, and respond angrily to any of the above. Clearly, each of the above procedures represents the patient’s loss of control. For this reason, a coordinated effort must be made wherein all interested parties, including the patient, must be consulted. With this in mind, the capacity to consent in an informed and ethical manner is paramount.
346
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Ethical Issues Bioethics or medical ethics is often cast in the framework of dilemma-based or conflict-based ethics, in which two or more goods are in conflict, and the problem is deciding the proper course of action in the face of such competing goods. A paradigmatic case in the situation of dementing illness is posed by the good of respecting a patient’s freedom versus the good of restraining freedom to protect the patient from self-harm, exploitation, or neglect. Often these goals can be in conflict, as in the case of a demented person driving an automobile or refusing potentially life-saving health care. A second model for ethical problems in medicine is the virtue ethics or agent-based ethics approach. Rather than focusing on particular moral problems at the times they arise, virtue or agent ethics focus on the everyday conduct of the moral person (the moral agent). Under this viewpoint, moral agents should cultivate virtues (fidelity, truth telling, respect for persons, etc.) in themselves and others. In the setting of dementing illness, a paradigmatic example of the value of virtue ethics is illustrated by a clinician’s careful planning for long-term care, where the patient’s day-in, dayout environment and social contacts are aimed toward maximizing the patient’s “good life.” We provide examples of how dilemma-based and virtue-based ethics are complementary as we consider the unfolding of a dementing illness over a long or a short period of time. In the circumstances of early diagnosis and management, dementing illnesses are often developmental in the sense that the disease process unfolds over time. An evolving or progressive disease process suggests a consideration of the dementing illness from such a pathodevelopmental perspective, at least for the purposes of clinical management. At the earliest period of the pathodevelopmental time line, dementing illness can be seen as a collection of risk factors involving such considerations as genetic testing for at-risk family members of dementia patients, early diagnosis, and planning for the possibility of incapacity and infirmity later in life. In the setting of a first-time medical evaluation for cognitively impaired persons, issues of informed consent for diagnosis, treatment, or research arise, usually followed by (and occasionally preceded by) considerations of long-term care, assuming the condition is not reversible. If a patient has irreversible and progressive disease, ethical concerns around deciding on the patient’s behalf arise as the patient’s ability to manage his or her activities of daily living are lost. In the case of dementing illness when undesirable or destructive behaviors arise, matters arise concerning behavior control and the appropriate use of restraint and psychopharmacological interventions. Finally, persons with dementia pose special consideration of ethical issues at the end of life
Legal and Ethical Issues
347
and terminal care. Each of these general issues is considered in turn in the following sections.
At Risk for Dementing Illness Consider the following example: A middle-aged businesswoman, Ms. R, recently lost her father to complications of Alzheimer’s disease. Having labored though extended efforts to rectify her father’s lack of medicolegal preparations for his dementing illness, Ms. R has wondered if a diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease is her fate. She has also noticed some word-finding difficulties in herself as well as occasional losing of household items. She wonders if she should pursue a medical evaluation.
Being at risk for a dementing illness can mean a number of things. As in the case of Ms. R, being at risk can mean having a particular probability of having a specific dementing disorder. To be at risk can also mean the patient or potential patient is engaging in behaviors, or has another medical condition, that may be associated with dementing illness, as, for example, inhalant abuse in the former case, and having cerebrovascular disease in the latter case. On the other hand, for a person who is sensitized to the insidious onset of dementing illness, being at risk may be signaled by forgetting a name or a face. Ethical considerations for at-risk situations can be broken down into three general situations: prospective planning for disability and death, early diagnosis before or at the onset of any set of suggestive symptoms, and the relation of a dementing illness at any stage of development to other family members.
Prospective Planning The legal aspects of dementing illness described earlier point to an obvious lesson in prospective planning for any medical impairment later in life: the sooner adults put their affairs in order the more likely their wishes are to be respected, and indeed, the easier matters become later. As will be noted below, advance genetic or other diagnostic testing is not required to make evident the wisdom of planning for disability. All families should discuss the possibilities of medical impairment later in life, so that each person’s viewpoints are well known about such important matters as end-of-life care, inheritance of fortune and property, preferred stewards of medical care and financial matters, and the like. Early discussions of each person’s wishes, values, and preferences permit a deliberate and detailed understanding of viewpoints that may be needed later, in application to complex
348
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
clinical situations. Often such discussions result in the execution of wills, powers of attorney, and various sorts of advance directives for health care, but what makes the latter two legal documents particularly useful is that they convey a full understanding of the individual’s view about the good life and the good death.
Early Diagnosis Ms. R in the example above may represent the worried well, a person who may be vulnerable to exploitation because of her fears of Alzheimer’s disease or other serious dementing illness. Such exploitation may manifest itself through the selling of unnecessary screening tests and evaluations. Clinical features suggesting a need for a more thorough medical evaluation are discussed in Chapter 3. Diagnostic issues. Two issues often arise related to diagnostic procedures; one is the matter of consenting to diagnostic workups and the other, the issue of revealing the diagnosis. The issue of diagnostic workups can be considered under a risk-benefit rubric. For instance, in the case of a patient with human immunodeficiency virus disease, the possibility of a dementing process being arrested or even improved through a thorough workup and treatment is much more warranted than an extensive workup for suspected Alzheimer’s disease, for which treatment effects are not great. The issue of telling patients their diagnosis can be considered similarly. Does the patient want to know her diagnosis? People differ considerably on this matter, and requests not to be told the diagnosis do occur. What is to be gained by telling the patient? Often, there is much to be gained, such as the possibility of prospective planning for disability; disclosure of the patients’ preferences about treatment, research participation, and the like; as well as the provision of considerable support from the health care team, family, and support organizations like the Alzheimer’s Association. In sharing a diagnosis with patients, style counts as much as substance, and providing a supportive setting with adequate time to answer questions and process the patient’s emotional reaction is important. Such discussions should include considerations of treatment, course of illness, the nature of the dementing process, and the availability of various forms of support for the patient and family. Genetic testing. More recent ethics attention has been given to the issue of genetic testing for dementing illness, particularly early-onset Alzheimer’s disease and Huntington’s disease. The evolving organizational consensus over the issue of genetic testing for Alzheimer’s disease clusters around the idea that such testing is unwarranted and even harmful for asymptomatic
Legal and Ethical Issues
349
individuals (Butler 1994; Farrer et al. 1995; Kahn 1997; Medical and Scientific Advisory Committee of Alzheimer’s Disease International 1995; Post and Whitehouse 1998b; Post et al. 1997; Relkin et al. 1996). For earlyonset, autosomal-dominant Alzheimer’s disease, the question about the value of genetic testing as a diagnostic adjunct is still open (Post 2000; Post et al. 1997). Ethical matters concerning genetic testing for conditions like Alzheimer’s disease are more complicated than this firm consensus suggests. One ambiguity that can be diminished with time and more research is the empirical value of adjunctive genetic testing. A second ambiguity is raised by the probabilistic nature of genetic testing even where a clear-cut probability of developing disease is known. What will it mean for a person, for instance, to have an 85% chance of developing a specific disease at some point in the future? What about a 50% chance? What are, or will be, the psychological ramifications and can or will they require treatment? The attractiveness of predictive testing in the public’s perception is unclear. One recent survey of relatives of Alzheimer’s disease patients found a majority of respondents positively disposed to predictive testing under five of six hypothetical case scenarios (J.S. Roberts 2000). The prevailing reasons for seeking testing revolved around the idea that testing would aid in planning for the future. This is in marked contrast with the experience of predictive testing for Huntington’s disease, where fewer than 20% of a sample of relatives of Huntington’s disease patients desired predictive testing (Babul et al. 1993). One wonders if the respondents in the Roberts study fully appreciated the need for advance preparation for medical disability regardless of the risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease. Roberts wondered also if the assenting respondents were overestimating the availability of effective treatment for Alzheimer’s disease. Unrealistic expectations on the part of the public regarding the benefits of genetic testing and genetics-driven therapy has been noted elsewhere (Nelkin and Lindee 1995; Post 2000), and are subject to exploitation by commercial interests. There is also the potential for creating needless anxiety and even worse psychological sequelae as a result of testing in the face of no or deficient therapies (Butler 1994; Post 2000).
Diagnosis and Other Family Members The questions of genetic testing spread to the issue of the impact of genetic diagnosis (for e.g., familial Alzheimer’s disease) on the patient’s relatives. In this scenario, the relatives have a vivid interest in the outcome of the testing; some may very much want such information, whereas others may not wish to know. Here the considerations of consent, risk, benefit, and
350
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
respect for persons all come into play. Are there true benefits to be had from testing, and does it offset the risks to patients and other family members who have a stake in the testing outcome? What will be the implications for matters like insurability and employability? Clinicians should prompt families to discuss the pros and cons of genetic testing under such conditions.
Informed Consent The matter of informed consent with persons diagnosed with (or to be diagnosed with) dementing illnesses is complicated for a number of reasons. Most obvious is that the disease process itself ultimately reduces the affected individual’s ability to make decisions. The patient’s capacity to solicit, apprehend, and weigh medical information is compromised in varying degrees, and such variable deficits may also impede the patient’s ability to definitively choose a course of action. On the other hand, such variable impairment makes for no simple relation between diagnosis and incapacity to consent. Thus, with dementing illnesses, patients may be limited in both aspects: the informed aspect as well as the consent aspect. In addition, persons with dementing illnesses fluctuate over time in their decision-making capacity; for instance, a rested person in the morning may be sharper than a fatigued person in the evening. The clinical, ethical, and legal issues surrounding informed consent with cognitively impaired persons differ somewhat in clinical and research settings. Furthermore, medical standards for assessing capacity for decision making are still emerging; what guidelines have been developed are too often not commonly used, and even when they are, clinical conclusions about decision-making capacity may differ between physicians (Marson et al. 1997). Because of the foregoing ambiguities, significant amounts of variation in regional standards of care and legislative guidelines occur across the nation; clinicians are well advised to be familiar with pertinent local and state laws as well as the medical community standards for informed consent with cognitively impaired persons. Nevertheless, some further attention to clinical versus research consent and assessments for decision-making capacity may shed light on the entire matter of informed consent.
Informed Consent in Clinical Care Informed consent—the procedure of disclosing relevant information so that individuals may choose freely—applies as much to persons with dementing illnesses as to persons with other diseases. A diagnosis of a dementing illness is not in itself an indication that a person lacks decisionmaking capacity. Lo (1990) offers three general conditions indicating rela-
Legal and Ethical Issues
351
tively intact decision-making capacity: 1) a recognition that the person has a choice; 2) an understanding of the medical situation and prognosis and an understanding of the recommended and alternative treatments with attendant risks and benefits and implications of no or limited treatment; and 3) a relative stability of the person’s decision-making capacity over time (e.g., a relative lack of vacillation or morbid indecisiveness). For example: A 90-year-old woman with impaired peripheral circulation was brought to a hospital for evaluation of a gangrenous big toe. Her surgeon found dry gangrene with no evidence of infection and recommended that her toe be amputated. When she refused, psychiatric consultation was obtained to determine her competence to consent or refuse treatment. The psychiatrist found her to be mildly to moderately demented but not depressed or suicidal. She was not oriented in time but knew that she was in a hospital and that her surgeon wanted to amputate her toe. She knew there was danger of an infection developing and spreading if the toe was not surgically amputated, and she knew that the toe would probably eventually fall off on its own. On that basis, the psychiatrist indicated to the surgeon that she was mentally capable of refusing surgery.
In the case of a person who cannot meet these general standards for informed consent, the physician or the family may want to seek a proxy decision maker as described earlier. It should be noted, however, that for some clinicians and commentators (e.g., Appelbaum and Grisso 1988), formal legal declarations of incompetence are often unnecessary; using the remaining decision-making capacities of the patient along with an appreciation of his or her earlier-stated values and preferences often suffices.
Informed Consent for Human-Subject Research Public and professional desires for basic and clinical research in dementing diseases have resulted in an increased awareness of the potential for discriminatory exclusion of mentally ill persons from research as well as an increased recognition of the potential for abuse of research subjects with varying levels of decision-making capacity (National Bioethics Advisory Commission 1999; Oldham et al. 1999; Osborn 1999). The Code of Federal Regulations (CFR) establishes eight general rules for informed consent procedures for research (45 CFR46.116): 1. A statement that the study involves research, an explanation of the purposes of the research and the expected duration of the subject’s participation, a description of the procedures followed, and identification of any procedures that are experimental 2. A description of any reasonably foreseeable risks or discomforts to the subject
352
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION 3. A description of any benefits to the subject or to others that may reasonably be expected from the research 4. A disclosure of appropriate alternative procedures or courses of treatment, if any, that might be advantageous to the subject 5. A statement describing the extent, if any, to which confidentiality of records identifying the subject will be maintained 6. For research involving more than minimal risk, an explanation as to whether any compensation and medical treatments are available if injury occurs and, if so, what they consist of or where further information may be obtained 7. An explanation of whom to contact for answers to pertinent questions about the research and research subjects’ rights and whom to contact in the event of a research-related injury to the subject 8. A statement that participation is voluntary, that refusal to participate will involve no penalty or loss of benefits to which the subject is otherwise entitled, and that the subject may discontinue participation at any time without penalty or loss of benefits to which the subject is otherwise entitled.
These guidelines are the model for human-subjects research in the United States and are explicitly used by institutional review boards in structuring informed consent documents for clinical research. However, they do not address impaired decision-making capacity; the guidelines pertain more to disclosure of information than to the competent authorization of research participation. More recent debate over the 1998 report by the National Bioethics Advisory Commission (NBAC), Research Involving Persons With Mental Disorders That May Affect Decision-Making Capacity, sharpens the particular complexities involved with obtaining informed consent for research with subjects who exhibit impaired decision-making capacity. The NBAC wanted to preserve the clinical research enterprise and the good it confers to persons with diseases that impair decision-making capacity. At the same time the NBAC wanted to prevent exploitation of research subjects with impaired decision-making capacity (IDMC) (L.W. Roberts and Roberts 1999). The NBAC report suggested that 1) research should not be carried out on subjects with IDMC if the research can be done on other subjects; 2) research should be well designed and minimize risk; 3) risks and benefits should be weighed carefully; 4) informed consent should be required; 5) mechanisms should be provided to include subjects with IDMC in medically beneficial research; 6) communication with the caregivers of patients with IDMC should be maintained; 7) educational materials about IDMC and clinical research should be developed; and 8) governmental-privateacademic coalitions should be fostered to address this issue. Controversy emerged around NBAC recommendations concerning a classification of research protocols by three kinds of risk: “minimal risk,”
Legal and Ethical Issues
353
“greater than minimal risk with the prospect of direct medical benefit to subjects,” and “greater than minimal risk that does not offer the prospect of direct medical benefit to subjects.” Also controversial was the recommendation for development of legally authorized representation for subjects with IDMC who are recruited to participate in protocols with greater than minimal risk. Regarding the levels of risk, the research community favored a more graded assessment of risk other than the NBAC’s “minimal risk” and “greater than minimal risk.” For instance, Oldham et al. (1999) suggested adoption of the Belmont Report (1978) guidelines for risk, which included categories of “minimal risk,” “minor increase over minimal risk” and “more than a minor increase over minimal risk.” There was fear that the NBAC’s recommendation would unduly stigmatize and exclude patients with mental disorders or IDMC and that the heavier burden on researchers to obtain valid consent would result in less research on such needy but vulnerable subjects. The use of a legally authorized representative was opposed for a similar reason—that of unduly denying and encumbering the research endeavor. The Ethics and Humanities Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology (1998) called on the research community to present “reasons why other (less vulnerable) subjects cannot be studied and identify what additional safeguards will be implemented to protect subjects’ rights and safeguard their welfare.” The American Geriatrics Society (1998) advocated for “advance consent” for subjects with IDMC and, in cases where this is unavailable or impractical, the use of family or other surrogate decision makers who can represent the subject’s wishes or value system. True moral or legal consensus about the proper manner to safeguard human subjects with IDMC and to allow research has yet to be reached, and no legislation embracing the NBAC guidelines has been enacted. A few general principles seem clear: 1. Human subjects, including those with IDMC, require more safeguards than have been previously recommended. 2. Planning for persons with dementing illnesses should include not only financial, medical, and other personal considerations, but the possibility of participating in research protocols for that condition, particularly if the person otherwise has a favorable view of human-subjects research. Such discussions should include consideration of an advance consent for research (Dresser 2001; Sachs 1994) for jurisdictions that permit or support such documents. 3. Researchers as well as institutional review boards should carefully weigh risks and benefits for protocols involving subjects with IDMC
354
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
and should be willing to make modifications of protocols for further protection of subjects with IDMC. 4. Involvement of family members or other caring surrogates in the consent process and even in the study proper (study partners) should be used if at all possible.
Empirical Assessment of Decision-Making Capacity in Dementia One of the most important clinical and ethical challenges in the management of dementing illness is the assessment of decisional capacity (Marson and Harrell 1996). As discussed earlier, the determination of whether an Alzheimer’s disease patient does or does not have decisional capacity has critical importance in consenting to treatment, consenting to research participation, and managing financial affairs (Marson et al. 1994, 1995). Clinical assessment of decisional capacity, however, has been impeded by a lack of objective instruments and a lack of conceptual knowledge and training among health care professionals (Karlawish and Schmitt 2000; Marson et al. 1997, 2000a). Recently there has been interest in the development of standardized instruments to assist clinicians and researchers in making capacity decisions (Grisso and Appelbaum 1991; Grisso et al. 1995; High 1992; Janofsky et al. 1992; Marson et al. 1994, 1995). A significant portion of this work has focused on assessment of the capacity to consent to treatment (Dymek et al. 2001; Janofsky 1992; Marson et al. 1995) and the capacity to manage financial affairs (Marson 2001; Marson et al. 2000b) in dementia patients. Marson and colleagues have developed an instrument for empirically assessing the capacity of patients with dementia and other cognitive impairments to consent to medical treatment under different legal standards, the Capacity to Consent to Treatment Instrument (CCTI) (Marson et al. 1995). Specifically, they have developed two specialized clinical vignettes (vignette A, neoplasm, and vignette B, cardiac) designed to test competency under five distinct standards of consent capacity. Each vignette presents a hypothetical medical problem and symptoms, and two treatment alternatives with associated risks and benefits. The vignettes are presented orally and in writing to subjects. The administration format for each vignette approximates an informed consent dialogue and requires the subject to consider two different treatment options with associated risks and benefits. The two CCTI vignettes together take about 20–25 minutes to administer to subjects. After simultaneously reading and listening to oral presentation of a vignette, subjects answer questions designed to test consent capacity under
Legal and Ethical Issues
355
different legal thresholds or standards (LSs). These standards or thresholds have been drawn from case law and the psychiatric literature (Appelbaum and Grisso 1988; Roth et al. 1977). They are set forth below in order of increasing difficulty for dementia patients (Marson et al. 1994): • • • •
LS1: capacity simply to “evidence” a treatment choice LS3: capacity to “appreciate” the consequences of a treatment choice LS4: capacity to provide “rational reasons” for a treatment choice LS5: capacity to “understand” the treatment situation and treatment choices • LS2: capacity to make the “reasonable” treatment choice (when the alternative choice is unreasonable) (vignette A only) LS2 is currently not generally accepted as an appropriate standard for testing consent capacity, because of concerns about arbitrariness in determining what is a “reasonable” choice (Tepper and Elwork 1984). However, LS2 is still used in some studies in order to better understand the treatment preferences of cognitively impaired persons. The CCTI has a scoring system for each legal standard of each vignette, with high interrater reliability for standards with both interval scales (r>0.83; P<0.0001) (LS3–LS5) and categorical scales (more than 96% agreement) (LS1–LS2) (Marson et al. 1995). The CCTI scoring system permits evaluation of both a subject’s competency performance and competency status on an LS. Competency performance refers to the quantitative score that a subject achieves on a particular LS as determined by the CCTI scoring system. Competency status refers to the categorical outcome (capable, marginally capable, or incapable) of a subject on an LS based on use of psychometric cutoff scores derived from the performance of healthy control subjects. These authors have used the CCTI to empirically investigate the loss of competency in persons with Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s disease patients are a useful patient population for studying loss of competency and the pathological cognitive changes that mediate this loss. Alzheimer’s disease is the most prevalent form of neurodegenerative disease among older adults, and its pattern of neurocognitive change has been staged and well characterized. In addition, loss of consent capacity is an inevitable consequence of Alzheimer’s disease and can occur fairly early in the disease course (Marson et al. 1995). Thus Alzheimer’s disease affords a relatively clear and often dramatic view of the relationship between abnormal cognition and loss of consent capacity. In the first published study using the CCTI, consent capacity was evaluated in a sample of older control subjects (n=15) and Alzheimer’s disease
356
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
patients (n=29) (Marson et al. 1995). Using the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) (Folstein et al. 1975), subjects with Alzheimer’s disease were divided into groups of mild dementia (MMSE score of 20 or higher) (n=15) and moderate dementia (MMSE score of 10 or more but less than 20) (n = 14). Performance on the five LSs was compared across groups. As shown in Table 10–1, the CCTI discriminated between the performance of the control, mild Alzheimer’s disease, and moderate Alzheimer’s disease subgroups on three of the five LSs. Although the three groups performed equivalently on minimal standards requiring merely a treatment choice (LS1) or the reasonable treatment choice (LS2), patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease had difficulty with more difficult standards requiring rational reasons (LS4) and understanding treatment information (LS5). Moderately impaired Alzheimer’s disease patients had difficulty with appreciation of consequences (LS3), rational reasons (LS4), and understanding treatment (LS5). Capacity status of Alzheimer’s disease patients on the LSs was classified (competent, marginally competent, or incompetent) using psychometric cutoff scores referenced to control group performance on each LS (Table 10–2). Assignment of capacity status resulted in a consistent pattern of compromise (marginal competency and incompetency) among Alzheimer’s disease patients that related to both dementia stage and stringency of the LS. Patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease demonstrated significant competency compromise on the two most stringent LSs (LS4 [53%] and LS5 [100%]). Patients with moderate Alzheimer’s disease demonstrated significant competency compromise on both moderate and stringent LSs (LS3 [64%], LS4 [93%], and LS5 [100%]). These findings raised the concern that, depending on circumstances such as level of treatment risk and the standard to be applied, a substantial number of individuals with mild Alzheimer’s disease may lack consent capacity (Marson et al. 1995). More recently, the CCTI has been used to study consent capacity in cognitively impaired patients with Parkinson’s disease (Dymek et al. 2001). Like Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease dementia is a neurodegenerative process in which competency is slowly eroded over the course of the disease. In the study, 20 patients age 60 and older with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease and related cognitive impairment and 20 older control subjects were administered the CCTI. Patients with Parkinson’s disease performed below control subjects on all four of the established LSs: capacity to evidence a treatment choice (LS1) (P<0.03), to appreciate consequences of a treatment choice (LS3) (P<0.03), to provide rational reasons for a treatment choice (LS4) (P<0.0001), and to understand the treatment situation and choices (LS5) (P<0.0001). With respect to competency outcomes, patients with Parkinson’s disease demonstrated increasing compe-
Legal and Ethical Issues
TABLE 10–1.
357
Performance on CCTI legal standards by diagnostic group
Group
n
LS2a LS1 0–1 0–4 (SD) (SD)
Older control subjects Mild Alzheimer’s disease Moderate Alzheimer’s disease
15
4.0 (0.0)
0.93 8.7b (1.2)
15
3.9 (0.4)
1.00
7.1 (2.0)
6.1f (3.4)
27.3f (9.6)
14
3.6 (0.9)
0.79
5.9 (2.7)
2.3 (2.4)
17.9 (10.6)
LS3 0–10 (SD)
LS4 0–12 (SD)
LS5 0–70 (SD)
10.3c,d (3.8) 58.3c,e (6.6)
Note. SD=standard deviation. a No group differences emerged on LS2 (?2 =4.2; P=0.12). b (P<0.0001). c Mean for control group differs significantly from mean for moderate Alzheimer’s disease group (P<0.001). d Mean for control group differs significantly from mean for mild Alzheimer’s disease group (P<0.01). e (P<0.0001). f Mean for mild Alzheimer’s disease group differs significantly from mean for moderate Alzheimer’s disease group (P<0.01). Source. Adapted from Marson DC, Ingram KK, Cody HA, et al.: “Assessing the Competency of Patients With Alzheimer’s Disease Under Different Legal Standards: A Prototype Instrument.” Archives of Neurology 52:949–954, 1995. Used with permission.
tency compromise (marginally capable or incapable outcomes) across the four standards: LS1 (30%), LS3 (45%), LS4 (55%), and LS5 (80%) (Table 10–3). The authors concluded that cognitively impaired Parkinson’s disease patients are likely to have impaired consent capacity and are at risk of losing competency over the course of their illness. Parkinson’s disease patients appeared to have particular difficulty meeting more stringent, clinically relevant competency standards that tap reasoning skills and comprehension of treatment information. Thus, the CCTI is a reliable and valid instrument for the assessment of consent capacity in persons with dementing illnesses. The CCTI and similar instruments represent a promising objective approach to assessment of treatment competency in dementia. Although they are hypothetical, the CCTI vignettes and their administration approximate the physician-patient consent dialogue (Stanley et al. 1984) and can satisfy face, content, and construct validity to a high degree (Fitten et al. 1989). As discussed, the CCTI vignettes also incorporate different legal standards, or thresholds, for determining capacity to consent (Weithorn and Campbell 1982). The availability of different standards is important insofar as it allows clinicians and legal professionals the flexibility to vary the standard for
358
TABLE 10–2.
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Competency outcomes by legal standard (LS) and group Competent
Marginally competent
LS1, evidencing choice Control group Mild Alzheimer’s disease Moderate Alzheimer’s disease
15 (100%) 13 (87%) 11 (79%)
0 (0%) 2 (13%) 1 (7%)
0 (0%) 0 (0%) 2 (14%)
LS2, making reasonable choice Control group Mild Alzheimer’s disease Moderate Alzheimer’s disease
14 (93%) 15 (100%) 11 (79%)
— — —
1 (7%) 0 (0%) 3 (21%)
LS3, appreciating the consequences Control group Mild Alzheimer’s disease Moderate Alzheimer’s disease
14 (93%) 10 (67%) 5 (36%)
1 (7%) 2 (14%) 2 (14%)
0 (0%) 3 (20%) 7 (50%)
LS4, providing rational reasons Control group Mild Alzheimer’s disease Moderate Alzheimer’s disease
14 (93%) 7 (47%) 1 (7%)
1 (7%) 5 (33%) 3 (22%)
0 (0%) 3 (20%) 10 (71%)
LS5, understanding treatment Control group Mild Alzheimer’s disease Moderate Alzheimer’s disease
15 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
0 (0%) 1 (7%) 0 (0%)
0 (0%) 14 (93%) 14 (100%)
LS
Incompetent
Source. Adapted from Marson DC, Ingram KK, Cody HA, et al.: “Assessing the Competency of Patients With Alzheimer’s Disease Under Different Legal Standards: A Prototype Instrument.” Archives of Neurology 52:949–954, 1995. Used with permission.
competency depending on a variety of factors, in particular the risks and benefits of different treatments. The psychometrically based performance and outcome results of the CCTI should not be construed as an actual index of a subject’s legal competency. Rather, the CCTI is an adjunct objective tool to be used by health care professionals, in conjunction with other clinical and historical information, to make clinical judgments about the quality of an individual’s medical decision making. Although CCTI results indicated deficits in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease patients’ consent capacity performance and outcomes, such findings must not be confused with legal competency, which is ultimately a judgment of the legal system. Also, vignette methodology has certain limitations for assessment of competency. Decision making concerning a real and personal medical problem is arguably distinct from decision making in a hypothetical medical situation.
Legal and Ethical Issues
TABLE 10–3.
359
Competency outcomes by legal standard (LS) and group Capable
Marginally capable
Incapable
LS1 Control subjects Parkinson’s disease patients
20 (100%) 14 (70%)
0 (0%) 5 (25%)
0 (0%) 1 (5%)
LS3 Control subjects Parkinson’s disease patients
17 (85%) 11 (55%)
3 (15%) 5 (25%)
0 (0%) 4 (20%)
LS4 Control subjects Parkinson’s disease patients
20 (100%) 9 (45%)
0 (0%) 10 (50%)
0 (0%) 1 (5%)
LS5 Control subjects Parkinson’s disease patients
20 (100%) 4 (20%)
0 (0%) 3 (15%)
0 (0%) 13 (65%)
LS2 Control subjects Parkinson’s disease patients
19 (95%) 19 (95%)
— —
1 (5%) 1 (5%)
LS
Note. Capable=For LS3–LS5, scores falling at or above 1.5 standard deviations (SDs) below the control group mean on the LS. For LS1, a score of 4. For LS2, a score of 1. Marginally capable=For LS3–LS5, scores falling between 1.5 and 2.5 SDs below the control group mean on the LS. For LS1, a score of 3. No marginally competent outcomes are possible on LS2. Incapable=For LS3–LS5, scores falling below 2.5 or more SDs below the control group mean on the LS. For LS1, a score of 0–2. For LS2, a score of 0. Source. Adapted from Dymek MP, Atchison P, Harrell L, et al.: Competency to consent to medical treatment in cognitively impaired patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 56:17–24, 2001. Used with permission.
It is always possible that patients facing real, personal medical issues might demonstrate treatment decision making different from that elicited by the CCTI and related instruments. One must keep in mind that, when competency is legally challenged, it is a matter to be decided by a judge or jury depending on the jurisdiction.
The Humane Long-Term-Care Environment Persons with dementing illnesses often need sustained support and assistance for the full range of activities of daily living; such care is collectively termed long-term care. One of the moral virtues discussed earlier in providing long-term care is the maintenance of both a safe environment and an environment that sustains the dignity of the person and optimizes, wherever possible, opportunities for independent decision making and func-
360
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
tioning. Indeed, where ethical dilemmas occur in long-term care is often between the dual values of patient safety and patient autonomy, dignity, and self-reliance. In this section we discuss areas where the virtues and dilemmas of long-term care are commonly considered.
Independent Living Living independently and as one chooses is very important for almost every adult, and so it is for persons with dementing illness. When they begin experiencing progressive limitations in energy, health, stamina, or memory, many healthy elderly persons scale back their lives on their own: moving to smaller homes or with younger family members, purging a significant chunk of a lifetime accumulation of possessions, executing trusts and other legal instruments to help with finances, or moving to retirement communities that offer a simpler life. Such foresight and judgment may represent more of an ideal than a norm, however. In the context of dementing illness, such judgments are often lost, and patients find themselves in conflict with others about how they have chosen to live in late life. Family members visiting cognitively impaired relatives may find themselves repulsed by the latter’s living conditions, dietary habits, or hoarding of seemingly meaningless objects. Neighbors may complain of the unmowed, weedy yard or the home that has become an eyesore because of disrepair. In cases such as these the temptation is for caring others, including physicians, to step in and make over the patient’s lifestyle, usually with a predetermined viewpoint about what is the right way for the person to live. Such impulses should be resisted and be substituted with a careful assessment of the person’s wishes, preferences, and flexibility. Is the person otherwise healthy and adequately nourished, or at least adhering to a diet and/or treatment regimen that acknowledges the person’s limitations? Is the cognitively impaired person satisfied with his or her living conditions? Is he or she willing to accept help in areas where it is warranted: personal care, medications, and upkeep of the home? Does the person consider his or her social contacts rewarding? Such inquiries can often present opportunities for practical compromises that respect the patient’s interests as well as those of the family and community, although perhaps not to the complete satisfaction of all. Such compromises may include, for instance, use of a weekly housekeeper or visiting nurse, or fixup visits by a handy and beloved adult grandchild. In cases where the cognitively impaired person is frankly and dangerously self-negligent, careless in safety (e.g., leaving burners on in a paper-strewn household), or overtly aggressive, more custodial legal interventions discussed earlier may be indicated.
Legal and Ethical Issues
361
In the case of long-term care in retirement or nursing facilities, the humane environment is constituted by attention to the needs of unique residents, considering their interests, capacities, aptitudes, and preferences in designing activities and an environment that reinforces preserved functions and enjoyments. Particularly important, and too often neglected, is the social environment. Often retirement and nursing home residents are foisted into a strange environment, leaving behind old friends, families, and neighborhoods. Such a situation requires particular effort for all to assimilate the new resident into a new set of friends and social activities.
Driving The issue of a cognitively impaired individual driving an automobile deserves special mention, although it too is embedded in the matters of independent living discussed earlier (see also Chapter 6). The popular image of driving a car is as much a symbol of freedom as a literal exercise in freedom. The diagnosis of a dementing illness is not in itself a reason to deny an individual’s driving privileges, despite the fact that at least one state (California) requires persons with Alzheimer’s disease to register with the state bureau of motor vehicles for more frequent monitoring of driving (Post 2000). Particularly in the early to middle stages of disease progression, the physician is often poorly placed to make decisions about a patient’s actual driving skills, and for this reason and for reasons of patient alliance it is perhaps best for the physician to play the role of the facilitator of family decisions about driving rather than that of decision maker. Impairment in driving ability may arise over the course of the disease process. Such changing abilities suggest the need for frequent assessment and reassessment, although after a few years from the dementia diagnosis, most patients stop driving voluntarily or involuntarily (Drachman and Swearer 1993). When practical, patients should be involved in the discussion of driving at each step. In the early course of a dementing process, the patient may voluntarily exercise constraints on driving (e.g., during the day or under favorable conditions, or only to and from the store). These voluntary changes may be prompted by an episode of getting lost in the vehicle or of having a minor accident. In addition to the dementing process, driving abilities may be compromised through other unrelated illnesses or disabilities like visual or hearing difficulties. With the progression of disability, at some point a threshold is crossed at which not only the patient’s safety must be considered but also the public safety as well—which, for families, raises the issue of financial or other liabilities. When cognitively impaired persons pose a risk to self or others and otherwise insist on
362
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
driving, paternalistic interventions such as confiscating keys or impounding vehicles can be ethically justified.
Decision Making and Competence for Particular Functions As discussed earlier, the patient’s decision-making capacity or competence is a graded and task-specific function and often changes with time. The ability to choose for oneself is a basic dignity to be respected as much as possible. This means that ongoing assessment should be made not just of patients’ incapacities but also of their capabilities. Such capabilities can be respected by providing activities that encourage their exercise. For example, a woman with vascular dementia, who was impoverished in speech and had difficulty walking and remembering to take her medicine, was recognized as a skilled bridge player at her assisted living facility. Her peers praised her abilities, and the staff took special effort to get her to her bridge sessions.
Cognitive Enhancers for Alzheimer’s Disease: To Take or Not to Take As cognition-enhancing medications currently stand, they may provide temporary slowing of disease progression and even transient cognitive improvement, but they do not affect long-term outcome of the disease process. Nevertheless, on first glance the potential for a person with a dementing illness to regain cognitive capacity may seem an obvious and attractive choice. However, in the historical context of a patient’s (and the family’s) travails with the progression of dementia, there are moral pros and cons involved. For instance, a partial restoration of cognition may provide varying degrees of insight into one’s illness, provoking the experiencing (or reexperiencing) of significant anxiety and/or demoralization. Such provocation of unpleasant emotions may occur with patients who, as a result of disease progression, are unaware of their deficits. As another example, crises associated with behavior dyscontrol that have resolved with disease progression may be reawakened through such improvement. As a general consideration for the ethical prescribing of cognitive enhancers, clinicians and family members should question the partial restoration and/ or preservation of cognitive function as the sole measure of quality of life. In fact, administering such medications may contribute to a poorer quality of life in some situations.
General Medical Care Brauner et al. (2000) have called attention to the special case of general medical care for persons with dementing illnesses. They note three general areas for the special handling of general medical illness in dementia
Legal and Ethical Issues
363
patients: 1) cognition and language, 2) decreased life expectancy, and 3) exclusion from studies. The considerations for cognition and language may include several scenarios. For example, persons with a complicated, three- or four-times-a-day medication regimen may have difficulty reporting to caregivers which and how many medications were administered on any given day; this is especially of concern in the context of as-needed prescriptions. Persons with impoverished thought and speech may have difficulty reporting unpleasant side effects, as well as reporting the need for medication, as in the case of pain. Clinicians may have difficulty assessing dose titrations from patients’ own reports. With these communication difficulties, the humane and indeed effective medical treatment involves persons other than the patient in good treatment planning and management. Preventive treatments may be inappropriate, such as administration of cholesterol-lowering agents, or resection of a colonic polyp to prevent the possible development of a malignancy in a person with far advanced dementia. Such situations may not justify the associated risks of the treatment. There is also a need for policy changes to allow otherwise eligible persons with dementia to be included in risk-benefit studies in other medical illnesses.
Proxy Decision Makers Farther along in the pathodevelopmental trajectory, patients’ capacities for self-care and decision making are lost, medical interventions and environmental manipulations fail, and humane care requires decisions to be made on behalf of patients—so-called proxy decision makers. As discussed earlier under “Legal Issues,” careful planning during periods of capacity can make matters more manageable later. Appointment of a trustee or conservator to manage finances is one such arrangement; appointment of a durable power of attorney for health care (also known as a medical power of attorney) is a path for respecting the patient’s interests and values in the health care realm. In general, proxy decision makers (PDMs) should have an intimate knowledge of and respect for the patient’s interests, values, and viewpoints relevant to the matters at hand: finances, health care, etc. PDMs, in acting with whatever legal authority they possess, should make decisions based not on the proxy’s view of what is right and good, but rather on the basis of the patient’s view of rightness and goodness. Such a standard for proxy decision making is called the “substituted judgment” standard. In contrast, the so-called best-interests standard addresses what the patient’s best interests are. Both standards pose disadvantages, but the best-interests standard in effect may default to the preferences of the PDM rather than what the patient would have wanted. Best interests too often lie in the eye of the
364
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
beholder. This is particularly likely in the situation in which the proxy is in disagreement with or uncomfortable about the patient’s wishes, as illustrated in the following example: Mr. M, stuporous from end-stage Alzheimer’s disease, had repeatedly stated earlier in life that he did not want to live like a vegetable and that if he developed a life-threatening complication of his Alzheimer’s disease he would like to “be allowed to die with my rights on.” His daughter, however, when faced with the situation of aggressive aspiration pneumonia in Mr. M, hesitated to permit a do-not-resuscitate (DNR) order, because she felt that she didn’t want to be responsible for his death. The ethics consultant involved suggested to her that her father had said that he didn’t want to live this way, and she would only be carrying out his wishes, not sentencing him to death. After a tearful sharing of her sense of bewilderment and loss, she assented to a DNR order.
Proxy decision makers should also be as free as possible from true conflicts of interest. This is, of course, not fully possible, because often the PDM, as a spouse or adult child, stands to benefit financially from the patient’s demise. In some states, laws concerning medical powers of attorney and durable powers of attorney for health care permit physicians various options in complying with the wishes of the PDM if the physician believes that the PDM is not acting in the patient’s interests or is otherwise acting selfishly. The possibility for misunderstandings concerning potential or real conflicts of interest in PDMs underscores the need for a true primary care relationship between patient, family, and clinicians from the early diagnosis on, so that everyone agrees on what the patient would have wanted for himself or herself. There is a reverse side to the issue of best interests for the patient. This can manifest itself in the context of end-of-life care, when a PDM assumes that a patient with dementing illness is miserable without actually considering the patient’s behavior, affect, attitude, and the like. A moderately to severely demented patient may be incapable of new learning or of caring for himself or herself, but these profound deficits do not in themselves indicate that the patient is miserable and that, for instance, a DNR order is warranted. Indeed, all these deficiencies may be present while at the same time the patient relishes her meals, moves with pleasure in relation to music, and otherwise seems content. These sorts of clinical observations raise the issue of whether it is even possible for a patient early in the process of dementing illness to accurately predict life’s satisfactions in the later stages of disease. From this vantage point, discussions among family and patient in the early phases of illness should address quality of life not only as the patient imagines it (which is usually badly), but how life with serious dementia might even be positive in some aspects (Dresser and Robertson 1989).
Legal and Ethical Issues
365
Behavior Control In the setting of moderate to severe dementing illness, patients may engage in or be subject to behaviors that are troubling at best and dangerous to self and others at worst. The moral question then arises about justifying various sorts of behavioral controls on the patient, how to judge the appropriateness of behavioral controls, and how to administer such controls in a manner that preserves dignity. Post and Whitehouse (1998a) make four points about the management of persons with dementia complicated by disruptive behaviors. They advocate reliance on social and environmental modifications and creative activities to preserve independence and self-esteem. Wandering, for example, can be effectively managed by permitting wandering in safe areas. They advocate against physical and chemical restraints as a substitute for social, environmental, and activity modifications, and they urge that behavior-controlling drugs be used cautiously and only for specified purposes. Their view is supported by Health Care Financing Administration (1991) guidelines, which require that nursing home residents should be free from chemical or mechanical restraint unless it is clearly justified by precise diagnosis and require that the least restrictive means of control be used. Finally, Post and Whitehouse (1998a) advocate the creation of an individual profile of each person with dementia that would be available to facility-based caregivers along with an interactive and activitybased care plan known to be the most effective for that individual. Continuity of care is important, but continuity of caregivers is also a means to achieve such a tailored approach to management, in which a familiar face gives continuity and depth to a daily routine.
End-of-Life Care Compared with more rapidly progressive diseases like cancers or amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, the range of diagnoses that make up the dementing illnesses leaves ambiguities about what constitutes a truly terminal illness. Dementing illnesses range from rapidly progressive conditions such as Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, to irregularly progressive syndromes such as vascular dementia, to perhaps the paradigmatic terminal dementing illness, Alzheimer’s disease. In each of these cases there will be times in the course of illness when it is clear that nothing more can be done and that to continue to treat would be more cruel than kind. Variability in disease phenomenology, etiology, and progression poses special problems for care at the end of life. Perhaps most vivid is the practical difficulty of transferring terminally ill persons with dementia into hospice care because of the Medicare requirement that hospice candidates
366
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
must have 6 months or less to live. In the case of dementing illnesses, such estimates are hard to make with confidence, at least until painful or unpleasant complications have supervened. For these reasons, early planning and ongoing discussion are once again essential. The family and the primary care physician should have ongoing discussions with the dementia patient regarding end-of-life issues. Revisiting issues over time is particularly important with patients who have been diagnosed relatively later in the disease process, so that an emerging pattern of the patient’s responses can be tallied over time, thus increasing the possibility of obtaining a pattern of preferences that are consistent and coherent. What should be discussed concerning the end of life? Certainly the patient’s general viewpoints about what constitutes the good death. Some people want to fight to the last, others are concerned about burdening their families, still others do not want to live without any responsiveness, still others again have cultural-religious convictions that prescribe scenarios around end-of-life decisions. If the patient were to have a cardiac arrest at home, would he or she want to be resuscitated? What about in a hospital or a nursing facility? If the patient is vegetative would he or she want to have artificial hydration and nutrition withdrawn? Withheld? At what point is meaningful life over for the patient, so that ongoing lifesaving or life-sustaining interventions should be withheld? What would it mean for an otherwise contented existence to be characterized by profound cognitive deficits? There is considerable legal support for the right of irreversibly and terminally ill patients with dementia to be allowed to die (e.g., In re Conroy 1985; In re Dinnerstein 1978). Although there is variation in state laws supporting the discontinuation or withholding of treatments in the setting of irreversible and terminal illnesses, most states in the United States have such statutes to support physicians and families wishing to discontinue or withhold treatments at the end of life. Persons who do not have advance directives or medical powers of attorney, and whose preferences at the end of life are not known, should have the health care institution’s ethics committee consider their case. Under the Joint Commission on Accreditation of Healthcare Organizations (McCarrick 1992), all health care institutions, including nursing facilities, are required to have a functioning ethics committee to hear out cases about the appropriate use of life-sustaining treatment and to develop programs for assisting patients in developing advance directives. Such ethics committees can help families and clinicians sort out the ethical issues involved, but they can also familiarize the involved parties with state laws and procedures for withholding or withdrawing life support from patients, including that of artificial nutrition and hydration. The lat-
Legal and Ethical Issues
367
ter point is important because some states (e.g., In re Guardianship and Protective Placement of Edna M.F. 1997) may require persons to have previously stated their wishes regarding artificial nutrition and hydration in a document. Clinicians should be familiar with their state’s regulations, although most defer to the family’s ability to decide about artificial nutrition and hydration (Meisel 1995; Post 2000).
References American Geriatrics Society Ethics Committee: Informed consent for research on human subjects with dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 46:1308–1310, 1998 Appelbaum PS, Grisso T: Assessing patients’ capacities to consent to treatment. N Engl J Med 319:1635–1638, 1988 Babul R, Adam S, Kremer B: Attitudes toward direct predictive testing for the Huntington disease gene. JAMA 270:2321–2325, 1993 Belmont Report: Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Research. Washington, DC, U.S. Government Printing Office, 1978 Bernstein B, Weiner M: The many faces of competence. Tex Med 76:54–57, 1980 Brauner DJ, Muir JC, Sachs GA: Treating nondementia illnesses in patients with dementia. JAMA 283:3230–3234, 2000 Butler RN: ApoE: new risk factor for Alzheimer’s. Geriatrics 49:10–11, 1994 Campbell v Groves, 774 SW 2d 717, 8th District El Paso (1989) Code of Federal Regulations, Title 45, Vol 1, Revised. Washington, DC, U.S. Government Printing Office, October 1, 1999, pp 114–116 Drachman DA, Swearer JM: Driving and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 43:2448– 2456 1993 Dresser R: Advance directives in dementia research: promoting autonomy and protecting subjects. IRB: Ethics and Human Research 23:1–6, 2001 Dresser R, Robertson J: Quality of life and non-treatment decisions for incompetent patients: a critique of the orthodox approach. Law Med Health Care 17:234–244, 1989 Dymek MP, Atchison P, Harrell L, et al: Competency to consent to medical treatment in cognitively impaired patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 56:17–24, 2001 Ethics and Humanities Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology: Ethical issues in clinical research in neurology: advancing knowledge and protecting human research subjects. Neurology 50:592–595, 1998 Farrer LA, Brin MF, Elsas L, et al: Statement on use of apolipoprotein E testing for Alzheimer disease. JAMA 274:1627–1629, 1995 Fitten L, Lusky R, Hamann C: Assessing treatment decision-making capacity in elderly nursing home residents. J Am Geriatr Soc 38:1097–1104, 1989 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189– 198, 1975
368
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Grisso T, Appelbaum P: Mentally ill and non-mentally ill patients’ abilities to understand informed consent disclosure for medication. Law and Human Behavior 15:377–388, 1991 Grisso T, Appelbaum P, Mulvey E, et al: The MacArthur Treatment Competence Study, II: measures of abilities related to competence to consent to treatment. Law and Human Behavior 19:127–148, 1995 Health Care Financing Administration: Medicare and Medicaid: requirements for long-term care facilities; final registration. Federal Register 56:48865–48921, 1991 High D: Research with Alzheimer’s disease subjects: informed consent and proxy decision-making. J Am Geriatr Soc 40:950–957, 1992 In re Dinnerstein, 380 NE2nd 134 (Mass App Ct 1978) In re Conroy, 486 A2d 1209 (NJ 1985) In re Guardianship and Protective Placement of Edna M.F. (Wis June 12, 1997) (No 95-2719) Janofsky J, McCarthy RJ, Folstein MF: The Hopkins Competency Assessment Test: a brief method for evaluating patients’ capacity to give informed consent. Hosp Community Psychiatry 43:132–136, 1992 Kahn J: Ethical issues in genetic testing for Alzheimer’s disease. Geriatrics 52 (suppl 2):S30–S32, 1997 Karlawish J, Schmitt F: Why physicians need to become more proficient in assessing their patients’ competency and how they can achieve this. J Am Geriatr Soc 48:1014–1016, 2000 Lo B: Assessing decision-making capacity. Law Med Health Care 18:193–201, 1990 Marson DC: Loss of financial capacity in dementia: conceptual and empirical approaches. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition 8:164–181, 2001 Marson DC, Harrell LE: Decision making capacity: in reply. Arch Neurol 53:589– 590, 1996 Marson DC, Schmitt FA, Ingram KK, et al: Determining the competency of Alzheimer patients to consent to treatment and research. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 8 (suppl 4):5–18, 1994 Marson DC, Ingram KK, Cody HA, et al: Assessing the competency of patients with Alzheimer’s disease under different legal standards: a prototype instrument. Arch Neurol 52:949–954, 1995 Marson DC, McInturff B, Hawkins L, et al: Consistency of physician judgments of capacity to consent in mild Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 45:453–457, 1997 Marson DC, Earnst KS, Jamil F, et al: Consistency of physicians’ legal standard and personal judgments of competency in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 48:911–918, 2000a Marson DC, Sawrie SM, Snyder S, et al: Assessing financial capacity in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: a prototype instrument. Arch Neurol 57:877–884, 2000b McCarrick PM: Ethics committees in hospitals. Kennedy Inst Ethics J 2:285–306, 1992
Legal and Ethical Issues
369
Medical and Scientific Advisory Committee of Alzheimer’s Disease International: Consensus statement on predictive testing for Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 9:182–187, 1995 Meisel A: Barriers to forgoing nutrition and hydration in nursing homes. Am J Law Med 21:335–382, 1995 National Bioethics Advisory Commission: Research Involving Human Biological Materials: Ethical Issues and Policy Guidance, Vol 1. Rockville, MD, National Bioethics Advisory Commission, 1999 Nelkin D, Lindee MS: The DNA Mystique: The Gene as a Cultural Icon. New York, Freeman, 1995 Oldham JM, Haimowitz S, Delano J: Protection of persons with mental disorders from research risk: a response to the report of the National Bioethics Advisory Commission. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56:688–693, 1999 Osborn DPJ: Research and ethics: leaving exclusion behind. Curr Opin Psychiatry 12:601–604, 1999 Post SG: The Moral Challenge of Alzheimer Disease, 2nd Edition. Baltimore, MD, Johns Hopkins University Press, 2000 Post SG, Whitehouse PJ: Fairhill guidelines on the ethics of the care of people with Alzheimer’s disease: a clinician’s summary. J Am Geriatr Soc 43:1423–1429, 1998a Post SG, Whitehouse PJ (eds): Genetic Testing for Alzheimer Disease: Ethical and Clinical Issues. Baltimore, MD, Johns Hopkins University Press, 1998b Post SG, Whitehouse PJ, Binstock RH, et al: The clinical introduction of genetic testing for Alzheimer disease: an ethical perspective. JAMA 277:832–836, 1997 Relkin NR, Kwon YJ, Tsai J, et al: The National Institute on Aging/Alzheimer’s Association recommendations on the application of apolipoprotein E genotyping to Alzheimer’s disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 802:149–176, 1996 Roberts JS: Anticipating response to predictive genetic testing for Alzheimer’s disease: a survey of first-degree relatives. Gerontologist 40:43–52, 2000 Roberts LW, Roberts B: Psychiatric research ethics: an overview of evolving guidelines and current ethical dilemmas in the study of mental illness. Biol Psychiatry 46:1025–1038, 1999 Roth L, Meisel A, Lidz C: Tests of competency to consent to treatment. Am J Psychiatry 134:279–284, 1977 Sachs GA: Advance consent for dementia research. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 8 (suppl 4):19–27, 1994 Slovenko R: The mental disability requirement in the insanity defense. Behav Sci Law 17:165–177, 1999 Stanley B, Guido J, Stanley M, et al: The elderly patient and informed consent: empirical findings. JAMA 252:1302–1306, 1984 Tepper A, Elwork A: Competency to consent to treatment as a psychological construct. Law Hum Behav 8:205–223, 1984 Weithorn LA, Campbell SB: The competency of children and adolescents to make informed treatment decisions. Child Dev 53:1589–1598, 1982
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER
11
Mobilizing Community Resources Doris Svetlik, B.S.N., R.N., M.S. Jan W. Weaver, Ph.D., R.N.
As noted in Chapter 1, most of the dementing illnesses seen clinically are not reversible, and many are progressive. For that reason, health care providers’ roles in the ongoing care of patients with dementing illness include helping families cope with their emotional reactions and physical stressors, supporting families in making complex care decisions, and facilitating the use of appropriate community professionals and resources. The purpose of this chapter is to help physicians and other clinicians assist caregivers in locating and utilizing community resources. This is a difficult task, because the needs of families change constantly as the patients’ status changes with the progression of the disease. Options for care and support interventions for patients and families are constantly changing and expanding. Use of these care options is further complicated when families live in areas where service provisions are scarce or when families adhere to cultural beliefs that differ from those of the service provider. Resource Needs and Factors Influencing Utilization A variety of services and care options may be needed for persons with dementing illness and their families. Table 11–1 lists such services.
Factors Influencing Recognition Factors influencing the recognition that help from community resources might be needed relate to the patient’s functional status and the degree of burden perceived by the caregiver. Once information is obtained about the physical, psychological, and social status of the patient and caregiver, deci371
372
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
TABLE 11–1. Services that may be needed for dementia patients and their families Medical care including treatment of coexisting medical conditions Psychotropic medications Multidimensional assessment Skilled nursing Physical therapy Occupational therapy Speech therapy Day care Respite care Caregiver education and training Caregiver counseling Family support groups Patient counseling Legal services Nutrition counseling Financial/benefits counseling Protective services Home health aide Homemaker Paid companion/sitter Shopping Home-delivered meals Chore services Telephone reassurance Personal emergency response system Recreation/exercise Transportation Escort service Special equipment (ramps, hospital bed, geri-chair, etc.) Source. Reprinted from Office of Technology Assessment: Losing a Million Minds: Confronting the Tragedy of Alzheimer's Disease and Other Dementias. Washington, DC, Office of Technology Assessment, 1987.
sions can be made in accordance with the availability of resources and the patient’s and family’s wishes.
Functional Status The periodic assessment of functional limitations is an integral part of evaluating patients’ needs. A functional assessment helps determine the person’s degree of independence or the amount of assistance required with activities of daily living. It guides optimal placement in residential facilities
Mobilizing Community Resources
373
or referral to community care settings. Declining functional status combined with cognitive impairment often triggers a move from communitybased to residential long-term care (Miller et al. 1999). With respect to persons with dementing illnesses, clinicians must rely on family caregivers’ reports of the patient’s abilities even though the judgments of these caregivers may be compromised. Family members tend to minimize or deny functional difficulties that might be quite apparent to others, especially if they are depressed or feel burdened (Loewenstein et al. 2001). Nonetheless, caregivers’ reports generally correlate with the deterioration in cognitive function and are much more reliable than the reports of patients themselves.
Caregiver Burden The existence of caregiver burden in families of dementia patients is well documented (Zarit 1989). The degree of burden is variable and depends on patient characteristics and caregiver factors such as age, health status, gender, personality, coping characteristics, amount of social supports, problem-solving ability, and the relationship between dementia patient and caregiver (Zarit and Teri 1992). A relatively small number of studies have been conducted that examined differences between caregivers of patients with and without dementia. Some studies (Cattanach and Tebes 1991; Draper et al. 1992) have reported few differences in caregiver burden and depression between dementia and nondementia caregivers. Others have reported that dementia caregivers experience significantly more negative effects than do nondementia caregivers (Hooker et al. 1998). Clyburn et al. (2000) report that caregiver burden is often more severe for caregivers of dementia patients than for other caregivers. There is a higher incidence of depression in these caregivers due to a greater frequency of disturbing behaviors by the patient and lower informal support. Although the effectiveness of care and support services in reducing caregiver burden is unclear (Zarit and Teri 1992), the desirability of these services is well documented (Caserta et al. 1987). A number of factors contribute to caregiver burden and depression, including the caregiver’s relationship to the patient, living arrangements, and the amount of assistance available to the caregiver. Characteristics of the patient and the caregiver also contribute. A higher frequency of behavioral problems manifested by patients has been identified as a strong predictor of distress and plays a significant role in the caregiver’s decision to institutionalize persons with dementing illnesses (Clyburn et al. 2000). Progression of cognitive impairment alone generally does not increase caregiver burden; however, as mentioned earlier, the onset of functional impairments tends to produce negative caregiver outcomes. The caregiver’s health and gender
374
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
also play a role. Female caregivers are more likely to experience depression and burden than are male caregivers; caregivers of either gender who have lower levels of physical health are more vulnerable to the psychological consequences of caregiving (Chappell and Penning 1996; Zanetti et al. 1997).
Factors Influencing Utilization Decisions to use community resources are often based on the availability of informal support systems and the patient’s and family’s personal preferences. Such factors as income, insurance coverage, transportation, waiting time, and relationships with others enable one’s ability to use health services, assuming the individual is predisposed to using them (Mitchell and Krout 1998). All of these factors have some bearing on decisions regarding living arrangements and type of care provisions.
Informal Support Systems Informal support is broadly defined as care from persons who provide unpaid assistance. A number of studies have examined informal relationships ranging from provisions for individuals living alone to extensive in-home care by families or friends. These studies reveal a higher use of services, including nursing home placement, for persons living alone, or with a spouse, who have fewer helpers available (Mitchell and Krout 1998). A variety of arrangements and types of assistance are used. In some instances, families substitute for the use of formal service, whereas in others personal care by families complements formal services. The strongest predictors of service utilization relate to the actual needs of the person with dementia. Problems with perceived health, mental status, ability to manage activities of daily living, and nutritional risk increase the chances that health and social services will be used (Mitchell and Krout 1998). When these needs are coupled with limited support, health care and social services providers become especially concerned. Approximately 20% of dementia patients live alone in the community; 10% of these live alone without outside support (Office of Technology Assessment 1987). Almost 70% of persons with dementia receive informal care in a community setting. Mobilizing care and support services for these individuals helps to reduce safety and health risks and helps to maximize their functioning so that they can remain at home as long as possible.
Personal Preferences The aspects of personal preferences and rights to choose programs and services are fundamental approaches not only for mobilizing resources but
Mobilizing Community Resources
375
also for maintaining the patient’s and family’s sense of well-being. According to Reker (1997), existential needs of older adults account for significant variance in the prediction and alleviation of late-life depression. These needs relate to life concerns with an emphasis on the capacity of individuals to choose and be responsible for their choices. Persons who have healthy outlooks and feel in control of life decisions have a lower incidence of depression. Although a need for services may be clear and patients and families are encouraged to participate in the selection process, they do not always use the services that are available. An example of this comes from a 3-year respite care demonstration project conducted in four North Carolina counties. The project was developed in response to needs identified in earlier studies. Although the majority of families who used the respite services reported that they were helpful, only a small percentage of the families who were eligible for services actually used them (Office of Technology Assessment 1987). On the basis of 11 reported studies, the Office of Technology Assessment concluded that 1) only one-fourth to one-half of all noninstitutionalized persons with dementia use any paid in-home or community services other than physicians’ services, 2) among those with dementing illness who do use services, many use very few services or use them infrequently, 3) families of many persons with dementing illness only use services very late in the course of the disease, and 4) on average, families of persons with dementia use fewer paid services than noninstitutionalized persons with physical impairments. More recent studies confirm these findings and indicate that individual preferences are often influenced by the community context in which older persons reside (Peek et al. 1997). Differences are noted, for example, in selection of nursing home use between urban and rural elders. Rural elders are more likely to enter a nursing home than are urban and suburban elders due in part to the lack of community-based alternatives. However, when surveyed about their preferences, rural elders are less likely to report that they would enter a nursing home if their failing health prevented them from living independently. Helping patients and families accept needed services and convincing them that they require additional care or services is an important issue in that many families may not seek outside assistance until there is a crisis (Zarit and Teri 1992). In most instances, we cannot and would not force services on families or individuals. Some strategies, however, may be useful in facilitating families’ acceptance of assistance. The most frequently identified reasons that caregivers and patients do not use community resources include lack of knowledge about services and inability to pay for services (Office of Technology Assessment 1997). There are numerous personal reasons for not using available services. On
376
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
the part of caregivers, they include denial of the patient’s illness, embarrassment over the patient’s behavior, fear of disapproval by friends or relatives, viewing caregiving as a personal responsibility, being too overwhelmed with work or emotions to enlist outside help, not wishing to have others in the home, and feeling uncomfortable making decisions for the patient. Patient issues include unawareness of need for services, inability to arrange for services, fear of others recognizing their cognitive deficits, fear of being exploited, and cost of services (Office of Technology Assessment 1997). Health care professionals can facilitate the use of resources for patients and families by informing them of resources and by assisting in overcoming personal resistances to their use.
Access to Care Access to care significantly delays functional decline among older caregivers who are functionally independent (Porell and Miltiades 2001). Access was originally conceptualized in terms of factors affecting potential entry into the healthcare delivery system, but in the past decade the concept has been broadened to encompass healthcare outcomes. Therefore, a more current definition of access to care includes the timely use of personal healthcare services to achieve the best possible healthcare outcomes (Porell and Miltiades 2001). Indicators of access involve a number of dimensions, including insurance coverage, service utilization, having a regular source of medical care, and satisfaction with care. In rural communities, older persons often have access to fewer service alternatives than do people living in urban areas. A number of communities have remedied this problem by developing initiatives to improve the care of elders in largely rural areas.
Informed Decisions A thorough assessment of cognitively impaired persons enables families to see them more objectively, especially if they can observe part of the assessment (see Chapter 1). When health care professionals present specific information to families concerning their patients’ strengths and weaknesses, it enables families to make informed decisions about care needs and appropriate matches between the patient’s needs and services. Such information helps to minimize misinterpretations of the level of care needed by patients and prevents premature decisions regarding services or care that may be unnecessary at the patient’s current level of functioning. This information helps families answer the question, “How will we know what services we need and when?”
Mobilizing Community Resources
377
The importance of obtaining a thorough assessment also applies to the family. In dementia care, the patient and family are viewed as an integral unit. It is equally important to obtain information concerning the family, especially the primary caregiver, to assist the patient and family in coping with the disease. We know that many factors influence the level of burden that an individual caregiver experiences. Obtaining information about the caregiver and family system combined with the assessment of the patient’s needs will aid the professional in identifying caregiver needs. Whether the health care professional performs this assessment directly or refers to another professional to carry it out, the importance of this assessment cannot be overstated. This process can strengthen the relationship between the health care provider and family and sets the tone for recognition of the importance of the caregiver’s health and well-being. It can also lay the foundation for family members’ acceptance of help in the future. Also, the process of obtaining objective data from families on their informal support systems, caregiving skills, health status, strengths, and limitations may help them realize more clearly what their needs are. When people are asked specific questions in these areas, they may be better able to view their situation more objectively and in the context of the total picture of patient as well as caregiver needs. Health care professionals, especially physicians, have much influence over the decisions of their patients and families. The health care provider can use this advantage by formally giving patients and families permission to use medically related and social support services. Some physicians write respite prescriptions for family members who evidence difficulty accepting the need and importance of time off from their caregiving responsibilities. Acceptance of care services is a process in the same way as is acceptance of the disease. Helping patients and families to work through this process by providing emotional support and encouragement are valuable interventions. In general, services and care are evaluated for the following: • • • • • • • • • •
Quality of care Levels and types of services provided Staff characteristics and dementia-related skills Philosophy of dementia care and program goals Physical plant and atmosphere Location and convenience Cost Dependability Safety and security Reputation
378
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Patients and family members may have their own unique standards by which they evaluate potential and current services. They may also have their own mechanisms to evaluate services. These may include information obtained by word of mouth, their own personal feelings, reactions and judgments during a site visit, and communications with the service providers. Some individuals, however, feel ill prepared to make decisions about care and services. Feeling more knowledgeable about evaluating services may help them be more accepting of various services. Being an informed consumer of services also enables persons to advocate for improved care or to choose another provider if personal standards are not met. Family members must also be reminded that there is no right way to care for a cognitively impaired person and that they must determine what is right for them (Gwyther 1998). Patients and families will be more willing to use services if they feel they have some amount of control and are not helpless or powerless in these difficult decisions. Multiple guides, checklists, and other items available from sources in Appendix K can aid consumers in precare decisions as well as in ongoing monitoring.
Summary Caregivers of dementia patients appear to differ significantly from caregivers of nondementia patients. These differences suggest the need to tailor programs and services to the unique challenges faced by these caregivers (Ory et al. 1999). Therefore, effective resource referral requires more than giving patients and families a list of services or a pamphlet. It requires health care providers to recognize the unique needs of these caregivers and the issues that affect service utilization and to recognize their role in facilitating timely and appropriate use. Health care providers do not need a comprehensive knowledge of all services. They need instead to serve as a critical link in the chain that helps patients and families to mobilize community resources.
References Caserta M, Lund D, Wright S, et al: Caregivers to dementia patients: the utilization of community services. Gerontologist 27:209–214, 1987 Cattanach L, Tebes JK: The nature of elder impairment and its impact on family caregivers' health and psychosocial functioning. Gerontologist 31:246–255, 1991 Chappell NL, Penning M: Behavioural problems and distress among caregivers of people with dementia. Aging and Society 16:57–73, 1996
Mobilizing Community Resources
379
Clyburn LD, Stones MJ, Hadjistavropoulos T, et al: Predicting caregiver burden and depression in Alzheimer’s disease. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 55:S2– S13, 2000 Draper BM, Poulos CJ, Cole AM, et al: A comparison of caregivers for elderly stroke and dementia victims. J Am Geriatr Soc 40:896–901, 1992 Gwyther L: Social issues of the Alzheimer’s patient and family. Am J Med 104:17S– 21S, 1998 Hooker K, Monahan DJ, Bowman SR, et al: Personality counts for a lot: predictors of mental and physical health of spouse caregivers in two disease groups. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 53:P73–P85, 1998 Loewenstein DA, Arguelles S, Bravo M, et al: Caregivers’ judgments of the functional abilities of the Alzheimer’s disease patient: a comparison of proxy reports and objective measures. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 56:P78–84, 2001 Miller ME, Longino CF, Anderson RT, et al: Functional status, assistance and the risk of a community-based move. Gerontologist 39:187–200, 1999 Mitchell J, Krout JA: Discretion and service use among older adults: the behavioral model revisited. Gerontologist 38:159–168, 1998 Office of Technology Assessment: Losing a Million Minds: Confronting the Tragedy of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias. Washington, DC, Office of Technology Assessment, 1987 Ory MG, Hoffman RR, Yee JL, et al: Prevalence and impact of caregiving: a detailed comparison between dementia and nondementia caregivers. Gerontologist 39:177–185, 1999 Peek CW, Coward RT, Lee GR, et al: The influence of community context on the preferences of older adults for entering a nursing home. Gerontologist 37:533–542, 1997 Porell FW, Miltiades HB: Access to care and functional status change among aged Medicare beneficiaries. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 56:S69–S83, 2001 Reker GT: Personal meaning, optimism and choice: existential predictors of depression in community and institutional elderly. Gerontologist 37:709–716, 1997 Zanetti O, Frisoni GB, Bianchetti A, et al: Depressive symptoms of Alzheimer caregivers are mainly due to personal rather than patient factors. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 13:358–367, 1997 Zarit SH: Do we need another “stress and caregiving” study? Gerontologist 29: 147–148, 1989 Zarit SH, Teri L: Interventions and services for family caregivers, in Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics. Edited by Schaie K, Lawton MP. New York, Springer, 1992, pp 287–310
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER
12
Nursing Care for Persons With Cognitive Impairment Elaine Souder, Ph.D., R.N. Cornelia Beck, R.N., Ph.D.
Nursing care of cognitively impaired individuals takes on increasing significance with the growing numbers of persons diagnosed with dementing illnesses, and the heavy burden assumed by family and institutional caregivers. This chapter focuses on care issues from the perspective of supporting activities of daily living and managing disturbing behaviors. In this discussion, we emphasize, where possible, the research basis for interventions. In areas that lack research, we identify empirically derived interventions from best practices guidelines. To find relevant studies, we used the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, which contains evidence-based reviews of randomized, controlled trials of effective interventions. Although this type of research is considered the best evidence, it is difficult to conduct with cognitively impaired persons. In the absence of studies classified as best evidence we sought to identify the best examples of clinical practice. One source was clinical practice guidelines developed by private-sector panels for the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. The guidelines (based on extensive review of the literature, expert judgment, and group consensus) are accepted as the current standards of care. For each problem area, we provide brief comments about the extent of the problem and its cost (when available) and summarize the research findings in chronological order. Communication Effective communication with cognitively impaired persons is essential, whether it is in relation to socializing, providing guidance, or intervening with problematic behaviors. Although research is scarce, there is consider381
382
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
able agreement about ways to communicate effectively with cognitively impaired persons. Several commonsense, though frequently overlooked, recommendations are to decrease or eliminate competing environmental noise from television, radio, unrelated conversations, and other sources. If the cognitively impaired person normally uses a hearing aid, this should be turned on and positioned correctly. Gaining the person’s attention is the second step in communicating. It is important to introduce oneself each time, because memory may fail. To gain the person’s attention, it is helpful to gently touch the arm or shoulder (unless this causes a negative reaction), face the individual, and make eye contact. Caregiver speech patterns need to accommodate patients’ decreased memory span. It is helpful to speak in short sentences, with one thought in each. It is best to avoid pronouns that are normally used in face-to-face conversation and to use proper names, or names of objects, to aid comprehension. Instead of “Please give that to me” it would be better to say, “Please give me the glass.” Multiple-step instructions should be avoided. Speaking in positive sentences is preferable to making a series of negative requests. It would be better to say, “Stay here with me,” than “Don’t leave the room.” Questions can be phrased so that a yes or no response suffices. Sufficient amount of time needs to be allowed for responses to questions. Due to delayed processing, persons with dementia often require more time than usual to respond. If questions or requests need to be repeated, the exact same words should be used. Different words or more information may add to confusion. Providing a relevant context is useful when conversing with cognitively impaired persons. Because traditional orienting cues about hour, day, and date may not be meaningful, Zimmermann (1998) suggests that relevant cues for cognitively impaired persons may relate to getting dressed, mealtime, or time to sleep. As verbal language deteriorates, nonverbal communication assumes a larger role (Feil 1992). Gwyther (1995) recommends freely using nonverbal praise, for example, nodding, patting, or smiling. It is crucial to remember that even severely impaired persons have ordinary emotional needs and reactions.
Activities of Daily Living Functional independence is conceptualized as the ability to perform activities of daily living (ADLs) with maximal autonomy. Often, it is assumed that persons with dementing illness are functionally dependent; however, research indicates that cognitive impairment is correlated with—but does not clearly predict—functional dependence (Gelinas and Auer 1996). In fact,
Nursing Care
383
much functional dependence results from caregivers providing care in a manner that encourages dependence instead of assessing the functional capability of cognitively impaired persons and determining what is actually needed. Caregivers reinforce dependence because they lack the knowledge and skills to promote independence. Also, they believe that 1) total care takes less time; 2) dependency makes residents more treatable or manageable; 3) expecting these persons to become independent is unrealistic; 4) the job requires provision of total care; and 5) routine care is easier to provide than individualized care (Beck et al. 1997). Interventions tested to increase ability to perform ADLs fall into two general categories: stimulation and providing assistance that encourages residents to do all that they can.
Overall ADLs Three groups of investigators have attempted in a systematic way to improve functional ability in overall ADLs in nursing home residents with dementia. Reichenbach and Kirchman (1991) undertook an unblinded trial in which 67 residents were randomized to treatment or control groups. The multistrategy program consisted of reality orientation, remotivation, sensory stimulation and integration, reminiscence, and physical exercise used with the theme of esteem building. Treatment for 1 hour five mornings a week over 4 months significantly improved morale, mental functioning, and overall ADL ability compared with control subjects. Tappen (1994) conducted a randomized, single-blind trial with 93 nursing home residents assigned to a skills training group, a stimulation group, or usual care. In skills training, residents practiced bathing, dressing, eating, grooming, standing, toileting, and walking with the help of verbal prompting, physical demonstration, positive reinforcement, and assistance graded to ability. Stimulation group members participated in adult games, group discussions, listening to music, and relaxation. Treatments were administered in groups for 2.5 hours five days a week for 20 weeks. Skills training resulted in a large decrease in assistance; stimulation resulted in a smaller decrease in assistance; usual care resulted in an increase in assistance. The postintervention difference on ADL ability between the skills training group and the usual care group was statistically significant (P=0.04). The stimulation group did not differ significantly from the other two groups. Rogers et al. (1999) contrasted two treatment groups with a control group during morning care (bathing, toileting, dressing, oral hygiene, and grooming) in a convenience sample of 84 residents. Skills elicitation involved a research rehabilitative therapist (RT) assessing ADL ability and individualizing intervention (graded assistance and occupational therapy
384
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
techniques) for 5 days. Habit training involved the same RT (who gradually reduced assistance as ADL ability improved) and lasted 15 days. The control group received usual care from nursing home staff for 5 days. Mean duration of morning care increased from approximately 11 minutes during usual care to 21 minutes during skills elicitation and 20 minutes during habit training. Residents in the treatment group displayed significantly increased participation in self-care. These studies suggest that persons with severe cognitive impairment may have remediable excess disability (functional impairment greater than actual incapacity for self-care [Brody et al. 1971]). However, they may lack the capacity to learn new skills.
Bathing Most of the research on bathing has dealt with reducing problem behaviors (see “Intervening in Dysfunctional Behaviors” later in this chapter). McEvoy and Patterson (1986) taught five training modules, one of which addressed bathing and oral hygiene, to 15 demented and 15 nondemented subjects. Verbal instructions, modeling, and practicing skills increased bathing ability, as evidenced by improvement in rating of appearance (P<0.05). Staff members used only the level of prompting needed and tried to decrease prompts as skills increased. The bathing module lasted about 20 weeks. Most cognitively impaired subjects needed longer periods of training (15.4 versus 7.4 weeks) to master the bathing and hygiene skills but eventually were able to do so.
Dressing Beck et al. (1997) conducted a 6-week trial of an intervention in a convenience sample of 90 nursing home residents with dementia. Certified nursing assistants (CNAs) received training (attending lectures, observing examples from a videotape, and role playing) to provide strategies to promote independence in dressing in eight 30-minute sessions. The research team gave feedback on implementation of strategies and advice about how and when to change strategies. There was significantly improved dressing independence from baseline performance. The interventions required only an average of 53 extra seconds of time, a statistically but not clinically significant difference. CNAs learned to do other tasks while waiting for the resident to complete dressing steps. The studies on overall ADLs, bathing, and dressing demonstrate that nursing assistants can learn and implement strategies that foster self-care and that persons with dementia can maintain and even improve their func-
Nursing Care
385
tioning. These improvements are stronger when nursing assistants are helped to individualize the interventions to specific residents’ abilities and disabilities.
Feeding In a cross-sectional study of geriatric outpatients, women were five times more likely to have a low body mass index (BMI) than were men, and patients with more impaired cognition were twice as likely to have a low BMI than patients with moderate to high cognition. This suggests that women with poor cognition should be targeted for evaluation of nutritional status (Bedard et al. 2000). Gillette-Guyonnet et al. (2000) found in a longitudinal study that community-dwelling Alzheimer’s disease patients living with a highly distressed caregiver might experience greater weight loss. Nursing home residents with dementia—particularly those who pace— consume more energy and protein per kilogram of body weight than those without dementia (Rheaume et al. 1987). They tend to weigh less and lose more weight than cognitively intact community-dwelling persons (Spindler et al. 1996) and nursing home residents (Wang et al. 1997). Furthermore, Keller (1993) found that 18% of nursing home residents with dementia had severe undernutrition and that 27.5% had mild to moderate undernutrition. Similarly, Holben et al. (1999) found signs of dehydration in 56 of 212 nursing home residents with dementia, and Spangler et al. (1998) reported that nursing home residents with dementia needed more fluids (roughly 350 mL, 65–85 years; 600 mL, 86–100 years). In another study of nursing home residents with dementia, most (88%) required some help with feeding, and almost 50% required total feeding. Furthermore, the ones who needed assistance had the highest intake, whereas the ones who fed themselves had the lowest intake (Steele et al. 1997). However, Porter et al. (1999) found that greater cognitive impairment was associated with lower food intake. They also reported that 22 of 38 residents received more than 30% of their protein from liquid supplements. In a 4-year observational study of mealtimes at nursing homes, Kayser-Jones et al. (1997a, 1997b, 1997c) found that staff members were mixing all foods together with milk and forcing them into the resident’s mouth with a large spoon; scolding the resident for not cooperating with them; feeding in a hurried, chaotic environment; skipping residents who remained in their rooms or who had eaten a large amount at the previous meal; and inaccurately recording intake. Problems intensified at the evening meal when fewer CNAs were available to feed the residents. Nursing home staffs may be inclined to use tube feedings to deal with feeding problems, but this method has failed to improve survival or pres-
386
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
sure sore outcomes, reduce risk of infections, decrease aspiration pneumonia, or increase resident comfort. Actually, both the caregiver and the resident have an increased burden with tube feedings. A dedicated attempt at feeding by hand is the safest and nutritionally superior approach (Finucane and Bynum 1996; Finucane et al. 1999; Mitchell et al. 1997). One observational study found that when CNAs allowed residents with dementia to select what, where, and how long they wanted to eat, residents consumed more fluid and food (Amella 1999). Osborne and Marshall (1993) compared self-feeding behavior of 23 subjects with Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) scores below 20 under usual and researcher-assisted conditions. They tested five levels of assistance: unassisted, verbal prompt, nonverbal prompt, physical guidance, and total assistance. When subjects were first observed under usual nursing home conditions, the investigators discovered that staff members provided either no assistance or total assistance. The researchers then used the levels of assistance, trying each three times before moving to the next level. All levels of assistance induced self-feeding behaviors, with subjects demonstrating significantly more self-feeding beyond their baseline performance. Lange-Alberts and Shott (1994) examined the effects of touch and verbal cueing on food intake during 5 days in 17 nursing home residents with dementia. The residents were divided into three groups according to caloric intake and were randomized into treatment groups. One group received touch and verbal cueing (intermittent forearm touch and verbal cueing during lunch). A second group received only touch, whereas the third group received only verbal cueing for 5 days. All groups had significantly increased mean caloric and protein intake. Coyne and Hoskins (1997) conducted a randomized, controlled trial with 24 nursing home residents with dementia. Research assistants gave standardized directed verbal prompts and positive reinforcement 1 minute after trays arrived and at 1-minute intervals until they had given six sets of prompts. Residents who received the intervention were significantly more independent in eating both solid and liquid food than were the control group and retained this independence through follow-up 10 days after treatment. The same levels of assistance strategies used in overall ADLs, bathing, and dressing also improve eating for persons with dementia. These strategies in eating not only promote independence and self-esteem, as they do with other ADLs, but they also promote better nutritional status.
Physical Activity Several recent reports have addressed the benefits of exercise in older adults in general (Christmas and Andersen 2000; National Blueprint on
Nursing Care
387
Physical Activity 2001; Surgeon General’s Report 1996). Physical activity and exercise may ameliorate disease and delay decline in function in elders (Christmas and Andersen 2000). Unfortunately, by age 75, about one in three men and one in two women engage in no physical activity (Surgeon General’s Report 1996). Teri et al. (1998) found that 30 community-dwelling individuals with Alzheimer’s disease had lower baseline measures of physical performance and function than was found in published data on nondemented older adults. However, these individuals were able to adhere to a 12-week exercise program directed by their caregivers, which suggests that physical training can be implemented in persons with dementia. Friedman and Tappen (1991) conducted a randomized, nonblinded two-group experimental study of a planned walking program on communication performance in 30 nursing home residents with moderate to severe Alzheimer’s disease. One group of subjects (7 from one nursing home; 8 from another) walked and conversed with the investigator for 30 minutes three times a week for 10 weeks. A second group (8 from one nursing home and 7 from another) conversed only with the investigator during the same amount of time. Post-study testing showed that the planned-walking group had significantly greater improvement in communication than the conversation-only group. Binder (1995) launched an exercise program with 25 nursing home residents who had moderately severe cognitive impairment and were randomized to receive or not receive vitamin D supplementation. The program involved a 50- to-60-minute group activity three times a week for 8 weeks with a variety of exercises to increase skeletal muscle strength, flexibility, and speed. When individual residents were ready, the difficulty of the exercises increased. No exercise-related falls occurred. Participants who received the vitamin D supplement had a significant increase in serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D levels, but there were no significant differences in performance between those who did and those who did not receive vitamin D. The total group demonstrated significant improvement in muscle strength and balance. Binder noted that compared with cognitively intact persons, participants required more supervision to perform the exercises and spent more time resting or observing. They needed constant verbal and visual cueing during exercise sessions. Binder recommended a staff-to-participant ratio of at least 1:4 and exercise once or twice a day. Koroknay et al. (1995) implemented a program of walking as a regular daily activity for 15 nursing home residents with cognitive impairment and 5 without cognitive impairment. Staff members received in-service training on the program. Environmental modifications were made, such as posting signs (“Keep on Going”) and putting up a bulletin board containing information on participants. There was a significant improvement in
388
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
ambulatory status, and the proportion of participants who fell decreased from 25% before the program to 5% after the program, which lasted a mean time of 5.4 months (range, 2 to 9 months). Using a delayed-intervention design, MacRae et al. (1996) tested a walking program in which 19 nursing home residents (average MMSE score, 18) had the opportunity to walk at their own pace 5 days a week for 12 weeks with a research assistant. A control group (n=12) received weekly social visits from a research assistant before starting the walking program. The walking group increased walking time by 77% and walking distance by 92%. An extension of the walking program to 22 weeks showed no improvement over the 12-week program. Schnelle et al. (1996) conducted a randomized, controlled trial of an intervention to improve mobility in physically restrained nursing home residents, whose mean MMSE score was 10, and 52% of whom were nonambulatory. Those in the intervention group (n = 35) received the 9-week protocol, which involved three major components: mobility exercise, safety practice, and rowing endurance and strength exercise. The control group (n=37) received usual care. There was a significant positive group-by-time interaction for wheelchair endurance, rowing endurance, rowing range of motion, and left handgrip strength, and a significant reduction in injury risk factors. In 15 institutionalized men with mild to moderate dementia (MMSE score, 18–21), Palleschi et al. (1996) tested the effects of an aerobic exercise program using a cycloergometer, an exercise machine, for 20 minutes 3 days a week for 3 months. Results showed significant improvement in cognitive function based on a battery of tests (test of attention, verbal span test, supraverbal span test, and MMSE). In a study of rocking as a therapeutic activity, 25 nursing home residents with dementia received two treatments in randomized order (Watson et al. 1998). Subjects received 6 weeks of therapy in rocking chairs in either a nonrocking mode or a rocking mode followed by a 2-week washout period in which they returned to their usual nonrocking chairs, and then 6 weeks of the opposite therapy. The treatment was instituted for an average of 101 minutes on weekdays. Subjects who rocked at least 80 minutes showed significant reductions in depression and anxiety and in as-needed doses of pain medications. A subgroup of 9 subjects who enjoyed rocking showed improved balance. Arkin (1999) offered independent study credit to students who served as rehabilitation partners and fitness supervisors to 14 noninstitutionalized persons with very mild to moderate dementia. The protocol involved stretching, aerobic exercise (treadmill and stationary bicycle), and weight training. The distance walked and the weights lifted increased significantly from levels before participation. Follow-up is in progress.
Nursing Care
389
Lazowski et al. (1999) conducted a trial with 68 residents (25% with dementia) in five nursing homes randomized to two treatments, range of motion (ROM) exercises (what institutions routinely use for their exercise programs), and a Functional Fitness for Long-Term Care (FFLTC) program. The aim was to maintain ROM and improve strength, balance, flexibility, mobility, and function. Classes were conducted in groups of 4–10 residents by trained facility staff members for 45 minutes three times a week for 4 months. The FFLTC group showed significant improvements in functional capacity and strength, whereas the ROM group showed no change. The authors found that exercises for cognitively impaired persons needed to be highly structured and repetitive. These individuals were enabled to follow exercises through constant cueing and through mimicking the movements of others. Tappen et al. (2000) randomly assigned 65 Alzheimer’s disease patients (average MMSE score, 11) to three groups: assisted walking, conversation, and walking with conversation. Treatment took place for 30 minutes three times a week for 16 weeks. Subjects in the walking-with-conversation treatment group had the smallest decline (2.5%) in functional mobility compared with a 21% decline in the walking program and a 19% decline in the conversation group. The studies described above suggest that physical activity has benefits for cognitively impaired persons. It can improve mood and cognition and prevent falls by increasing functional mobility, balance, and strength.
Falls The average 100-bed nursing home reports 100–200 falls per year. Of these, 11.8% result in serious injuries, and 3.8% result in fractures. In addition, injuries from falls are a leading cause of legal action against nursing homes (Rubenstein 1997; Rubenstein et al. 1996). In one study, falls resulted from weakness and impaired gait or balance (26%); dizziness (25%); environmental factors (16%); medication, pain, acute illness, arthritis, or epilepsy (12%); and confusion (10%) (Rubenstein et al. 1996). Exercise combined with behavioral interventions that address risk factors (including environmental hazards) is helpful in reducing falls (Gillespie et al. 2000). Hornbrook et al. (1994) conducted a randomized, controlled trial of a fall prevention program with 3,182 independently living health maintenance organization members. There were four weekly 90-minute meetings, with 10–25 participants per group. All subjects received home assessment of falls and safety hazards. The treatment group also received home repair information and encouragement to make changes on any
390
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
identified risks. Treatment included a health behavior intervention; a presentation about fall hazards (environment, behaviors that cause risk, and ways to assess and decrease risks). There were 20 minutes of exercise training, written descriptions, and encouragement to use exercises at home, report of successful fall prevention measures from the previous week, and plans for the upcoming week. At 23-month follow up, the intervention group had significantly fewer falls than did the control group. Men were significantly less likely than women to fall or to have a fall resulting in injury and requiring medical care or a fracture requiring hospitalization. Persons older than age 65 years were significantly more likely to fall and to have fall-related injuries. Persons with one or more falls in the 6 weeks before the study were significantly more likely to fall and to have fall-related injuries. The risk of falling and fall-related injuries increased in relation to the number of chronic medical conditions at baseline. In a randomized, controlled trial, Ray et al. (1997) conducted a comprehensive assessment of 482 residents from 14 nursing homes. A multidisciplinary team recommended changes in environmental and personal safety, wheelchair use, psychotropic medications, transferring, and ambulation in three 45-minute in-service training sessions for staff. Intervention facilities implemented 63% of the recommendations. Compared with control facilities, intervention facilities had 19% fewer recurrent fallers. Close et al. (1999) conducted a randomized, controlled trial to prevent additional falls in 397 community-dwelling elders who had received hospital treatment for a fall. The intervention consisted of a medical assessment of mood, balance, cognition, medications prescribed, and vision one time during their hospital stay, and occupational therapy assessment for functional independence and environmental hazards. Seventy-two percent of falls had an identifiable cause (e.g., ice on the sidewalk, cardiovascular or circulation disorders). Assessment resulted in referrals to outpatient care, day hospital, general practitioner (half of the time for medication management), or optician for 84% of subjects. At 12-month follow-up, significantly fewer falls occurred in the intervention group than in the control group. After controlling for functional ability and previous falls, the risk of falling once or more was significantly lower in the intervention group than in the control group. Assessment and alteration of medication regimens (usually by a consulting pharmacist) have decreased falls and saved injury-related costs by preventing falls. Cooper (1997a) prospectively studied falls in 98 nursing home residents. At monthly drug review, pharmacists recommended alternatives to medications that were likely to cause falls in these residents. Pharmacists recommended that 57 patients switch from benzodiazepine or other psychotropic drugs to buspirone. Of the 27 persons who switched, 24 did not
Nursing Care
391
need to restart their former medication or begin treatment with another psychotropic drug. For the acceptance group, falls decreased from 50 in the 6 months before the medication change to 8 falls in the 6 months after the change. Pharmacists also suggested other drug-related changes, such as switching from metoclopramide to cisapride, that 23 persons accepted. For the all acceptance groups combined, falls declined from 91 before the changes to 13 afterward, an 86% decrease. Cooper (1997b) also retrospectively analyzed fall injuries and estimated the actual cost of injuries attributable to use of psychoactive medication. For 83 patients, there were 257 falls. The estimated cost for a fall-related injury from psychotropic medication use was $858, whereas fall-related injuries from other psychoactive medication use averaged $427, for a total savings of $58,812. The average cost of hospitalization for treatment of fracture, pneumonia, or pressure sore developing after a fall was $12,000. Physical activity combined with environmental alteration can reduce the incidence of falls in community dwellers and nursing home residents with dementia. In addition, assessment of and changes in psychotropic medications have reduced falls and saved medical expenses for nursing home residents with dementia.
Incontinence Urinary Incontinence Urinary incontinence affects 13 million Americans (more than 50% of elders) and costs an estimated $16.4 billion annually ($5.2 billion in longterm care), with $1.1 billion spent on absorbent products, according to the Agency for Healthcare Quality and Research’s Clinical Practice Guideline on Urinary Incontinence in Adults: Acute and Chronic Management, which offers recommendations for management of urinary incontinence in persons who are frail, live in nursing homes, or both. Recommendations reflect the evidence in the current scientific literature and, in some cases, the professional opinion of the guideline developers. Many nursing homes have applied this guideline and achieved positive outcomes. For example, a Tennessee nursing home decreased the number of incontinent residents by 65% in 14 months of use, and a Connecticut nursing home improved continence status in 50% of the residents who had participated in a prompted voiding program for 6 months (Urinary Incontinence Guidelines 1996). The guideline forms the basis of the discussion that follows. The use of devices, absorbent pads or diapers, or both, is the most common way to manage incontinence in nursing homes (Ouslander and Schnelle 1995). These products are most appropriate for persons whose
392
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
incontinence cannot be managed any other way, but they should be used with caution because they may increase dependence. Toileting strategies appear to be effective in managing urinary incontinence in nursing home residents with and without cognitive impairment. These strategies include prompted voiding. This technique involves asking about the need to toilet on a regular schedule, usually every 2 hours, and toileting only when the resident answers affirmatively (Eustice et al. 2000). Regularly scheduled toileting is also useful at a frequency of every 2 hours or some other pattern based on the individual resident. These techniques tend to be most effective during waking hours and with residents who can walk unassisted (Fantl et al. 1996). For incontinence during sleep, staff members usually apply absorbent products, which they check for wetness at intervals throughout the night and change as needed. However, this practice tends to disrupt sleep. In a review of the research on toileting in nursing home residents with dementia and incontinence, Skelly and Flint (1995) found that scheduled toileting decreased episodes of incontinence by up to 26%, whereas prompted voiding decreased incontinence by 22%–50%. Schnelle et al. (1995) estimated the cost of prompted voiding at $4.31 for 12 hours during the day for each resident. Environmental changes also help decrease incontinence (Skelly and Flint 1995). Environmental changes include increased visibility of and access to toilets, appropriate toilet seat height and placement of grab bars, availability of mobility aids, use of portable commodes or urinals with residents who have impaired mobility, clothing that allows for easy disrobing, and avoidance of physical restraints. In addition, improving mobility has decreased incontinence (Skelly and Flint 1995). A daily exercise program (walking) significantly decreased the incidence of incontinence in 15 nursing home residents with dementia between 7 A.M. and 3 P.M. (Jirovec 1991). Residents walked with assistance by a person or a device (with rest stops as needed) for 30 minutes a day, 5 days a week for a month. The average number of incontinent episodes decreased between 7 A.M. and 3 P.M. from 2.33 times to 1 time per day. Schnelle et al. (1998a) compared usual versus intervention care in 92 residents at four nursing homes. The intervention included assignment of residents to 2- or 4- hour wet checks based on skin condition and to decreased noise and light. It was unclear how long the treatment phase lasted; both usual care and intervention data collection lasted 5 days. The intervention resulted in decreased waking due to sound or light. Residents with 4-hour wet checks had decreased numbers of changes, increased amounts of urine per pad, and longer amounts of time exposed to urine before changing.
Nursing Care
393
Fecal Incontinence Approximately 30% of nursing home residents experience fecal incontinence (Chassagne et al. 1999). Of residents with urinary incontinence, 50%–75% also have episodes of fecal incontinence (Ouslander and Schnelle 1995). Ouslander and Schnelle suggested the use of bowel-training programs (use of stool softeners or fiber, regular toileting after breakfast, and administration of oral laxative or suppository three to four times a week to stimulate a bowel movement) for residents with severe cognitive impairment. Ouslander et al. (1996) conducted a prospective, uncontrolled 9- to 10-week trial of prompted voiding to decrease urinary and fecal incontinence in 165 nursing home residents. The intervention consisted of focusing residents on wet-dry status, checking them, prompting them to use a toilet, toileting residents if they requested it, providing positive reinforcement for dryness and appropriate toileting, and offering fluids to promote bladder filling for the next prompting episode. There was a nonsignificant decrease in the number of incontinent bowel movements and a significant increase in continent bowel movements. Residents who responded well to prompted voiding for their urinary incontinence also responded well for their fecal incontinence. Prompted voiding appears to be more effective than scheduled toileting in reducing urinary incontinence, and it also helps to decrease fecal incontinence. Simple measures such as regular walking and using clothing that allows for easy disrobing also decrease episodes of both urinary and fecal incontinence.
Sleep Nighttime sleep problems are among the most common reasons that persons with dementia are admitted to nursing homes by their families. Once there, they continue to experience fragmented sleep. They spend as much as 35%–40% of the day in bed and about 20%–23% of the day asleep. At night, they have trouble falling asleep, awaken frequently, and rise prematurely (Cohen-Mansfield et al. 1995; Schnelle et al. 1998b). In a 3-month study, only 2 of 16 residents with dementia slept an entire night (CohenMansfield et al. 1995). Okawa et al. (1991) found that 59% of nursing home residents with dementia had abnormal sleep-wake cycles (nighttime awakenings and daytime sleeping) compared with 12.5% of residents with little or no cognitive impairment. Major factors that significantly affect sleep include limited bright light exposure during the day (Ancoli-Israel et
394
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
al. 1997) and sleep apnea (Bliwise et al. 1990). Environmental, behavioral, and pharmacological interventions can improve sleep in elders with dementia. The studies described below included subjects with and without dementia Lyketsos et al. (1999) undertook a randomized, controlled, crossover clinical trial in patients with dementia employing two conditions: bright light and control. They had complete data on 8 patients and partial data on 15 patients. Bright light therapy was employed hourly every morning using a 10,000-lux full-spectrum lamp 3 feet from the subjects. The subjects were instructed every 15 minutes to keep their eyes open and in the direction of the light source. Treatment lasted 4 weeks. Treatment subjects slept an average of 6.4 hours/night before treatment. After 2 weeks, they slept an average of 7.6 hours/night. After 4 weeks, they slept an average of 8.1 hours, a significant improvement. Alessi et al. (1999) conducted a randomized clinical trial on 29 nursing home residents. Subjects were randomized to a combination of increased daytime physical activity with a nighttime program to decrease noise and sleep-disruptive practices or the nighttime program alone. Treatment 1 consisted of 14 weeks of daytime activity and 5 days of decreased noise. Treatment 2 involved 5 days of decreased noise. Sleep percentage was reported as the “total amount of time asleep divided by the total Actigraph recording time at night” (Alessi et al. 1999, p. 786). Sleep increased significantly in treatment group 1 (from 51.7% to 62.5% at follow-up) but not in group 2. Treatment group 1 participants averaged 32% less time in bed, significantly more than did group 2. Schnelle et al. (1999) used a randomized control group design with a delayed intervention for the control group in 184 nursing home residents. In-service education included information about noise and sleep. Researchers demonstrated the noise level in the nursing home to the staff. Follow-up sessions occurred nightly. Noise abatement involved closing doors, turning down televisions and radios, turning off unwatched television sets, not using intercoms, and speaking in low voices at night. Research staff provided hourly incontinence care when residents were awake. Residents at low risk for skin problems were allowed to sleep for 4 hours, and those at high risk for skin problems were allowed to sleep for 2 hours before staff checked on them. Noise and light decreased significantly after intervention. Awakenings from light alone and with noise decreased significantly, but light and noise reduction did not significantly change percentage sleep, peak sleep, or sleep duration. Bright light therapy during the day, a walking program, and reduced light and noise at night proved effective in ameliorating sleep problems in cognitively impaired nursing home residents.
Nursing Care
395
Sensory Deficits Sensory deficits may exaggerate functional deficits that accompany cognitive impairment and thereby increase disability (Mendez et al. 1990). Normative aging changes in vision (e.g., decline in visual acuity, accommodation, and low light–level functioning) affect 40% of individuals age 65 and older (Christen and Mangione 1994). In addition, specific age-associated eye diseases (e.g., cataracts, macular degeneration, glaucoma) may occur. Although there is no evidence of a difference in prevalence of these disorders in patients with dementing illnesses, patients may stop receiving regular ophthalmologic examinations or using prescribed glasses; thus, these visual disorders may remain undetected and untreated. Additionally, caregivers may forget, misplace, or minimize wearing eyeglasses as their loved ones lose everyday competencies. Furthermore, up to 60% of Alzheimer’s disease patients have both pervasive and specific visual impairments associated with Alzheimer’s disease. These impairments may be related to changes in the primary visual and visual association cortices, but they appear to be unrelated to the severity of Alzheimer’s disease or eye pathology per se (Cronin-Golomb et al. 1991). Cognitively impaired individuals with hearing loss along with visual limitations are particularly likely to misunderstand their environment and to react inappropriately.
Intervening in Dysfunctional Behaviors Disruptive behaviors change over the course of dementing illness. Mildly impaired persons may become apathetic or irritable. The majority of disruptive behaviors, such as wandering, agitation, aggression, disruptive vocalizations, and delusions and hallucinations occur with loss of competence in ADLs. Late in dementing illnesses, individuals are debilitated and less active, and consequently disruptive behaviors recede (Davis et al. 1997). As indicated earlier, we base interventions on research, when available, best practice guidelines, or empirically based published recommendations. The Cochrane Library contains several reviews of interventions that address behavioral problems in dementing illness. For example, the value of reality orientation for patients with dementia has been debated in clinical circles. In a recent Cochrane review of six randomized, controlled trials involving 125 subjects over 4–21 weeks, Spector et al. (2000a) concluded that classroom and 24-hour reality orientation has beneficial effects on cognitive and behavioral outcomes and apparently caused no harm when used to address behavior problems in dementia. They also caution that it is unclear how long the benefits continue after the intervention ends.
396
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Spector et al. (2000b) conducted a systematic review of studies that used reminiscence therapy in dementia. They located two randomized clinical trials, one of which met criteria for inclusion in the Cochrane Review. The results were insignificant, although there was a trend favoring reminiscence in behavioral outcome. The authors concluded that reminiscence therapy can have some beneficial effects and apparently causes no harm when used to address behavior problems in dementia, but that further research is needed.
Agitation and Aggression Agitation can be defined as “inappropriate verbal, vocal, or motor activity that is not explained by needs or confusion of the individual per se” (CohenMansfield 1986). Although agitation probably results frequently from a combination of needs and confusion, these antecedent conditions are not always apparent (Cohen-Mansfield and Billig 1986). Agitation, which is common in dementing illness, is often not benign. Bair et al. (1999) found that agitation was accompanied by a twofold increase in mortality rate, a more than doubled nursing home placement rate, and a 42% increase in nursing home staff stress level. All of the following strategies (each tested in a separate study) showed promise in managing aggressive, agitated, or disruptive behavior: planned walking program, bright light therapy, and restraints (used at times to control aggressive behaviors). Camberg et al. (1999) reported on the experimental use of Simulated Presence (SimPres) in 54 nursing home residents with dementia. SimPres consisted of playing through a headset a personalized audiotape of a family member’s telephone conversation about cherished memories. This was done when residents became agitated or withdrawn. SimPres was compared with a tape of newspaper reading and with usual care. Each condition lasted 17 days with a 5-day washout. Trained observers recorded seven problem behaviors using time sampling. Staff members, blinded to assignment of individuals to SimPres or newspaper-based tapes, recorded observations on several standardized scales. Although the observers detected no changes in problem behavior, SimPres was more effective than the other two conditions as assessed by daily staff observation logs of agitation, the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (Cohen-Mansfield 1986), and the Mood and Interest scales of the Multidimensional Observation Scale for Elderly Subjects (Helmes et al. 1987). Music reduces problem behaviors (verbal and physical agitation, aggression, and resistance) while nursing home residents receive care (bathing and eating). Although classical music has some ability to decrease these
Nursing Care
397
behaviors, the resident’s favorite music has an effect that starts earlier and lasts longer. One-to-one social interaction and watching a videotape of familiar persons decreased verbally disruptive behaviors more than music did. Clark et al. (1998) studied the use of favorite music during bathing in 18 residents of one nursing home. Subjects were randomized to treatment or control conditions for 10 observations and then the conditions were reversed for 10 observations. The research assistants observed 18 behaviors and found significant decrease in total aggression and hitting. Walking or other light exercise appears to decrease problem behaviors, as does bright light therapy used 2 hours or less daily. Studies of the impact of touch (most often massage) on problem behaviors have yielded contradictory results.
Wandering Cognitively impaired persons may also wander within a facility or may elope. Patients may wander in search of meeting an unarticulated need, for physical exercise, out of boredom, because of hunger or pain, or in response to delusions or hallucinations. The major nursing concerns are that individuals who wander may injure themselves by going into unsafe areas, may starve or die exposure if they leave the institution, or may provoke aggressive behaviors from others (Holmberg 1997). Price et al. (2000) reported results of a systematic review of 38 studies addressing barrier interventions for wandering in dementing illness, and they noted the absence of controlled trials. The majority of the studies were not experimental, and the remaining 8 experimental studies were judged unsatisfactory for a variety of reasons, including performance bias, failure to classify patients according to severity of dementia, and limited measurement of outcomes. Algase (1999), in a thorough review of the research literature on wandering, concluded that numerous studies are descriptive in nature and that the limited intervention studies are weak, consisting of single case studies, few if any control subjects, and poorly defined and operationalized outcome measures. Holmberg (1997) carried out a walking intervention program consisting of 90 minutes of walking, including rest stops, initiated immediately after the evening meal. These sessions, for 11 physically active persons with severe dementia, were supervised by community group leaders and occurred for three consecutive evenings weekly. When frequency of residentto-resident and resident-to-staff aggression was compared for 24 hours after walking with times in which walking did not occur, there were 30% fewer aggressive incidents in the 24-hour periods after the walking sessions.
398
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Disruptive Vocalizations Despite its tremendous impact on those in the environment, disruptive vocalization is the focus of few published studies. In a longitudinal cohort study, Sloane et al. (1999) collected data on 203 subjects residing in 107 long-term-care facilities who met criteria for severe disruptive vocalization. Of these individuals, 62% could be heard more than 100 feet away, and the average person in the study vocalized 6 hours each day. These individuals with disruptive vocalization were grossly impaired in cognition (98%), locomotion (68%), and eating (49%). Activities that were associated with the onset of disruptive vocalization included being alone, pain, noise from someone else, presence of more than one person in the resident’s room, and staff members trying to assist with ADLs. The study documented the difficulty of identifying useful interventions. Staff members reported that helpful interventions included interpersonal contact; attending to physical comfort; activities; and administration of antipsychotic, analgesic, and anxiolytic medications; but these same interventions were also cited as making things worse. In the absence of conclusive research, a group met in Chapel Hill, North Carolina, in 1996 to develop guidelines and recommendations for the care of disruptive vocalization (Sloane et al. 1997). The suggestions included starting with a multidisciplinary assessment of the person with disruptive vocalization—including a description of the vocalization; the setting, antecedents, and consequences of vocalization; inquiry into past events; and assessment of relevant health problems. The guidelines suggest considering that disruptive vocalization probably reflects an unmet need or a response to physical or environmental stimuli. They also suggest that persons with less cognitive impairment are more likely to respond to behavioral interventions, whereas persons with severe dementia may do better with comfort and environmental measures. The final suggestion was trial of a variety of behavioral and environmental approaches and the development and dissemination of a plan of care based on these trials for use among all staff members.
Delusions and Hallucinations There have been few studies of behavioral or environmental interventions specifically targeted for delusions and hallucinations in dementia. The care guidelines developed at Chapel Hill acknowledge that delusions and hallucinations may be a cause of disruptive vocalizations (Sloane et al. 1997). Pankow et al. (1996) note that many elders with visual hallucinations expe-
Nursing Care
399
rience sensory deprivation due to cognitive and visual impairments. It therefore may be useful to maximize hearing and visual acuity in persons with delusions and hallucinations. One should also assess the content of hallucinations and delusional communications because they may be related to delirium or psychotic depression, which are treatable conditions. For hallucinations or delusions that are troubling to nursing home residents or that lead to disruptive behaviors, a trial of regularly administered antipsychotic medication is indicated. A useful way for staff members to respond to delusions and hallucinations can be to recognize the feeling tone (such as concern for safety) and to avoid trying to prove to residents that their perceptions and beliefs are imaginary or groundless. This is part of the technique of validation advocated by Feil (1992).
Use of Restraints Strong evidence from at least four studies involving a total of 3,005 nursing home residents indicates that staff education results in reduced use of restraints and that education combined with consultation on problem residents by a clinical nurse specialist or a multidisciplinary team produces even more dramatic reductions. Generally, the educational interventions included information about the hazards of restraint use, assessment of factors that often lead to the use of restraints (problem behaviors and falls), and alternatives to restraint use, many of which were behavioral interventions. They focused primarily on CNAs, but some involved nurses, administrators, and physicians. The length of education ranged from 5 hours to 2 days. The reduction in the use of physical restraints after education did not increase use of psychoactive drugs, serious injury from falls, or caregiving time. In fact, these educational programs frequently resulted in the use of fewer psychoactive drugs. Finally, residents who were physically restrained required more care that took longer to provide compared with unrestrained residents.
Summary In this chapter, we briefly discussed principles of communicating with impaired individuals, research-based interventions that have been used to promote ADLs, and means to deal with several disruptive behaviors. Although a growing body of research literature helps to inform nursing care, many areas are in need of beginning or additional research.
400
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
References Agency for Healthcare Quality and Research: Urinary Incontinence in Adults: Acute and Chronic Management. Clinical Practive Guideline No 2, 1996 update (AH-CPR Publ No 96-0682). Rockville, MD, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1996 Alessi CA, Yoon EJ, Schnelle JF, et al: A randomized trial of a combined physical activity and environmental intervention in nursing home residents: do sleep and agitation improve? J Am Geriatr Soc 47:784–791, 1999 Algase DL: Wandering in dementia. Annu Rev Nurs Res 17:185–217, 1999 Amella EJ: Factors influencing the proportion of food consumed by nursing home residents with dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 47:879–885, 1999 Ancoli-Israel S, Klauber MR, Jones DW, et al: Variations in circadian rhythms of activity, sleep, and light exposure related to dementia in nursing-home patients. Sleep 20:18–23, 1997 Arkin SM: Elder rehab: a student-supervised exercise program for Alzheimer’s patients. Gerontologist 39:729–735, 1999 Bair B, Toth W, Johnson MA, et al: Interventions for disruptive behaviors use and success. J Gerontol Nurs 25:13–21, 1999 Bedard M, Molloy DW, Bell R, et al: Determinants and detection of low body mass index in community-dwelling adults with Alzheimer’s disease. Int Psychogeriatr 12:87–98, 2000 Beck C, Heacock P, Mercer SO, et al: Improving dressing behavior in cognitively impaired nursing home residents. Nurs Res 46:126–132, 1997 Binder EF: Implementing a structured exercise program for frail nursing home residents with dementia: issues and challenges. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity 3:383–395, 1995 Bliwise DL, Carroll JS, Dement WC: Predictors of observed sleep/wakefulness in residents in long-term care. J Gerontol 45:M126–M130, 1990 Brody EM, Kleban, MH, Lawton, MP, et al: Excess disabilities of mentally impaired aged: impact of individualized treatment. Gerontologist 11:124–130, 1971 Camberg L, Woods P, Ooi WL, et al: Evaluation of simulated presence: a personalized approach to enhance well-being in persons with Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 47:446–452, 1999 Chassagne P, Landrin I, Neveu C, et al: Fecal incontinence in the institutionalized elderly: incidence, risk factors, and prognosis. Am J Med 106:185–190, 1999 Christen WG, Mangione CM: Outcome assessment in ophthalmology: function, in Principles and Practice of Ophthalmology. Edited by Albert DM, Jakobiec FA, Robinson NL, Philadelphia, PA, WB Saunders, 1994, pp 1311–1321 Christmas C, Andersen RA: Exercise and older patients: guidelines for the clinician. J Am Geriatr Soc 48:318–324, 2000 Clark ME, Lipe AW, Bilbrey M: Use of music to decrease aggressive behaviors in people with dementia. J Gerontol Nurs 24:10–17, 1998 Close J, Ellis M, Hooper R, et al: Prevention of falls in the elderly trial (PROFET): a randomized controlled trial. Lancet 353:93–97, 1999
Nursing Care
401
Cohen-Mansfield J: Agitated behaviors in the elderly; II: preliminary results in the cognitively deteriorated. J Am Geriatr Soc 34:722–727, 1986 Cohen-Mansfield J, Billig N: Agitated behaviors in the elderly; I: a conceptual review. J Am Geriatr Soc 34:711–721, 1986 Cohen-Mansfield J, Werner P, Freedman L: Sleep and agitation in agitated nursing home residents: an observational study. Sleep 18:674–680, 1995 Cooper J: Consultant pharmacist assessment and reduction of fall risk in nursing facilities. Consultant Pharmacist 12:1294–1304, 1997a Cooper J: Consultant pharmacist assessment of psychoactive fall injury incidence and costs within nursing facilities. Consultant Pharmacist 12:1305–1309, 1997b Coyne ML, Hoskins L: Improving eating behaviors in dementia using behavioral strategies. Clin Nurs Res 6:275–290, 1997 Cronin-Golomb A, Corkin S, Rizzo JF, et al: Visual dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: relation to normal aging. Ann Neurol 29:41–52, 1991 Davis LL, Buckwalter K, Burgio LD: Measuring problem behaviors in dementia: developing a methodological agenda. Adv Nurs Sci 20:40–55, 1997 Eustice S, Roe B, Paterson J: Prompted voiding for the management of urinary incontinence in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2000 Feil N: Validation therapy. Geriatr Nurs 13:129–133, 1992 Finucane TE, Bynum JP: Use of tube feeding to prevent aspiration pneumonia. Lancet 348:1421–1424, 1996 Finucane TE, Christmas C, Travis K: Tube feeding in patients with advanced dementia: a review of the evidence. JAMA 282:1365–1370, 1999 Friedman R, Tappen RM: The effect of planned walking on communication in Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 39:650–654, 1991 Gelinas I, Auer S: Functional autonomy, in Clinical Diagnosis and Management of Alzheimer’s Disease. Edited by Gauthier S. London, Martin Bunitz, 1996, pp 213–226 Gillespie LD, Gillespie WJ, Cumming R, et al: Interventions for preventing falls in the elderly. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2000 Gillette-Guyonnet S, Nourhashemi F, et al: Weight loss in Alzheimer disease. Am J Clin Nutr 71(suppl):S637–S642, 2000 Gwyther LP: Care of Alzheimer’s Patients: A Manual for Nursing Home Staff. Chicago, IL, Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association, 1995 Helmes E, Csapo KG, Short JA: Standardization and validation of the Multidimensional Observational Scale for Elderly Subjects (MOSES). J Gerontol 42:395– 405, 1987 Holben DH, Hassell JT, Williams JL, et al: Fluid intake compared with established standards and symptoms of dehydration among elderly residents in a longterm-care facility. J Am Diet Assoc 99:1447–1450, 1999 Holmberg SK: Evaluation of a clinical intervention for wanderers on a geriatric nursing unit. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 11:21–28, 1997 Hornbrook MC, Stevens VJ, Wingfield DJ, et al: Preventing falls among community-dwelling older persons: results from a randomized trial. Gerontologist 44:16–23, 1994
402
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Jirovec MM: The impact of daily exercise on the mobility, balance and urine control of cognitively impaired nursing home residents. Int J Nurs Stud 28:145–151, 1991 Kayser-Jones J, Schell E: The effect of staffing on the quality of care at mealtime. Nurs Outlook 45:64–72, 1997a Kayser-Jones J, Schell E: The mealtime experience of a cognitively impaired elder: ineffective and effective strategies. J Gerontol Nurs 23:33–39, 1997b Kayser-Jones J Schell E: Staffing and the mealtime experience of nursing home residents on a Special Care Unit. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 12:67– 72, 1997c Keller HH: Malnutrition in institutional elderly: how and why? J Am Geriatr Soc 41:1212–1218, 1993 Koroknay VJ, Werner P, Cohen-Mansfield J, et al: Maintaining ambulation in the frail nursing home resident: a nursing administered walking program. J Gerontol Nurs 21:18–24, 1995 Lange-Alberts ME, Shott S: Nutritional intake: use of touch and verbal cueing. J Gerontol Nurs 20:36–40, 1994 Lazowski D, Ecclestone NA, Myers AM, et al: A randomized outcome evaluation of group exercise programs in long-term care institutions. J Gerontol 54: M621–M628, 1999 Lyketsos CG, Veiel LL, Baker A, et al: A randomized, controlled trial of bright light therapy for agitated behaviors in dementia patients residing in long-term care. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 14:520–525, 1999 MacRae PG, Asplund LA, Schnelle JF, et al: A walking program for nursing home residents: effects on walk endurance, physical activity, mobility, and quality of life. J Am Geriatr Soc 44:175–180, 1996 McEvoy CL, Patterson RL: Behavioral treatment of deficit skills in dementia patients. Gerontologist 26:475–478, 1986 Mendez MF, Tomsak RL, Remler B: Disorders of the visual system in Alzheimer’s disease. J Clin Neuroophthalmol 10:62–69, 1990 Mitchell SL, Kiely DK, Lipsitz L: The risk factors and impact on survival of feeding tube placement in nursing home residents with severe cognitive impairments. Arch Intern Med 157:327–332, 1997 National Blueprint on Physical Activity Among Adults Age 50 and Older. Princeton, NJ, Robert Wood Johnson Foundation, 2001 Okawa M, Mishima K, Hishikawa Y, et al: Circadian rhythm disorders in sleepwaking and body temperature in elderly patients with dementia and their treatment. Sleep 14:478–485, 1991 Osborne CL, Marshall MJ: Self-feeding performance in nursing home residents. J Gerontol Nurs 19:7–14, 1993 Ouslander JG, Schnelle JF: Incontinence in the nursing home. Ann Intern Med 122:438–449, 1995 Ouslander JG, Simmons S, Schnelle J, et al: Effects of prompted voiding on fecal incontinence among nursing home residents. J Am Geriatr Soc 44:424–428, 1996
Nursing Care
403
Palleschi L, Vetta F, De Gennaro E, et al: Effect of aerobic training on the cognitive performance of elderly patients with senile dementia of Alzheimer type. Arch Gerontol 5:47–50, 1996 Pankow L, Pliskin N, Luchins D: An optical intervention for visual hallucinations associated with visual impairment and dementia in elderly patients. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 8:88–92, 1996 Porter C, Schell ES, Kayser-Jones J, et al: Dynamics of nutrition care among nursing home residents who are eating poorly. J Am Diet Assoc 11:1444–1446, 1999 Price JD, Hermans DG, Grimley-Evans J: Subjective barriers to prevent wandering of cognitively impaired people. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2000 Ray WA, Taylor JA, Meador KG, et al: A randomized trial of a consultation service to reduce falls in nursing homes. JAMA 278:557–562, 1997 Reichenbach VR, Kirchman MM: Effect of a multi-strategy program upon elderly with organic brain syndrome. Phys Occup Ther Geriatr 9:131–152, 1991 Rheaume Y, Riley ME, Volicer L: Meeting nutritional needs of Alzheimer patients who pace constantly. J Nutr Elder 7:43–52, 1987 Rogers JC, Holm MB, Burgio LD, et al: Improving morning care routines of nursing home residents with dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 47:1049–1057, 1999 Rubenstein LZ: Preventing falls in nursing homes. JAMA 278:595–596, 1997 Rubenstein LZ, Josephson KR, Osterweil D: Falls and fall prevention in the nursing home. Clin Geriatr Med 12:881–902, 1996 Schnelle JF, Keeler E, Hays RD, et al: A cost and value analysis of two interventions with incontinent nursing home residents. J Am Geriatr Soc 43:1112–1117, 1995 Schnelle JF, MacRae PG, Giacobassi K, et al: Exercise with physically restrained nursing home residents: maximizing benefits of restraint reduction. J Am Geriatr Soc 44:507–512, 1996 Schnelle JF, Cruise PA, Alessi CA, et al: Individualizing nighttime incontinence care in nursing home residents. Nurs Res 47:197–204, 1998a Schnelle JF, Cruise PA, Alessi CA, et al: Sleep hygiene in physically dependent nursing home residents: behavioral and environmental intervention implications. Sleep 21:515–523, 1998b Schnelle JF, Alessi CA, Al-Samarrai NR, et al: The nursing home at night: effects of an intervention on noise, light and sleep. J Am Geriatr Soc 47:430–438, 1999 Skelly J, Flint AJ: Urinary incontinence associated with dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 43:286–294, 1995 Sloane PD, Davidson S, Buckwalter K, et al: Management of the patient with disruptive vocalization. Gerontologist 37:675–82, 1997 Sloane PD, Davidson S, Knight N, et al: Severe disruptive vocalizers. J Am Geriatr Soc 47:439–445, 1999 Spangler AA, Chidester JC: Age, dependency, and other factors influencing fluid intake by long term care residents. J Nutr Elder 18:21–35, 1998 Spector A, Orrell M, Davies S, et al: Reality orientation for dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2000a
404
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Spector A, Orrell M, Davies S, et al: Reminiscence therapy for dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2000b Spindler AA, Renval MJ, Nichols JF, et al: Nutritional status of patients with Alzheimer’s disease: a 1-year study. J Am Diet Assoc 96:1013–1018, 1996 Steele CM, Greenwood C, Ems I, et al: Mealtime difficulties in a home for the aged: not just dysphagia. Dysphagia 12:43–50, 1997 Surgeon General’s Report: Physical Activity and Health, Washington, DC, U.S. Government Printing Office, 1996 Tappen RM: The effect of skill training on functional abilities of nursing home residents with dementia. Res Nurs Health 17:159–165, 1994 Tappen RM, Roach KE, Applegate EB, et al: Effect of a combined walking and conversation intervention on functional mobility of nursing home residents with Alzheimer disease. Alz Dis Assoc Disord 14:196–201, 2000 Teri L, McCurry SM, Buchner DM, et al: Exercise and activity level in Alzheimer’s disease: a potential treatment focus. J Rehabil Res Dev 35:411–419, 1998 Urinary Incontinence Guideline: Real World Examples of Use. Rockville, MD, Agency for Health Care Policy and Research, 1996. Available at http:// www.ahcpr.gov/clinic/uistory.htm Wang SY, Fukagawa N, Hossain M, et al: Longitudinal weight changes, length of survival, and energy requirements of long-term care residents with dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 45:1189–1195, 1997 Watson NM, Wells TJ, Cox C: Rocking chair therapy for dementia patients: its effect on psychosocial well-being and balance. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 13:296–308, 1998 Zimmermann PG: Effective communication with patients with dementia. J Emerg Nurs 24:412–415, 1998
CHAPTER
13
Structuring Environments for Persons With Cognitive Impairment Paul K. Chafetz, Ph.D. Kevan H. Namazi, Ph.D.
The last few years have seen rapid development of both interest and options in the design of environments for cognitively impaired persons. The trend toward an increasingly differentiated continuum of care levels has progressed steadily. Assisted-living dementia care (ALDC) is now a well-established level of care, and many facilities are distinguishing between higher-functioning and lower-functioning ALDC units. Mirroring developments in the field has been the appearance of many excellent books and research reports. In this chapter we discuss recent developments relevant to care environments in several settings: patients’ homes, ALDC units, and nursing-level dementia care (NLDC) units. Distribution of Older Adults Across Living Settings Of all Americans age 65 and over, some 67% live in their own home or apartment. Of these, the portion with dementia is estimated to be between 5% (Regier et al. 1988) and 10% (Evans et al. 1989; Jorm et al. 1987). Approximately 15% of older Americans live in the home of a nonspouse relative (U.S. Bureau of the Census 1996); the estimated portion of these with dementia is 10%–12%. Some 10% of older Americans live in a diverse group of settings often referred to as regular retirement, where the portion with dementia may range from 5% to 20%, depending on the stringency of formal admission criteria and informal expectations for residents’ capacity for independent functioning. Rabins et al. (1996) reported a 10.5% 1-month prevalence of 405
406
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
cognitive disorders among residents of public housing for the elderly, a low-income retirement housing option. An estimated 4%–5% of older Americans live in assisted-living or board and care settings. Fitzgerald (1995) reported at least 1 million residents in assisted-living facilities alone. Of these, the portion with dementia is usually reported to be 30%–40% (Fisher 1996; Hawes et al. 1995; Hyde 1995). Davis et al. (2000) closely examined 56 residential care (e.g., assisted living) facilities in seven states and found that an average of 6.6% of such facilities provided specialized dementia programs. However, probably no more than 1%–2% of assisted-living residents in the United States are in a formal-assisted living dementia care setting. In contrast, up to 15% of residents in Australian hostels for the elderly are in dementia programs, according to Rosewarne et al. (1997). Some 5% of older Americans live in a nursing facility. This percentage varies widely with age. Nursing facilities care for 1.4% of adults ages 65– 74, 6% of adults ages 75–84, and 24% of adults age 85+ (U.S. Bureau of the Census 1996). At least 45% of nursing-facility residents have dementia. Lair and Lefkowitz (1990) found 42% in the 1987 National Medical Expenditure Survey. Magaziner et al. (2000) found that, of 2,285 adults age 65+ admitted to a stratified random sample of 59 Maryland nursing homes between 1992 and 1995, 48.2% had dementia. Davis et al. (2000) report earlier data from a national sample of nursing homes in 10 metropolitan areas, which found that 76% of nursing facility residents show moderate or worse cognitive impairment. Nevertheless, only 2%–4% of nursing facility residents are in a formal NLDC unit (Krauss et al. 1997; Phillips et al. 1997). The figures provided above illustrate an important dynamic. Most older adults (two-thirds) live independently, with smaller percentages occupying more intensive levels of care. One the other hand, a continuum of care and housing options is flourishing in America, providing care to 20% of older adults. As the number of older old Americans rises, the need for more intensive services will only grow.
Theories of the Effect of Environment on Older Adults Older adults are not all alike. Many measures of physical, social, and mental functioning show greater variability among older adults than among middle-aged or young adults. This diversity of needs makes necessary a range, or continuum, of housing options. A key variable that helps determine a person’s need for environmental support is cognitive intactness. More
Structuring Environments
407
intact individuals need less support. More impaired individuals not only need more support but are more vulnerable to the toxic effects of a poor match between their needs and their environment. Modern theories of relevance to housing options were articulated by Kahana (1982) and Lawton and Nahemow (1973). Kahana’s congruence model posited that persons with particular needs search for the environments that will best meet those needs. The better the environment meets the individual’s needs, the greater the individual’s satisfaction in life. Lawton and Nahemow’s person-environment model describes the relationship between the person’s competence, or maximum ability to function in the world (defined in terms of biological health, sensorimotor functioning, cognitive skills, ego strength, and social status and resources), and the environment’s press (the physical, interpersonal, or social demands placed by a given environment on its occupants). When competence and press are well matched, the individual’s adaptation, including contentment and daily functioning, is good. When press exceeds competence somewhat, the individual feels challenged and stressed. Press that greatly exceeds competence produces great distress, maladaptive behavior, and functional failure in the individual. On the other hand, when competence exceeds press somewhat, the individual is initially comfortable but then becomes bored. When competence greatly exceeds press, the individual is greatly bored, has a sense of deprivation, and often shows maladaptive behavior. A somewhat more general theory, which nevertheless has implications for the impact of environment on well-being, is control theory as articulated by Schulz and Heckhausen (1999). They describe the striving for control over all aspects of one’s life as a universal and lifelong human characteristic, which is modified only in its manifestation by cultural and other contextual factors. This striving is expressed through both primary control (i.e., behaviors aimed at modifying the external world for one’s benefit) and secondary control (i.e., thoughts and actions designed to change the individual’s internal world and preserve the motivation for primary control striving). Gitlin et al. (2001) suggest that the unsuccessful application of these mechanisms to achieve control may result in negative affective consequences. This would provide a theoretical foundation for individuals’ motivation, according to Kahana (1982) and Lawton and Nahemow (1973), to seek an environment in which their primary and secondary control strategies bring success. In an approach that is complementary to those of Kahana (1982) and Lawton and Nahemow (1973), Namazi and Johnson (2001) propose the continuity model of person and environment. This model holds that older adults’ psychosocial, cultural, and personal identities and needs are at least as important as their medical needs. Impaired adults, including cognitively
408
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
impaired persons, should not be separated from the mainstream of life, but rather should be provided environmental means to retain elements of their accustomed lifestyle. The model ascribes three lifelong expectations to older adults: a desire for autonomy and independence, an expectation for a continuation of the normal course of events, and the goal of being functional as long as possible. Autonomy refers to the older adult, even with cognitive impairment, retaining all reasonable freedom of choice or control over life circumstances. Related terms include independence and self-sufficiency. The term pertinent autonomy highlights the need to continually assess in which functions the individual retains some capacity and to allow appropriate freedom in that area. The model thus takes into account the need for preventing cognitively impaired persons from exercising autonomy when the result would be unacceptable risk to the safety of the person or others. Normalcy is a property of the older adult’s physical environment and behavioral routine. It is desirable that the individual’s housing and work and leisure activities resemble what is normal or typical for any other adult in that culture. All things being equal, a living environment that feels homelike to the individual—in harmony (called continuity and balance by Namazi and Johnson) with the person’s regional, cultural, and socioeconomic background—will lead to the best outcomes. Individuals thus differ in the degree of privacy versus space sharing they prefer and in their desire for pets, plants, children, access to the outdoors, etc. Functionality is also a property of the individual’s environment. It includes any aspect of the physical environment that encourages residents to achieve better function in activities of daily living. Environmental features that help cognitively impaired persons function include assistive devices, multisensory way-finding aids, modified furniture and closets, improved lighting, and increased visual access.
Theories of the Effect of Environment on Cognitively Impaired Persons The “progressively lowered stress threshold model” of Hall and Buckwalter (1987) proposes that because persons with progressive dementing illnesses experience progressive reduction in their ability to adapt, reduction of environmental stress will result in a slower rate of deterioration of the person’s physical and behavioral functions. This reduction of environmental stress can be accomplished through caregiver training and care planning and by lowering environmental demands to a level congruent with the person’s abilities. This model is often invoked when reduced stim-
Structuring Environments
409
ulation is recommended for cognitively impaired persons. Day et al. (2000) reviewed the research evidence on low-stimulation dementia nursing units and described the findings as mixed. Reducing overstimulation and unpleasant stimulation such as noise, odors, and agitated neighbors has been found to lead to weight gains, less behavioral disturbance, and reduced use of physical and chemical restraints. There has been no effect, however, in normalizing sleep patterns or in decreasing incontinence or wandering. In many instances, specialized dementia units actually provide more stimulation, albeit more congruent with residents’ needs, than do traditional nursing units, and this has been associated with improved resident care and staff morale. The last 15 years have seen the publication of many lists of key environmental dimensions, environmental design principles, or therapeutic goals of the environment, for occupants with dementia. Lawton et al. (2000) compiled the work of Moos and Lemke (1994), Lawton (1986), Calkins (1988), Cohen and Weisman (1991), and Regnier and Pynoos (1992) into seven generic, late-life-specific environmental dimensions. The dimensions are 1) safety and orientation; 2) functionality, including prosthetic aids, competence in activities of daily living (ADLs), and accessibility; 3) stimulation, including sensory aspects, aesthetics, and appearance; 4) personal control, including privacy, personalization, and choice; 5) social interaction, including privacy and recreation; 6) continuity, including familiarity and ties with healthy persons; and 7) change, including adaptability. On the basis of their review of the cited models of person-environment interaction in dementia, Lawton et al. (2000) developed an instrument (the Professional Environmental Assessment Procedure [PEAP]) to comprehensively evaluate dementia units. They created a set of nine environmental dimensions of such units, summarized in Table 13–1, each a desired outcome of quality environments. Although it is designed for specific application to NLDC units, this list of dimensions is an excellent synopsis of the theories in the general field of designing environments for cognitively impaired persons. Most dimensions are actually environmental qualities that are considered desirable for cognitively impaired persons. In some cases, however, two or more dimensions are mutually exclusive and must be balanced against each other appropriately, according to individual needs.
The Lesson The fundamental implication of this overview of environmental parameters is that there is no one environment that is correct for all persons, even for all
410
TABLE 13–1.
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Environmental dimensions of dementia units
Social contact: Do the physical environment and rules governing its use support social contact and interaction among residents? Awareness and orientation: Are individual staff members, visitors, and residents enabled to orient themselves to physical, social, and temporal dimensions of the environment? Safety and security: Does the environment minimize threats to residents’ safety and maximize the sense of security of residents, staff, and family members? Privacy: Is there regulation of input from the environment (e.g., noise) and output to the environment (e.g., confidential conversations)? Stimulation and coherence (regulation): Are stimuli maintained at acceptable levels and the number of competing stimuli limited? Stimulation and coherence (quality): Is there a predominance of enjoyable levels of stimulation of any of the senses, at levels strong or frequent enough to provide interest and novelty without exceeding tolerable levels? Support of functional abilities: Do the environment and the rules regarding the use of the environment support the practice or continued use of everyday skills? Opportunities for personal control: Do the physical environment and the rules governing its use provide residents with opportunities to exercise personal preference, choice, and independent initiative? Continuity of the self: Is there preservation of continuity between present and past environments, and past and present self, through presence of personal items of the individual and a noninstitutional ambiance? Source. Adapted from Lawton MP, Weisman GD, Sloane P, et al.: “Professional Environmental Assessment Procedure for Special Care Units for Elders With Dementia Illness and Its Relationship to the Therapeutic Environment Screening Schedule.” Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 4:28–38, 2000. Used with permission.
cognitively impaired persons. The ideal environment for any persons depends on that individual’s unique needs and level of competence. Furthermore, at any level of need and competence, good adaptation is often achievable if placement in an appropriate environment can be arranged. The practical answer to the need for appropriate environments for people with various degrees of impairment is the provision of a continuum of housing options. This continuum now exists in many United States cities, and increasingly in more rural areas. The continuum tends to be more finely graded, or subdivided, in urban areas than in rural areas. The evolution of this continuum (and its increasing availability) is a happy sign of our times, a zeitgeist in the graying of the Western world. The phrase aging in place has become popular again in recent years. Several years ago, the phrase referred to the efforts of older adults to stay in their own homes in the community (Callahan 1993). More recently, it has been adopted by the senior housing industry as a marketing concept. Residents are encouraged to stay at the same facility, even as their care needs
Structuring Environments
411
intensify. It is our opinion that this can be a sound recommendation if the facility in fact provides a full continuum of levels of care, as described earlier. If, on the other hand, the facility offers only a narrow range of levels of care, such as lighter to heavier regular assisted living, it is unwise to encourage residents with intensive care needs to remain in that facility. The merits of aging in place do not outweigh the merits of living in a setting that meets the individual’s needs. The availability of a full continuum allows, but does not guarantee, optimal alignment of patients’ needs with therapeutic environments. This alignment must be achieved one person at a time, and it requires assessment of the resident’s cognition, emotion, behavior, and mobility (Lichtenberg 1999). The assessment findings should then be compared with the definitions of target populations for the various care levels available, and a placement decision should be made. Because many forms of dementing illness are progressive, the assessment should be repeated at least annually.
The Private Residence as a Dementia Caregiving Environment Mrs. Fry is an 80-year-old married college graduate and former bookkeeper. She drank heavily until 12 years ago. She and her husband moved from a large, active retirement development to their own home in this suburb 10 years ago. Geropsychological assessment was requested regarding her cognitive status. She shows only very mild impairment in feeding, dressing, bathing, and toileting herself. She has difficulty filling her time with appropriate activities. She still likes to cook but has found this confusing for about 2 years. Her housekeeping has deteriorated noticeably, she makes errors in taking her pills, and her husband has prohibited her from driving. Cognitive assessment reveals intact expressive and receptive language, immediate memory, and receptive visuospatial skills; mild impairment of orientation, long-term memory, abstraction, and productive visuospatial skills; moderate impairment of concentration and calculation; and severe impairment of delayed memory. Her score on the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) (Folstein et al. 1975) is 23/30. She reports some sadness about her declining abilities but expresses love for her husband and her cat. She has been administered the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Scale (Hughes et al. 1982) (score, 1) and the Global Deterioration Scale (GDS) (Reisberg et al. 1982) (score, 3–4). It is concluded that her cognitive impairment is mild and that, with her husband’s assistance, she would do well at home for the time being
As indicated earlier, two-thirds of older Americans live in their own home or apartment, and another 15% live in the home of a nonspouse relative. It is therefore unavoidable that the vast majority of older adults with
412
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
cognitive impairment also live in these private residences, with relatives as caregivers. The gradual course of progressive dementias such as Alzheimer’s disease allows successful caregiving of many persons in an unmodified home setting for 2 or 3 years. Eventually, however, almost every person with Alzheimer’s disease will develop impairments that render unmodified conditions dangerous or antitherapeutic. Because most cognitively impaired persons are older adults, they often have impaired vision, balance, reflexes, and strength. There is now very substantial literature on designing or modifying environments for access by all (so-called universal design) and general strategies for modifying home settings to maximize the safety of older adults. Universal design is the effort to create living environments that are user friendly to persons of all ages, sizes, and abilities, enhancing occupants’ independence for as long as possible (AARP 2001). This approach includes recommendations for every room of the house, for lighting, exterior spaces, and storage. The emphasis is on reducing the required strength, height, and reaching distance for using all spaces, features, and appliances. Flat floors, wide doors, low counters, easy-action handles, and conveniently placed storage, outlets, and controls typify this approach (Riley 1999). Within this tradition, Pinto et al. (2000) add an emphasis on tipresistant tables and chairs, improved task lighting, nonskid floors, and simplified appliance controls. A general approach to modifying living environments for the needs of older adults is to focus on their risk of falling. Falls often bring serious health consequences, and detection and removal of environmental hazards can produce measurable reductions in falls and their sequelae. Of course, the risk of falling and the need to minimize hazards are even greater for cognitively impaired elders. Tideiksaar (1997) has condensed several published checklists of environmental fall hazards and recommended modifications (Table 13–2). The actual process of screening home environments for safety hazards is often performed by nurses and by occupational therapists (Gitlin et al. 2001; Mann et al. 1999; Salkeld et al. 2000). Lowery et al. (2000) examined the frequency of environmental hazards in the homes and care environments of persons with dementia, as well as the association of the hazards with falls. For each of 41 dementia patients living at home and 21 living in a formal care setting, Tideiksaar’s (1986) environmental hazards checklist was completed, and caregiver diaries recorded falls by patients. The researchers found hazards in 95% of patient’s homes and in 74% of residential or nursing facilities occupied by subjects. The average number of hazards was 5.4 in homes and 1.8 in formal care settings. The most common hazards in homes were low chairs, absence of grab rails in bathrooms,
Structuring Environments
413
TABLE 13–2. Environmental hazards and modifications obtained from safety checklists Illumination
Eliminate low lighting; provide sufficient lighting, especially in high-risk areas (e.g., bedroom, bathroom, stairways). Place illuminated light switches in similar locations for easy visibility. Place night lights along the pathway from the bedroom to bathroom to permit safe nighttime travel. Avoid lighting glare; use glare-free light bulbs or shields.
Floor surfaces Maintain nonskid floors; especially avoid waxing kitchen floors. Cover slippery surfaces with nonskid carpeting. Remove throw rugs; replace with nonskid rugs. Wipe up floor spills immediately. Avoid floor clutter, low-lying objects, and thick pile carpets to minimize tripping. Secure loose lamp and telephone cords that are in walkways. Furnishings
Provide chairs and sofas of proper height to permit safe sitting and standing. Remove furniture that is too low and difficult to get up from. Provide beds that are low to permit safe movement. Arrange furniture to allow for wide walkways.
Stairways
Equip stairways with secure handrails for support. Clearly mark step edges with bright nonskid tape to indicate where steps begin and end. Check that step surfaces are in good repair and are nonskid.
Bathroom
Install grab bars and toilet risers if toilet seat is too low. Place nonskid strips or mats in the bathtub to prevent slipping. Install grab bars in the bathtub or shower to support balance.
Source. Adapted with permission from Tideiksaar R: Falling in Old Age: Prevention and Management, 2nd Edition. New York, Springer, 1997, p. 265. Copyright 1997 Springer Publishing Company, Inc., New York, 10012.
loose rugs, and lack of chair armrests. The most common hazards in formal care settings were low toilets and inadequate night lights. This study demonstrates that multiple rectifiable risks are common in the environments of dementia patients. To analyze the impact of the home’s physical features on dementia caregiving, Olsen et al. (1999) conducted extensive at-home interviews with 90 familial caregivers of persons with dementia. Of the respondents, 66% were female, 51% were spouses, and 46% were adult children. Respondents rated their homes as caregiving environments, citing specific helpful and unhelpful features, and then described an “ideal” caregiving environment. Eight specific design features were identified that made it easier to be a caregiver. There were significantly different ratings given to different
414
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
house forms. On a scale from 1 (“makes it much harder”) to 4 (“makes it much easier”), one-level homes were rated 3.50, apartments were rated 3.17, multistory homes were rated 2.45, and split-level or raised-ranch styles were rated 2.00. Residences that had a master bedroom and bathroom on the main level were rated significantly more positively than those that did not, underscoring the detrimental impact of stairs on caregiving. Caregivers highly valued having adequate space at home. Larger rooms, even at the expense of number of rooms, were seen as helping caregivers and care recipients feel less confined. An extra bedroom for live-in help was also seen as being advantageous. Simple floor plans without long hallways and many turns were seen as less confusing. Cognitively impaired persons find their destination more easily if they can see it (Namazi and Johnson 1991). Respondents also preferred open layouts that provided straight sight lines. These created many advantages, including allowing the caregivers to monitor unobtrusively and allowing care recipients to feel reassured by the caregiver’s presence. Caregivers emphasized the need for the environment to assist them in achieving privacy during their own personal time. Adult children and other nonspousal caregivers, in particular, wanted some private space, while recognizing the need to balance this against the need to monitor the patient. Caregivers attested to the importance of a safe, accessible, easy to find, and conveniently located bathroom. This generally translated into a full bathroom on the main floor with a stall shower large enough for a shower chair. Safe kitchens were seen as those that could be easily closed off with doors, contained electric rather than gas stoves, and had appliances with hard-toaccess controls or hidden on-off switches. Finally, caregivers pointed to the value of safe outdoor access. Cognitively impaired care recipients seemed to feel less confined if their home had an enclosed or screened porch, a securely fenced yard, or a deck with secure gates. Olsen et al. (1999) summarized their respondents’ comments about helpful existing home features, and features of an ideal dementia care home, into six interrelated qualities. Safety included such details as antiscalding devices, power-protected stoves, lockable storage for dangerous household chemicals, and methods of curtailing access to the front door. Accessibility into and throughout the house helps maintain the patient’s independence and mobility, and makes it easier for caregivers to assist them with basic ADLs. Independence is also supported by orientation aids, labeled objects and places, organized closets, and grab bars. Decreasing the potential for conflict is accomplished by removing fragile or irreplaceable objects from easy access. Caregivers can then be less fearful of an accident involving these possessions, and cognitively impaired persons can enjoy their own home with fewer admonitions to be careful. Increasing safe movement
Structuring Environments
415
includes creating safe indoor wandering paths and safe outdoor access. Calkins and Chafetz (1996) refer to this as “secure freedom”—the freedom to be mobile within boundaries that exclude dangers. An open floor plan for monitoring and interacting allows both caregivers and cognitively impaired persons more flexibility in meeting their needs. Both wish to have the other in visual and conversational contact, yet be able to engage simultaneously in other tasks. Open layouts allow this. Olsen et al. (1999) suggest a temporal model of home environment adaptation. For persons with minimal cognitive impairment, the emphasis should be on making the home safe, to support the independence of the person. As judgment deteriorates, the emphasis should move to restricting access to dangerous areas and creating safe areas. If mobility becomes severely impaired, emphasis moves to creating wheelchair accessibility. Concise guides to modifying home environments for dementia care were provided more than a decade ago by Calkins et al. (1990) and by Olsen et al. (1993). Warner (2000), however, has recently provided an encyclopedic guide, The Complete Guide to Alzheimer’s Proofing Your Home. Writing it for laypeople, Warner discusses in detail ways to adapt virtually every aspect of a private residence to play a more positive role in dementia caregiving. Warner provides an overview of the symptoms and course of Alzheimer’s disease and a detailed discussion of the many resulting behavioral abnormalities such as illusions, mood disturbance, shadowing, and ADL deficits. Issues of access to spaces, continence support, mobility support, and safety are extensively discussed. In most instances, recommendations address both comfort for the person with Alzheimer’s disease and ease of use by the caregiver. Almost every page includes one or more brief vignettes of actual experiences, and the book is generously illustrated with photographs and diagrams. The author also provides of names and addresses for vendors of hundreds of specific products that support caregiving. These include common products, such as grab bars or motion-detecting light switches, and unusual products, such as a surveillance camera disguised as a clock radio, locking mailboxes, and electric-eye faucets. With regard to conceptual content, the author recommends that caregivers think of their homes as divided into three zones. There are danger zones, which should be off limits to the person with dementia; a respite zone reserved as a sanctuary for the caregiver; and a safe zone, encompassing most of the house, which the person with dementia is free to occupy and enjoy. Colombo et al. (1998) describe an innovative program in which professionals assessed the environment of private residences of families caring for persons with dementia, and recommended modifications. Part of a sixcountry, 3-year project funded by the European Community studying the ethics of applying technological modifications in ordinary housing for per-
416
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
sons with dementia, the research involved 69 persons with severe dementia who passed through a dementia special care unit (SCU), which is apparently a short-stay evaluation and stabilization unit. Staff members made two visits to each patient’s residence, one during the patient’s SCU stay and one following discharge. Both visits were used to assess the residential caregiving environment and the needs of both the patient and the caregivers. Staff then crafted lists of recommendations for environmental modifications that were personalized, simple, feasible and adaptable, shareable (e.g., not disruptive to the functioning of other household members), and affordable. Recommendations included covering bedroom windows with dark curtains to assist with sleeping, placing a portable toilet near the bed at night, removing mirrors and televisions, improving access to gardens, reducing access to basements and unfenced yards, adding grab bars in bathrooms and handrails in hallways, increasing task lighting and color contrast at the dining table, and removing bath mats and loose carpets. A recent American Association of Retired Persons survey (2000) found that the percentage of Americans age 55 and over who strongly or somewhat agreed that they wanted to remain in their current residence as long as possible rose from 84% in 1992 to 89% in 2000. Even when asked to consider a scenario of needing assistance, 82% preferred not to move from their current residence. The desire to stay home is indeed powerful, and it is clear that professionals can do much to assist families to optimize their homes as caregiving environments.
Contributions of Digital Technology to Home Care Mace and Rabins’ (1991) The 36-Hour Day includes a description of gadgets available to make home caregiving easier. Some of the electronic items they listed now seem quaint, such as recording telephones, sound monitors, and clock radios. Since then, the digital revolution has dramatically raised the sophistication and capability of electronic devices. Various devices (such as heat, motion, or sound detectors or call buttons located around the house or on the person of a cognitively impaired individual) can be programmed to notify, depending on the time of day, the home caregiver, neighbor, physician, local 911 provider, etc. Marshall (1999) describes a system in which smoke and heat detectors are located near the stove and are connected to circuitry that, if triggered, automatically notifies a local resource center, turns on the house fan, turns off the stove, and unlocks the house door. Similarly, motion detectors can now be aimed at various potentially dangerous appliances (stove, oven, etc.) or exits and can be pro-
Structuring Environments
417
grammed through a computer to create a notification only if triggered during designated protected times of the day. Digital technology enhances the caregiver’s ability to prevent wandering away from private residences or nursing facilities. A few vendors now offer systems that use the Global Positioning System (GPS) to ensure that a person who does wander off can be quickly located. For example, Digital Angel, available as a wristwatch or a belt-clipped, pager-shaped unit, contains a GPS transmitter, a skin temperature sensor, a sudden fall sensor, and a 911 call button. Information from the unit is transmitted to the company’s computer system and is continually available to the caregiver both through the Internet and by telephone. The trend in the application of digital technology to dementia care has been to focus on safety issues, but Marshall (1999) points out that other dementia care functions can also profit from this technology, such as monitoring and maintaining health and enhancing quality of life. One rapidly developing application of digital technology is smart pill containers (Szeto and Giles 1997). Some such devices keep the time and are programmed to remind the user, at each appointed time, to take a pill. Others keep a digital history of container openings, thus measuring the individual’s compliance with the prescribed regimen. There is also a computerized pager system that reminds users to take their medication. Digital technology also includes automated cueing devices. These are computers that emit voice prompts to remind individuals how to progress, step by step, through an activity such as washing or dressing (Milhailidis et al. 1998). In all applications of technology to dementia care, professionals and caregivers must remember that the human element cannot be completely replaced (Ellis 2000).
Assisted Living Settings as Dementia Caregiving Environments Mrs. M, an 83-year-old widow, has shown progressive cognitive decline for about 7 years. Two years ago, she moved from her own home of 23 years to an independent retirement apartment, and, 1 year ago, into a regular assisted living setting. Geropsychological assessment was requested to explain a “discrepancy between her terrible memory and her excellent conversational ability.” She has mild impairment in feeding and dressing herself. She resists bathing and has some body odor. She is continent, but her room sometimes smells of feces, and she resists having the linens and carpet cleaned. Cognitive assessment reveals intact expressive and receptive language; mild impairment of productive and receptive visuospatial skills; moderate impairment of long-term memory, abstraction, and judgment;
418
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
and severe impairment of orientation, immediate and delayed memory, and insight. Her MMSE score is 17/30. She reports boredom and expresses anger regarding her living arrangement, insisting that she is perfectly fine and wants to live on her own. It is concluded that her dementia is mild to moderate (CDR score, 1–2; GDS score, 4), and that she would be best served on an ALDC unit.
The explosive growth of the assisted living industry is easily seen in every American city (Namazi and Chafetz 2001). Individuals with mild cognitive impairment and no other impairment no longer go to a nursing home, unless they are in the minority who need access to Medicaid benefits. These individuals now enter assisted living. As a result, as mentioned earlier, the percentage of assisted-living residents with dementia is approximately 40%. Therefore, principles of environmental design for dementia care are relevant to every assisted-living setting and are compelling for assisted-living dementia care (ALDC) settings. Chafetz (2001) finds the appropriate degree of impairment for ALDC residents to be mild to moderate (CDR score, 1–2; GDS score, 4–5) ALDC differs from regular assisted living in several ways. The staff provide a full schedule of dementia-graded activities, from morning to night, 7 days a week, to help residents engage in appropriate behaviors and thereby maintain their mood, self-esteem, and normal sleep cycle. The staff have knowledge of dementia are trained in dementia care skills. Although the physical environment conforms to all the principles described earlier, the ambiance is homelike. Unit census is limited; a cap of 20 is ideal, but 30–35 is sometimes workable. Exit doors are secured with locks controlled by digital keypad or similar devices. Light levels are high. There is easy access to an outdoor space that is designed for safety and is surrounded by an unclimbable fence. Two excellent sourcebooks for designing formal, congregate care environments for persons with dementia at both the assisted-living and nursing levels are those by Calkins and Sloane (1997) and Brawley (1997). Calkins and Sloane describe how the physical environment can support the functioning of cognitively impaired residents, accommodate behaviors, maximize functional abilities, promote safety, and encourage independence. They describe 10 subgoals and provide strategies for achieving them. The first subgoal is maximizing awareness and orientation. Strategies include effective signage, predictability in use of space and time, visual differentiation of spaces, visual access, and use of all the senses. Ensuring safety and security includes the strategies of balancing safety with autonomy, monitoring residents, controlling unauthorized exiting, and providing appropriate physical and technological supports. Providing privacy includes providing residents with options for privacy in bedrooms and elsewhere and imple-
Structuring Environments
419
menting policies for staff behavior that respect residents’ privacy rights. Supporting functional abilities includes strategies for supporting independence in dressing, toileting, bathing, eating, transfer and ambulation, and activities. Adjusting the amount of stimulation emphasizes minimizing potentially distracting acoustic stimulation, regulating the amount of visual stimulation, and avoiding unpleasant odors while judiciously using pleasant scents and aromas. Providing positive quality stimulation addresses the therapeutic use of pleasant or useful stimulation in the visual, acoustic, olfactory, and tactile realms. Providing opportunities for personal control involves allowing residents choices both between spaces (for example, by having a variety of common areas) and within spaces (for example, though varied seating options and table sizes and through adjustable window treatments). Facilitating social contact includes strategic placement of furniture, providing props to stimulate conversation, and arranging spaces that provide diverse social stimuli. Maintaining self-identity emphasizes the importance of a noninstitutional-appearing environment that is actively personalized with residents’ decorations and possessions and that therefore continually reminds residents of their lifelong interests. Adaptation to stage of disease refers to the gradual shift in care goals from continuity and self-expression in early dementing illness, to functional maintenance in middle dementia, and to comfort in late-stage dementia. Brawley’s (1997) text, written primarily for architects and design professionals, is a detailed, accessible, and beautifully illustrated volume. Although the text seems to focus on NLDC environments, the content is equally applicable to ALDC. After discussing normal aging, the causes and symptoms of dementia, and dementia design principles, Brawley explains in considerable detail three areas of design consideration. The discussion of the sensory environment thoroughly addresses designing the acoustical environment for impaired ears and brains. Brawley then reviews the roles and correct uses of lighting, colors, pattern, and visual texture for eyes and brains affected by age and dementia. This discussion of the importance and uses of lighting in dementia care is the best available. Brawley makes seven recommendations and provides guidance in accomplishing them. They are 1) raise the level of illumination; 2) provide consistent, even light levels; 3) eliminate glare; 4) provide access to natural daylight; 5) provide gradual changes in light levels; 6) provide focused task lighting; and 7) improve color rendition from lamps or light sources (p. 87). Means to accommodate persons with dementia in such interior design features as furniture, fabrics, floor coverings, wall and ceiling finishes, and windows are thoroughly covered. Dementia-specific considerations in designing activity spaces and outdoor spaces, and in creating a feeling of home, are discussed.
420
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Davis et al. (2000) closely examined 259 randomly selected residents of 56 residential care facilities providing specialized dementia programs (RC-SDP) in seven states. This is analogous to the ALDC terminology used above. Based on bed capacity and administrative relationships with other facilities or units, the RC-SDPs were found to be of five types. There were small, independently operated homes (34%); multiple small homes with joint administration (12.5%); larger, all-dementia facilities (20%); SDPs operated within larger, exclusively residential care facilities (21%); and RC-SDPs in multilevel facilities (12.5%). The five types varied little in the average severity of dementia of their residents, in the prevalence of disruptive behaviors reported, or in residents’ use of psychotropic medication. Furthermore, the RC-SDP residents were only slightly less impaired in cognition and ADL function than a comparably assessed sample of 1,340 residents of 138 nursing home dementia special care units. It thus appears that the continuum of residential dementia care services may actually be a series of largely overlapping, rather than discrete, levels, at least in terms of the characteristics of residents. Many persons wonder at the ease with which health professionals and loved ones are allowed to place persons with dementia involuntarily behind the locked doors of specialized dementia care units, either ALDC or NLDC (Redmond and Chafetz 2001). The lack of a requirement for a court order or any other comment from any part of the legal system is a striking contrast to the legal hurdles placed before an involuntary psychiatric admission. The secure-perimeter feature of ALDC and NLDC is certainly no secret, and these units operate, indeed advertise, in all jurisdictions, actually under state regulation. Yet these units clearly restrict the civil rights of individuals who have not been adjudicated as being incapacitated by a judge. California is the only state that has explicitly addressed this issue. California law prohibits residential care facilities for the elderly from restricting residents’ right to come and go at any time and to be free of any restraining device. Recognizing the clinical benefits of an ALDC program, the California Secured Perimeters Pilot Program was begun in 1989 and was made permanent in 1996 (Wilber and Machemer 1999). Initially, six facilities participated in a 3-year demonstration. In return for permission to secure their perimeters, these facilities were required to provide dementia-specific staff training, detailed clinical assessment of residents, specialized dementia care plans, and other service enhancements. Placement of residents in secured units required the permission of the residents or their authorized decision makers by virtue of conservatorship, guardianship, or durable power of attorney for health care. Outcome data strongly supported the clinical value of these now-standard ALDC care components, and there was widespread support for the ALDC model among fam-
Structuring Environments
421
ilies and providers. However, there are numerous challenges inherent in determining the capacity for informed consent and in balancing autonomy and safety. ALDC residents usually retain the ability to perform many of the steps involved in dressing. They have the strength, dexterity, balance, and flexibility needed to dress themselves, but they often lack the judgment required to appropriately choose garments. The results can include visually unappealing combinations, excessive rewearing of garments between washings, omission or layering of garments, or choices that are inappropriate to the season, weather, or occasion. These deficits in the cognitive component of the dressing task can be remediated by environmental modifications. The now-popular approach is to provide closets with a main compartment containing the resident’s wardrobe, and a compartment holding only the clothes needed for the resident’s next clothing change. The main compartment is unobtrusively locked, and staff and family members hold keys. The current-need compartment displays the outfit components in the order they should be donned. It may either be equipped with an unlocked door or have no door, leaving the outfit totally open to view (see Brawley 1997, p. 176). Namazi and Johnson (1992) found that this two-compartment closet increased the dressing independence of a group of residents with moderate to severe dementia. Although the preparation of the currentneed compartment requires some staff time, this is probably offset by greater efficiency in the actual dressing process. This sort of specialized closet should become standard in ALDC settings. Similar considerations as in dressing also apply in way finding and room identification. ALDC residents possess all the requisite abilities except the cognitive abilities, and these are supportable environmentally. Therefore, ample pertinent signage (such as to the kitchen or restroom) and use of landmarks (such as a decorative fountain, fireplace, large plant or clock, or even a water fountain) is an important component of ALDC design. Passini et al. (2000) refer to such landmarks as reference points or anchor points, and they indicate that “an ideal reference point combines form, function, and meaning” (p. 698). A personalized display on or just outside the entry door often facilitates room finding.
Nursing Facility Settings as Dementia Caregiving Environments Mr. F, an 87-year-old married man with a college education, began showing symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease 7 years ago. For about a year, he has been incontinent of urine several times daily, and of feces about once a day. He
422
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
requires, and is cooperative with, total assistance with bathing and dressing, and he feeds himself fairly well after setup. When his symptoms became too difficult for his wife to manage at home, he was admitted to an ALDC unit. Because he never participated in unit activities, an evaluation was requested. Geropsychological assessment revealed no areas of cognitive preservation. He showed moderate impairment of expressive and receptive language and severe impairment of orientation, concentration, constructional (clock drawing) and receptive visuospatial skills, and all aspects of memory (immediate, delayed, and long term). His MMSE score was 3/30, and he was unable to participate meaningfully in formal memory testing. It was concluded that his dementia was severe (CDR score, 3; GDS score, 6) and that he would be best served by an NLDC unit.
The first formal specialized dementia care units in the United States were in nursing homes, appearing in the late 1960s and early 1970s. The concept of the nursing dementia SCU evolved over the ensuing decades, from large wards with a strong psychiatric emphasis, into a stronger focus on activity support, and later into more diversification (Sloane and Lindeman 1996). The result in most cities is now a clear demarcation between ALDC and NLDC. Appropriate NLDC residents are completely or largely independent in mobility, are awake most of the day, and exhibit severe cognitive impairment (CDR score, 3; GDS score, 6). As in ALDC, NLDC is characterized by a staff trained in dementia care, a physical environment that is rich in orientation cues and illumination but clutter free, a homelike ambiance, a limited unit census, secured exit doors, and easy access to a therapeutic outdoor space. Unlike ALDC, the activities focus more on simple, gross motor, and multisensory elements such as music and song, aromas, pets, massage, and simple movement. There is greater use of nursing services, including total assistance in bathing, eating, toileting, and dressing. Kovach (1997) also emphasizes the importance of pain control and attention to overall comfort. There are probably no colors that are particularly well or poorly suited to dementia care environments. Persons with dementia are generally likely to enjoy the same color schemes as are healthy people. Walls of bright red or yellow probably have no intrinsically agitating quality. Rather, they are not visually appealing for most people. However, an important special consideration with this population is that colors should be used to create contrast only when contrast is helpful, and to avoid contrast when contrast is not helpful. In calculating contrast, designers must consider the relative difficulty that elderly persons have in accurately seeing blue, violet, purple, and red. Brawley (1997) lists the following for contrast: light color against black, dark color against white, light yellow against dark blue, and dark red against light green. Poor color choices for contrast are dark green against bright red, yellow against white or similar lightness, blue against green, and
Structuring Environments
423
lavender against pink (p. 115). Contrasting colors are desirable to aid in locating tableware, handrails, and toilets but are undesirable within flat surfaces such as floors and walls. Related to color choices is the use of patterns on walls, floors, curtains, and upholstery. As cognitive impairment increases, the likelihood of misperceiving patterns as actual objects increases. This strongly suggests that decorative finishes portraying potentially disturbing images, such as insects, eyes, or animals, should not be selected, especially in NLDC settings. Perceptual problems of persons with dementia include prosopagnosia, misidentifications, and illusions that are frequently aggravated by mirrors. Kittur and Ruskin (1999) present two examples of Alzheimer’s disease patients who were confused by mirrors. An 83-year-old man with Alzheimer’s disease lived at home. He perceived in his mirror a dog that was constantly watching him and routinely became very angry in response. A 72-year-old man who lived in a specialized Alzheimer’s disease unit appeared to feel positively toward the image that appeared in his mirror, but he became enraged when it would not follow him to other locations or respond to other requests. In both of these cases, episodes of angry agitation was significantly reduced when the mirrors were removed. The essence of the dementia SCU or NLDC is easy to describe but difficult to quantify and assess. As Gwyther puts it, “we are coming to understand that not everything we can count, counts, and not everything that counts can be counted” (Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association 1997, Introduction). As mentioned in the discussion of the PEAP by Lawton et al. (2000), various desirable environmental qualities for dementia patients may be mutually exclusive. For example, privacy and personal control can easily be the opposite of social contact, safety, and security. Unit and program designers must balance these qualities against each other, according to the needs of their target population. The difficulty of this task is illustrated by Morgan and Stewart (1998, 1999), who studied a group of dementia patients in a long-term-care facility in Saskatchewan, Canada, before and after a move from an old to a new SCU. Relative to the old SCU, the new SCU had lower resident density, all private rooms with private bathrooms, smaller unit census (20 versus 69), larger and fewer dining and lounge areas, longer corridors, and no toilets near the common area. Residents spent significantly more time alone in their new private bedrooms during the day and early evening than they had on the old unit. Thus, it can be said that residents’ privacy was increased. In addition, use of sleeping medication by 11 residents during two 4-month periods separated by 1 year (before and after the move) was reduced by more than 75%. It therefore appeared that residents were sleeping better in private rooms. Qualitative data on the impact of the new unit were also gath-
424
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
ered. A sample of four registered nurses, five attendants, four spouses, three adult children, and two other relatives were interviewed. There was consensus that sleep was improved and that overstimulation of residents (and resulting irritability and interresident conflict) was reduced. Trespassing and rummaging were also reduced. Staff members, however, also pointed out several disadvantages of private rooms. These included reduced efficiency of supervision and less opportunity to work in the same room with a colleague. Longer corridors meant more walking for staff and residents, which was especially problematic due to the lack of a toilet near the common areas. Married or lower-income residents had often preferred semiprivate rooms. The second article by Morgan and Stewart (1999) appears to be a fuller reporting of this same interview of these nine staff members and nine family members. Responses to the question, “What are your impressions of the new unit?” were thematically analyzed. An initial list of 176 thematic codes was eventually reduced to five larger categories of resident need pertaining to the physical environment. These were safety, homelike setting, optimal stimulation, cues, and options for privacy and social interaction. The new SCU was clearly preferred, on balance. However, responses also reflected recognition of the trade-offs involved. Although it was blamed for higher rates of agitation and irritability than were observed in the new SCU, the high spatial and social density of the old SCU was also seen as enriching and usually tolerable. The new SCU was described as providing too little stimulation and too little opportunity for socialization. Although the new SCU was more attractively decorated, it “lacked the homey atmosphere that resulted from the physical closeness on the old unit” (p. 114). Constructs such as optimal stimulation and homelikeness appear to be rather elusive and subjective. Day et al. (2000) reviewed the empirical research regarding environmental issues in NLDC. They examined 1) planning principles, such as relocation issues, group size, and levels of care offered; 2) general environmental attributes, such as pursuit of a noninstitutional quality, safety, and appropriate levels of stimulation and light levels; 3) building organization, or how space is arranged and used in and around the structure; and 4) specific rooms and activity spaces, regarding ways to promote functionality and autonomy in bathrooms, dining rooms, and sleeping rooms. Despite the widespread use of small samples or nonequivalent comparison groups, and failure to report the degree of participants’ dementia, the authors note that there is at least some empirical support for most elements of NLDC. On that basis, the authors present the design recommendations shown in Table 13–3. Late-stage dementia (GDS score, 7) progresses beyond the parameters described earlier for NLDC. Calkins (1997) addresses the design of
Structuring Environments
425
TABLE 13–3. Recommendations for therapeutic design and planning of dementia environments Make units large enough to facilitate interaction and small enough to avoid crowding. Separate cognitively intact residents from persons with advanced dementia. Relocate residents, when necessary, in intact units rather than individually. Use homelike design throughout the facility, especially in dining rooms. Provide moderate levels of environmental stimulation. Use high light levels in general, and provide exposure to bright light when possible. Conceal panic bars and doorknobs to reduce unwanted excitement. Build in easily accessible, safe outdoor areas. Make toilets accessible to reduce incontinence. Reduce environmental factors that increase stress in bathing. Source.
Adapted from Day et al. 2000.
environments for caring for late-stage dementia patients by discussing stimulation of the various senses at the microenvironmental versus macroenvironmental levels. Although comprehension is impaired in advanced dementia, it is easy to observe that odors or sounds that are unpleasant to healthy people are also unpleasant for persons with late-stage dementia. Pleasant smells such as coffee, popcorn, and cookies can be a prelude to eating. Unpleasant odors, such as those of excrement or strong cleaning solutions, should be reduced or eliminated. In the tactile realm, pleasant stimulation—such as soothing touch; rocking and cradling; light massage with safe oils; comfortable furniture; and soft, manipulable objects to hold—can be provided. In the acoustic realm, soft music consistent with the patient’s earlier tastes can be provided, and noxious sounds such as call bells, machinery, squeaking wheelchairs, yelling staff, and inane television shows should be eliminated. Because elderly persons are generally more sensitive to cold and their immobility and cognitive impairment render them incapable of donning or doffing layers independently, they require close supervision for thermal comfort. In the visual realm, there is a strong need to avoid bright direct lighting because of the older adult’s increased vulnerability to glare. Rather, indirect but ample light should be provided. Full-spectrum illumination of the NLDC environment is not in conflict with recommendations to apply extremely bright light (10,000 lux) directly at residents, from a distance of 2–5 feet, for therapeutic purposes against depression (Sumaya et al. 2001) or insomnia (Lyketsos et al. 1999). Such phototherapy normally entails daily sessions of 0.5–2 hours of exposure to extremely bright light, during which the resident is not asked to perform any productive functions. The exception is the work of Van Someren et al.
426
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
(1997), who installed high-intensity white fluorescent tubes in the living rooms of a psychogeriatric ward, raising the average light intensity from 436 lux at baseline to 1,136 lux. The increased daytime illumination level resulted in decreased sleep cycle disturbance in residents with intact vision. Thus, brighter light (perhaps 1,000 to 2,000 lux) may be helpful for general daytime illumination, but bright light (10,000 lux) is best reserved for brief treatment periods.
Contributions of Digital Technology to Facility-Based Care Residents in formal dementia care settings at the nursing or assisted-living level need few of the safety-oriented, medication reminder, or ADL prompting digital devices discussed earlier in the context of residential caregiving, because staff members are routinely present and are charged with these same responsibilities. However, other applications of digital technology are enhancing the environments and caregiving in ALDC and NLDC units. In the area of clinical care, technology can be the quiet eyes and hands of staff, monitoring for incontinence with in-clothing detectors and monitoring for out-of-bed episodes with video links. By thus minimizing potentially disruptive room checks, nighttime sleep may be protected. Many facilities have already replaced staff time clocks with handprint readers. These devices can also supplement digital keypads as a way to quickly recognize employees or relatives and release electromagnetic door locks. Pharmacy automation (Wagner 2001) and paperless (i.e., computer-based) charting will facilitate both accuracy and chart-based research.
Summary There have been a series of very positive developments in environments for cognitively impaired persons. The increased number of standard dementia care levels has created the opportunity to achieve greater fit between residents’ evolving needs and the respective environments’ supportive characteristics. The identification and definition of key environmental components of dementia care have been improved, and there is increased recognition that optimizing the environment presents many opportunities to intervene therapeutically at every level of care. Digital technology is increasingly contributing to home- and facility-based care, and there are ever more useful published resources available for professionals and caregivers.
Structuring Environments
427
References American Association of Retired Persons: Fixing to Stay: A National Survey on Housing and Home Modification Issues. Washington, DC, American Association of Retired Persons, 2000. Available at: http://research.aarp.org/il/home_ mod_1.html. Accessed June 26, 2002 American Association of Retired Persons: Universal Design: Home Modification. Washington, DC, American Association of Retired Persons, 2001, Available at: http://www.aarp.org/universalhome. Accessed June 26, 2002 Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association: Key Elements of Dementia Care. Chicago, IL, Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association, 1997 Brawley EC: Designing for Alzheimer’s Disease: Strategies for Creating Better Care Environments. New York, Wiley, 1997 Calkins MP: Design for Dementia. Owings Mills, MD, National Health Publishing, 1988 Calkins MP: A supportive environment for people with late-stage dementia, in Late-Stage Dementia Care: A Basic Guide. Edited by Kovach CR. Washington, DC, Taylor & Francis, 1997, pp 101–112 Calkins MP, Chafetz PK: Structuring environments for patients with dementia, in The Dementias: Diagnosis, Management, and Research, 2nd Edition. Edited by Weiner MF. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1996, pp 297–311 Calkins MP, Sloane PD: Environment, in Key Elements of Dementia Care. Chicago, IL, Alzheimer’s Association, 1997, pp 67–80 Calkins MP, Namazi KH, Rosner TT, et al: Home Modifications: Responding to Dementia. Chardon, OH, Research Center of the Corinne Dolan Alzheimer Center at Heather Hill, 1990 Callahan JJ (ed): Aging in Place. Amityville, NY, Baywood, 1993 Chafetz PK: Developing dementia care units in assisted living facilities, in Assisted Living: Current Issues in Facility Management and Resident Care. Edited by Namazi KH, Chafetz PK. Westport CT, Auburn House, 2001, pp 105–126 Cohen V, Weisman GD: Holding on to Home. Baltimore, MD, Johns Hopkins University Press, 1991 Colombo M, Vitali S, Molla G, et al: The home environment modification program in the care of demented elderly: some examples. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 28 (suppl l6):83–90, 1998 Davis KJ, Sloane PD, Mitchell CM, et al: Specialized dementia programs in residential care settings. Gerontologist 40:32–42, 2000 Day K, Carreon D, Stump C: The therapeutic design of environments for people with dementia: a review of the empirical research. Gerontologist 40:397–416, 2000 Ellis D: Technology and the Future of Health Care: Preparing for the Next Thirty Years. San Francisco, CA, Jossey-Bass, 2000 Evans DA, Funkenstein H, Albert MS, et al: Prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease in a community population of older persons: higher than previously reported. JAMA 262:2551–2556, 1989
428
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Fisher C: Assisted living residents less frail. Provider 22:9–10, 1996 Fitzgerald S: Cost effective quality alternative. Assisted Living Today 3:1–5, 1995 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189– 198, 1975 Gitlin LN, Corcoran M, Winter L, et al: Randomized, controlled trial of a home environmental intervention: effect on efficacy and upset in caregivers and on daily function of persons with dementia. Gerontologist 41:4–14, 2001 Hall G, Buckwalter K: Progressively lowered stress threshold: a conceptual model for care of adults with Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 1:399–406, 1987 Hawes C, Mor V, Wildfire J, et al: Analysis of the effect of regulation on the quality of care in board and care homes. Washington, DC, Office of the Assistant Secretary for Planning and Evaluation, Office of Aging, Disability, and LongTerm Care Policy, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1995 Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL: A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry 140:566–572, 1982 Hyde J: Serving People With Dementia: Regulating Assisted Living and Residential Care Settings. Wellesley, MA, Hearthstone, 1995 Jorm AF, Korten AE, Henderson AS: The prevalence of dementia: a quantitative integration of the literature. Acta Psychiatr Scand 76:465–479, 1987 Kahana E: A congruence model of person-environment interactions, in Aging and the Environment: Theoretical Approaches. Edited by Lawton MP, Windley PG, Byerts TO, New York, Springer, 1982, pp 97–121 Kittur SD, Ruskin P: Environmental modification for treatment of agitation in Alzheimer’s patients. Neurorehabilitation 12:211–214, 1999 Kovach CR: Behaviors associated with late-stage dementia, in Late-Stage Dementia Care: A Basic Guide. Edited by Kovach CR. Washington, DC, Taylor & Francis, 1997, pp 101–112 Krauss N, Freiman M, Rhoades J, et al: MEPS Highlight: Nursing Home Update 1996: Characteristics of Nursing Home Facilities and Residents. Rockville, MD, Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, 1997. Available at: http:// www.AHCPR.gov/papers/h/2 97-0036/h/20.pdf Lair T, Lefkowitz D: Mental Health and Functional Status of Residents of Nursing and Personal Care Homes. National Medical Expenditure Survey Research Findings 7, Agency for Health Care Policy and Research (DHHS Publ No PHS-90–3470). Rockville, MD, U.S. Public Health Service, 1990 Lawton MP: Environment and Aging. Albany, NY, Center for the Study of Aging, 1986 Lawton MP, Nahemow L: Ecology of the aging process, in The Psychology of Adult Development and Aging. Edited by Eisdorfer C, Lawton MP. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 1973, pp 619–674 Lawton MP, Weisman GD, Sloane P, et al: Professional Environmental Assessment Procedure for special care units for elders with dementia illness and its relationship to the Therapeutic Environment Screening Schedule. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 14:28–38, 2000
Structuring Environments
429
Lichtenberg PA (ed): Handbook of Assessment in Clinical Gerontology. New York, Wiley, 1999 Lowery K, Buri H, Ballard C: What is the prevalence of environmental hazards in the homes of dementia sufferers and are they associated with falls? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 15:883–886, 2000 Lyketsos CG, Veiel LL, Baker A, et al: A randomized, controlled trial of bright light therapy for agitated behaviors in dementia patients residing in long-term care. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 14:520–525, 1999 Mace NL, Rabins PV: The 36-Hour Day. Baltimore, MD, Johns Hopkins University Press, 1991 Magaziner J, German P, Zimmerman SI, et al: The prevalence of dementia in a statewide sample of new nursing home admissions aged 65 and older: diagnosis by expert panel. Gerontologist 40:663–672, 2000 Mann WC, Ottenbacher KJ, Fraas L, et al: Effectiveness of assistive technology and environmental interventions in maintaining independence and reducing home care costs for the frail elderly: a randomized controlled trial. Arch Fam Med 8:210–217, 1999 Marshall M: Technology to help people with dementia remain in their own homes. Generations 23:85–87, 1999 Mihailidis A, Schuller R, Tierney MC, et al: Effective cueing techniques for prompting patients with dementia during a washroom task. Proceedings of the Rehabilitation Engineering and Assistive Technology Society of North America VOLUME:336–338, 1998. Also available at http://www.swchsc.on.ca/ ~csia/CsiAInfo/Alexflyer.htm Moos RH, Lemke S: Group Residences for Older Adults. New York, Oxford University Press, 1994 Morgan DG, Stewart NJ: Multiple occupancy versus private rooms on dementia care units. Environ Behav 30:487–503, 1998 Morgan DG, Stewart NJ: The physical environment of special care units: needs of residents with dementia from the perspective of staff and family caregivers. Qual Health Res 9:105–118, 1999 Namazi KH, Chafetz PK (eds): Assisted Living: Current Issues in Facility Management and Resident Care. Westport, CT, Auburn House, 2001 Namazi KH, Johnson BD: Physical environmental cues to reduce the problems of incontinence in Alzheimer’s disease units. Am J Alzheimer Care Relat Disord Res 6:22–28, 1991 Namazi KH, Johnson BD: Dressing independently: a closet modification model for Alzheimer’s disease patients. Am J Alzheimer Care Relat Disord Res 7:22–28, 1992 Namazi KH, Johnson BD: Life course continuity in the design of residential care facilities to improve function and independence for elderly persons, in Assisted Living: Current Issues in Facility Management and Resident Care. Edited by Namazi KH, Chafetz PK. Westport, CT, Auburn House, 2001, pp 105–126 Olsen RV, Ehrenkrantz E, Hutchings BL: Homes That Help: Advice From Caregivers for Creating a Supportive Home. Newark, NJ, New Jersey Institute of Technology Press, 1993
430
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Olsen RV, Hutchings BL, Ehrenkrantz, E: The physical design of the home as a caregiving support: an environment for persons with dementia. Care Manag J 1:125–131, 1999 Passini R, Pigot H, Rainville C, et al: Wayfinding in a nursing home for advanced dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Environ Behav 32:684–710, 2000 Phillips CC, Sloane PD, Hawes C, et al: Effects of residence in Alzheimer disease special care units on functional outcomes. JAMA 278:1340–1344, 1997 Pinto MR, De Medici S, Van Sant C, et al: Ergonomics, gerotechnology, and design for the home environment. Appl Ergon 31:317–322, 2000 Rabins PV, Black B, German P, et al: The prevalence of psychiatric disorders in elderly residents of public housing. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 51:M319– M324, 1996 Redmond R, Chafetz PK: Legal and ethical issues for assisted living facility operators: managing risk by doing the right thing, in Assisted Living: Current Issues in Facility Management and Resident Care. Edited by Namazi KH, Chafetz PK. Westport, CT, Auburn House, 2001, pp 105–126 Regier DA, Boyd JH, Burke JD, et al: One-month prevalence of mental disorders in the United States. Arch Gen Psychiatry 45:977–986, 1988 Regnier V, Pynoos J (eds): Housing the Aged. New York, Elsevier, 1992 Reisberg B, Ferris SH, deLeon MJ, et al: The Global Deterioration Scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. Am J Psychiatry 139:1136–1139, 1982 Riley CA: High-Access Home: Design and Decoration for Barrier-Free Living. New York, Rizzoli, 1999 Rosewarne R, Bruce A, McKenna M: Dementia programme effectiveness in longterm care. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 12:173–182, 1997 Salkeld G, Cumming RG, O’Neill E, et al: Cost-effectiveness of a home hazard reduction program to reduce falls among older persons. Aust N Z J Public Health 24:265–271, 2000 Schulz R, Heckhausen J: Aging, culture and control: setting a new research agenda. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 54:P139–P145, 1999 Sloane PD, Lindeman DA: The future of special care programs, in Special Care Programs for People With Dementia. Edited by Hoffman SB, Kaplan M. Baltimore, MD, Health Professions, 1996, pp 195–207 Sumaya IC, Rienzi BM, Deegan JF, et al: Bright light treatment decreases depression in institutionalized older adults: a placebo-controlled crossover study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 56:M356–M360, 2001 Szeto AYJ, Giles JA: Improving oral medication compliance with an electronic aid. IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology 58:48–54, 1997 Tideiksaar R: Preventing falls: home hazard checklists to help older patients help themselves. Geriatrics 41:26–29, 1986 Tideiksaar R: Falling in Old Age: Prevention and Management, 2nd Edition. New York, Springer, 1997 U.S. Bureau of the Census: 65+ in the United States (Current Population Reports, Special Studies, P23–190). Washington, DC, U.S. Government Printing Office, 1996
Structuring Environments
431
Van Someren EJW, Kessler A, Mirmiran M, et al: Indirect bright light improves circadian rest-activity rhythm disturbances in demented patients. Biol Psychiatry 41:955–963, 1997 Wagner L: Pharmacy automation gains ground: using technology to improve safety, efficiency. Provider 35:32–33, 2001 Warner ML: The Complete Guide to Alzheimer’s Proofing Your Home. West Lafayette, IN, Purdue University Press, 2000 Wilber KH, Machemer J: Balancing the competing values of freedom and safety in long-term dementia care: the secured perimeter program. J Ethics Law Aging 5:121–130, 1999
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER
14
Advances in the Molecular and Genetic Basis of Alzheimer’s Disease Roger N. Rosenberg, M.D.
Research in Alzheimer’s disease is proceeding at a rapid pace. Clinicians now have drugs at their disposal that partially ameliorate the cognitive and behavioral symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease and slow its clinical progression, presumably by augmenting cholinergic function. Of greater importance, advances in knowledge of the molecular and genetic aspects of Alzheimer’s disease are providing the tools to attack the molecular disease processes (Rosenberg 2000). Much of this knowledge comes from study of familial Alzheimer’s disease. Although only about 10% of Alzheimer’s disease cases have an autosomal dominant mode of transmission, 42% of Alzheimer’s disease patients seen in our clinic at the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center have an affected first-degree relative, partly due to the high prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease and partly to the inheritance of one or more risk factors. Although the early-onset autosomal dominant form of Alzheimer’s disease clearly results from specific genetic mutations, late-onset sporadic Alzheimer’s disease appears to result from multiple environmental (such as head trauma) and genetic influences (inheritance of the apolipoprotein E A4 allele). The importance of nongenetic factors in Alzheimer’s disease is underscored by the fact that only onethird of identical twins are concordant for the disease. Mutations in the amyloid precursor protein (APP) gene on chromosome 21q and of the presenilin 1 (PS1) and presenilin 2 (PS2) genes on chromosomes 14q and 1q, respectively, account for approximately one-half of early-onset forms of This chapter is adapted with permission from Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Inc., for use of original material published in Neurology, 54:2045–2054, 2000, “The Molecular and Genetic Basis of Alzheimer’s Disease: The End of the Beginning: The 2000 Wartenberg Lecture,” by Roger N. Rosenberg, M.D.
433
434
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
autosomal dominant inherited disease, as shown in Table 14–1. Additional loci on chromosomes 10, 12, and 19 have also been suggested. Although the functions of the proteins coded by the APP, PS1, and PS2 genes are not fully known, they all affect APP processing, causing a large increase in the self-aggregating 40– to 42–amino acid >-amyloid peptides. These peptides then coalesce under the influence of apolipoprotein E (ApoE) to form amyloid plaques. In all cases, whether early or late onset, familial or sporadic, there is eventual transformation of diffuse amyloid plaques to neuritic plaques, with a core of >-amyloid surrounded by degenerating neurites and inflammatory cells. Stimulated at least in part by >-amyloid (Lewis et al. 2001), neurofibrillary tangles composed of hyperphosphorylated tau protein develop, and loss of synapses and neurons occurs in the affected parts of the brain. The direct neuronal toxicity of >-amyloid in this pathological cascade causes activation of microglial cells (Meda et al. 1995) and induces the proinflammatory cytokines tumor necrosis factor = and interleukin 1> (IL-1>) in these cells (Dickson et al. 1993; Griffin et al. 1998), with subsequent formation of membrane-damaging free radicals (Markesbery 1999). TABLE 14–1. Genetic aspects of Alzheimer’s disease Chromosome 1 (autosomal dominant) early onset 14 (autosomal dominant) early onset 19 (risk factor) late onset 21 (Down syndrome)
Gene effects
Functional effects
Missense mutations of presenilin 2 gene
Increased synthesis and secretion of A>40, A>42
Missense mutations of presenilin 1 gene
Increased synthesis and secretion of A>40, A>42
Inheritance of ApoE A4 Increased >-amyloid allele aggregation destabilizes microtubules Reduplication of APP Overloads APP gene processing; plaques and tangles by age 40
Note. APP=amyloid precursor protein; A>40 =40–amino acid length >-amyloid protein; A>42 =42–amino acid length >-amyloid protein; ApoE=apolipoprotein E.
Amyloid Processing in Alzheimer’s Disease Alzheimer’s disease may be considered as a form of amyloidosis resulting from the abnormal processing of APP, a transmembrane protein whose function is unknown. There is evidence, however, that when released from the rest of the molecule, the intracellular portion of APP can activate or suppress gene expression in the nucleus (Cao and Südhof 2001). APP catab-
Advances in the Basis of Alzheimer’s Disease
435
olism is thought to involve three enzymes: =, >, and C secretase. As shown in Figure 14–1, APP is first cut enzymatically by = or > secretase. The products of these first cleavages are cut again by C secretase, yielding a soluble fragment from the portion of the molecule produced by =-C cleavage, and a self-aggregating insoluble fragment (>-amyloid 40–42) from the portion of the molecule produced by >-C cleavage. The C secretase apparently does not operate alone; recently a glycoprotein cofactor (nicastrin) was found (Yu et al. 2000). Additional peptides are needed for full activity, including aph-1=, aph-1>, and pan-2, in addition to presenilin 1 and nicastrin. Mutations in APP and in PS1 and PS2 lead to increased synthesis of >-amyloid from APP in the early-onset forms of Alzheimer’s disease (Tanzi et al. 1996). >-Amyloid peptides are produced by recycling endosomes after internalization of APP molecules from the cell surface (Yamazaki et al. 1996). >-Amyloid 40 is the most common form of >-amyloid in human cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) and plasma. >-Amyloid 42 aggregates into amyloid fibrils more rapidly and completely than does >-amyloid 40 and is contained in both early diffuse plaques and fully formed neuritic plaques (Selkoe 1998).
FIGURE 14–1.
Metabolic pathways for amyloid precursor protein.
436
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Deposition of >-amyloid precedes clinical symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease, and the brains of persons with Alzheimer’s disease contain increased numbers of >-amyloid plaques over time. The total concentration of >-amyloid in cortex is elevated early in the course of Alzheimer’s disease, correlates with cognitive decline, and appears before tangle formation occurs (Näslund et al. 2000). The latter observation suggests that abnormal processing of APP to >-amyloid or abnormal degradation and clearance of >-amyloid may be the initiating biochemical event in Alzheimer’s disease. Persons with Down syndrome have an extra copy of the APP gene on chromosome 21 (Masters et al. 1985), leading to overproduction of >-amyloid and the extensive development of plaques and tangles by age 40. Fibrillar >-amyloid is neurotoxic in vitro and in vivo (Geula at al. 1998). Accumulation of amyloidogenic peptides >-amyloid 40–42 may be due to increased activity of the proteases > and C secretase in familial cases due to APP and PS1 and PS2 gene mutations. Environmental or other nongenetic factors may also activate > and C secretase in sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. For example, PC12 neural cells in culture expressing amyloidogenic peptides can be differentiated further with phorbol esters, which increases nonamyloidogenic peptide synthesis (Baskin et al. 1992). There also may be decreased turnover of >-amyloid peptides. Neprilysin, a >-amyloid– degrading peptidase, is reported to be reduced in the brain in Alzheimer’s disease in regions enriched for amyloid plaques. Decreased degradation and clearance of >-amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease may be the result, partly due to reduced neprilysin activity (Iwata 2001; Yasojima et al. 2001). Another possible mechanism is downregulation of the protease = secretase, which cuts APP within the >-amyloid region (see Figure 14–1) and makes it soluble. The insulin-like degrading enzyme is also important for >-amyloid degradation and clearance.
Amyloid Precursor Protein Gene Mutations Missense mutations in the APP gene located on chromosome 21 cause some cases of early-onset inherited Alzheimer’s disease (Goate et al. 1991; Mullan et al. 1992; St. George-Hyslop et al. 1987). A mutation in this gene also causes hereditary cerebral hemorrhage with amyloidosis of the Dutch type (Haan et al. 1991; Levy et al. 1990). In this disease, there are plaques containing >-amyloid and deposits of >-amyloid in cerebral blood vessels that cause repeated cerebral hemorrhages. This mutation, near the proteolytic cleavage site inside the >-amyloid region, may decrease >-amyloid degradation. APP mutations in codon 717 cause early-onset autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease (Goate et al. 1991). A mutation at codon 692
Advances in the Basis of Alzheimer’s Disease
437
in exon 17 has been found in a family with both >-amyloid plaques and cerebral hemorrhages (Hendriks et al. 1992). At least seven different APP mutations in more than 20 families with early-onset dominantly inherited Alzheimer’s disease have been described; all missense mutations in the APP gene causing Alzheimer’s disease are situated at or near =, >, or C secretase sites and alter APP proteolysis (Rosenberg 2000).
The Presenilin Genes Although they are highly conserved throughout evolution, the function of the presenilin proteins is unknown. There is a high degree of homology of the human presenilin genes with sel-12, part of the Notch pathway in Caenorhabditis elegans for intercellular signaling during embryogenesis (Levitan et al. 1996). The PS1 gene on chromosome 14 encodes an integral membrane protein of 467 amino acids with eight transmembrane domains. PS1 was initially found to contain five different missense mutations that cosegregated with early-onset familial Alzheimer’s disease (Sherrington et al. 1995) and has since been associated with at least 90 different mutations in more than 80 families causing onset of Alzheimer’s disease between ages 35 and 65 years. All but two presenilin mutations are missense mutations, and most reside in regions that are conserved between PS1 and PS2 genes. Mutations in the PS1 gene account for about 25% of autosomal dominant cases of Alzheimer’s disease; the age at onset of Alzheimer’s disease in different PS1 families relates to the position and the type of amino acid substitution (Rosenberg 2000). The PS2 gene on chromosome 1 encodes a transmembrane protein of 448 amino acids with 67% overall homology with the PS1 protein amino acid sequence (Sherrington et al. 1995). A missense mutation in this gene causing Alzheimer’s disease has been found in descendants of a single German family that emigrated to Russia and later to the United States (Bird et al. 1998; Rogaeva et al. 1995). Two additional missense mutations have been found in an Italian and a Dutch family with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Rogaeva et al. 1995). The finding that missense mutations in the presenilin genes increase synthesis of >-amyloid and its own metabolic breakdown products by 50% (Lee et al. 1997) suggests that the presenilin genes are important for the regulation of APP processing. This is confirmed by the observation that PS1 transgenic mice and fibroblast cell cultures with PS1 mutations increase their synthesis of >-amyloid 42. The presenilins may either influence C secretase activity or may actually be C secretase. The presenilins may direct APP to specific intracellular endosomes containing C secretase, pro-
438
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
ducing >-amyloid 42 preferentially (Beyreuther and Masters 1997; Cruts and Van Broeckhoven 1998). PS1 mutations increase the activity of caspase (cysteine-dependent aspartate-specific proteases), induce apoptosis, and downregulate the important signaling pathway of the unfolded protein response in the endoplasmic reticulum (Katayama et al. 1999). The increased levels of >-amyloid in PS1 mutant cells might therefore result from retention of the unfolded APP in the endoplasmic reticulum due to the impaired protein-folding system.
Apolipoprotein E There are three alleles (A2, A3, and A4) of the gene located on chromosome 19 that encodes the cholesterol transport protein apolipoprotein E (ApoE). Inheritance of the A4 allele of this lipoprotein is a risk factor for late-onset Alzheimer’s disease, whereas ApoE A2 is associated with delayed onset of Alzheimer’s disease (Corder et al. 1993). ApoE A4 heterozygotes have a threefold increase in risk and homozygotes an eightfold increase in risk for developing Alzheimer’s disease by age 75 compared with ApoE A3 heterozygous individuals (Saunders et al. 1993). However, Alzheimer’s disease may occur in the presence or absence of ApoE A4. The question of whether inheritance of the ApoE A4 allele affects the clinical course of Alzheimer’s disease is controversial. In reviewing our own cases at the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, we were unable to find evidence of an association between the ApoE A4 allele and either the onset or rate of progression of Alzheimer’s disease (M.F. Weiner et al. 1999). The ApoE A4 allele has been associated with an earlier age at onset in families having APP mutations, with the exception of the 692 mutation, but no similar effect has been found in early-onset families having PS1 and PS2 gene mutations (Rosenberg 2000). The increased risk for Alzheimer’s disease associated with ApoE A4 may be related to its effect on cholesterol metabolism. Notkola et al. (1998) studied elderly men in a population-based investigation and found high plasma cholesterol to be an independent risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease (odds ratio, 3.1; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.2–8.5) after controlling for age and the presence of the ApoE A4 allele. Simons et al. (1998) showed that reducing the cholesterol level of hippocampal neurons in vitro by 70% with lovastatin and methyl->-cyclodextrin completely inhibited >-amyloid production while not affecting the generation of APP. The inhibition of >-amyloid formation was reversed by readding cholesterol to previously depleted cells. The requirement of cholesterol for >-amyloid formation suggests a possible link between cholesterol, >-amyloid, and Alzheimer’s
Advances in the Basis of Alzheimer’s Disease
439
disease. Depleting neuronal cholesterol may inhibit >-amyloid production by removing APP from cholesterol and sphingolipid-rich membrane microdomains and may reduce the ability of >-amyloid to self-aggregate (Simons et al. 2001). It is possible that the activity of > and C secretase is affected by cholesterol depletion. There is evidence that specific cholesterol-reducing agents may help prevent Alzheimer’s disease by lowering cholesterol, but also by lowering ApoE concentration, for example, by drugs inhibiting the action of 3-hydroxy-3-methylglutaryl coenzyme A (HMG CoA) reductase (Nishikawa et al. 1999; Nordoy et al. 1998). Evidence for a protective effect of HMG CoA inhibitors has been provided by Wolozin et al. (2000), who compared the prevalence of the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in three groups of individuals from Veterans Administration hospital records. They found that the cohort of individuals receiving HMG CoA–inhibiting drugs had a prevalence 60%–73% lower than that of either the total patient population or compared with patients taking other medications typically used to treat hypertension or cardiovascular disease. In another retrospective study, persons age 50 or older who were prescribed HMG CoA inhibitors had substantially lower risk of developing dementia, independent of the presence or absence of untreated hyperlipidemia or exposure to non– HMG CoA lipid-lowering agents. The adjusted risk ratio for those receiving HMG CoA inhibitors was 0.29 (CI, 0.13–0.63; P=0.002) (Jick et al. 2000).
Transgenic Models of Alzheimer’s Disease Several human APP genes have been transfected into mouse lines. The human mutant V717F APP gene transfected into a mouse line causes these mice to express high levels of human APP 770, which selectively increases the generation of >-amyloid 42 (Games et al. 1995). These transgenic mice progressively develop many of the pathological features of Alzheimer’s disease, including numerous extracellular >-amyloid deposits, neuritic plaques, synapse loss, astrocytosis, and microgliosis. The development of amyloid plaques in the V717F transgenic mouse depends on the expression of ApoE. Knock-out mice lacking an ApoE gene developed only small amounts of cortical and hippocampal amyloid plaques; they were diffuse plaques and did not display the thioflavine-S fluorescence of mature neuritic plaques (Bales et al. 1997). Holtzman et al. (1999, 2000) found that human ApoE A3 and ApoE A4 suppressed >-amyloid deposition at 9 months of age in the APP V717F+/– transgenic mouse compared with mice lacking ApoE. By 15 months of age, expression of human ApoE A3 and A4 in this mouse model expressing APP V717F+/– and mouse ApoE–/– resulted in sig-
440
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
nificant fibrillar >-amyloid deposits. There were at least 10 times more fibrillar deposits in mice expressing human ApoE A4, suggesting that a critical concentration of ApoE and >-amyloid is required for mature fibrillar >-amyloid to be deposited as plaque material. There also appears to be a relationship between hypercholesterolemia and >-amyloid deposition in the transgenic mouse, with high levels of cholesterol being related to increased deposition of plaque (Refolo et al. 2000). This would support the possibility that HMG CoA reductase inhibitors (statins) might influence the development of Alzheimer’s disease by reducing cholesterol synthesis in brain
Lipoprotein Receptor–Related Protein The lipoprotein receptor–related protein (LRP) is one of the low-density lipoprotein receptors. Reduced LRP expression correlates with increased Alzheimer’s disease susceptibility and younger age at disease onset (Kang et al. 1997). The LRP is a receptor that mediates the internalization and degradation of substances involved in the metabolic pathways of lipoproteins. Furthermore, eight ligands potentially involved in Alzheimer’s disease interact with the LRP. They include cholesterol, APP, =2-macroglobulin, ApoE, the cytosolic adaptor protein FE65, Mena, Dab1, and Reelin (Cooper and Howell 1999; Hyman et al. 2000). APP-ApoE complexes binding to LRP may be an early event in the intracellular routing of APP and may affect the ratio of nonamyloidogenic APP processing by = secretase to amyloidogenic APP processing by the > and C secretases. The =2-macroglobulin molecule binds both to >-amyloid and the LRP (Blacker et al. 1998). FE65 binds both to the LRP and APP. The LRP contains the NPxY motif found in the cytoplasmic tails of many cell surface receptors. NpxY is required for the clustering of the receptor in the coated pits and subsequent receptor endocytosis. Thus, the LRP receptor interacting with cholesterol, APP, >-amyloid, =2-macroglobulin, and ApoE may be involved in their internalization and subsequent trafficking of molecules to specific subcellular compartments (Trommsdorff et al. 1998). FE65, the peptide promoting APP and LRP binding, in turn binds the protein Mena. An additional protein, Dab1, binds both to APP and to the LRP. As a result, there is a complex of proteins involved in cell transduction that may determine the nature of APP processing and >-amyloid synthesis. Kang et al. (2000) have demonstrated that both the soluble >-amyloid 40 and the pathogenic >-amyloid 42 are transported from plasma by the LRP pathway and postulate that the reduction of LRP activity that normally accompanies aging may predispose to Alzheimer’s disease. They also note that the risk
Advances in the Basis of Alzheimer’s Disease
441
for or protection from Alzheimer’s disease associated with the LRP is greatest among persons without the ApoE A4 allele, which suggests that the LRP pathway cannot overcome the pathogenic effects of ApoE A4. Beffert et al. (1999) found the LRP C allele to be more frequent in autopsy-confirmed Alzheimer’s disease subjects (86.7%) than in control subjects (82.8%) (odds ratio, 1.34; 95% CI, 1.16–1.54; P<0.0001), but this weak correlation with Alzheimer’s disease has led investigators to seek another locus on chromosome 12 as a risk factor. A locus near the gene for the LRP ligand =2-macroglobulin is a possible candidate (Wu et al. 1998).
Tau Protein The pathogenesis of neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) in Alzheimer’s disease has yet to be satisfactorily explained. The intraneuronal NFTs of Alzheimer’s disease are hyperphosphorylated, microtubule-associated tau protein aggregated into paired helical filaments. Tau normally binds to and stabilizes the microtubular protein that forms the cytoskeleton of axons. When tau is dissociated from its microtubular binding domains, it auto-polymerizes, is phosphorylated, and becomes unable to rebind to microtubules. A link has been established between >-amyloid deposition and NFT formation. Lewis et al. (2001) crossed transgenic mice expressing a mutant tau protein (which develop NFTs) with mice expressing mutant APP and found increased NFT formation in the limbic system and olfactory cortex. By injecting >-amyloid 42 into the brains of mutant tau transgenic mice, Götz et al. (2001) were able to increase fivefold the number of NFTs in cell bodies within the amygdala from where neurons projected into the injection sites. A possible trigger for formation of paired helical filaments is a shift between neuronal protein kinases and phosphatases that control the phosphorylation state of tau. Protein phosphatase 2A (PP2A), located on microtubules, has been shown to regulate tau phosphorylation in vivo (Sontag et al. 1996), and a decrease in PP2A activity has been observed in the brains of individuals with Alzheimer’s disease (Gong et al. 1995). In addition, alterations in the microtubule binding site of tau due to mutation or posttranslational modification might compromise the ability of PP2A to bind to and thereby dephosphorylate tau (Sontag et al. 1999). Wilhelmsen et al. (1994) found that mutations in the tau gene on chromosome 17 cause a frontotemporal dementia with Parkinsonism (FTDP-17), but none of the many tau mutations found in families with autosomal dominant FTDP-17 (see Chapter 3 of this book) have been found in persons with Alzheimer’s disease. Hiesberger et al. (1999) have studied double-knock-out mice lacking low-density lipoprotein (LDL) receptor proteins, VLDLR and ApoE-R2.
442
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
In this strain, there is an increase of hyperphosphorylated tau, linking VLDLR and ApoE-R2 expression to phosphorylated tau. Thus, tau hyperphosphorylation may be regulated directly by the ApoE-R2–VLDLR signaling pathway, contributing to the disruption of microtubules. A subtle alteration of cellular signals initiated by members of the LDL receptor gene family might affect the stability of the neuronal cytoskeleton and promote neuronal cell death. Hiesberger et al. (1999) have also shown that both VLDLR and ApoE-R2 can bind Reelin on their extracellular domains. Inside the cell, the Reelin signal is received by the cytosolic adapter protein Dab1, which contains a protein interaction domain that binds to the cytoplasmic tail of the VLDLR. Reelin stimulates tyrosine phosphorylation of Dab1 and thereby decreases Dab1. The action of Reelin on VLDLR and ApoE-R2 receptors modulates levels of cytoplasmic Dab1 tyrosine phosphorylation and tau phosphorylation. This has been demonstrated in the mouse mutants lacking Reelin, which have increased tau phosphorylation (Hiesberger et al. 1999). It therefore seems likely that tau hyperphosphorylation is regulated by the Reelin–VLDLR–ApoE-R2– Dab1 signaling pathway. When altered, this pathway may induce tau hyperphosphorylation and neurofibrillary tangle formation. The activity of cyclin-dependent kinase 5 (Cdk5) is also increased in the brain in Alzheimer’s disease (Patrick et al. 1999). This enzyme is partly responsible for the hyperphosphorylation of tau, reducing the ability of tau to associate with microtubules. Cdk5 activation requires that it associate with its plasma membrane regulatory subunit, p35. A truncated form of p35, p25, also accumulates in neurons in Alzheimer’s disease brain in direct proportion to the increased Cdk5 kinase activity. This p25-Cdk5 complex hyperphosphorylates tau. The plasma membrane location of p35 also suggests a possible linkage of altered tau phosphorylation with Reelin.
Amyloid Biomarkers Possible biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease include elevated plasma levels of >-amyloid 42 in individuals over age 65 (Mayeux et al. 1999). Elevated plasma >-amyloid 42 levels fall as individuals develop Alzheimer’s disease; CSF levels of >-amyloid 42 fall with disease progression as well. The fall in CSF >-amyloid 42 levels is associated with a rise in tau in CSF, possibly related to increased neurofibrillary turnover and clearance in the CSF. The mean ratio of the 120- to 130-kD APP isoform to the 110-kD APP isoform in blood platelets in patients with Alzheimer’s disease has been shown to be significantly lower than that of control subjects (Rosenberg at al. 1997). APP processing by platelets differs in persons with Alzheimer’s disease
Advances in the Basis of Alzheimer’s Disease
443
from control subjects. This ratio decreases as the disease progresses, suggesting that the APP isoform ratio might be a severity marker for Alzheimer’s disease progression (Baskin et al. 2000). Unpublished data from our group indicate partial normalization of the mean ratio of the 120- to 130-kD APP isoform to the 110-kD isoform in Alzheimer’s disease patients treated for 6 weeks with a statin drug, suggesting possible use of this biomarker for monitoring drug effect.
Protective Genes Just as the ApoE A2 allele appears to delay the onset of Alzheimer’s disease, there may be other genetically determined factors that protect against Alzheimer’s disease. Hendrie et al. (1993) reported that Alzheimer’s disease is rare in the Cree Indian tribe of Canada compared with an urban white population. Henderson et al. (2002) found lower frequency of the ApoE A4 allele in Chocktaw Indians. We found among Cherokee Indians that as the genetic degree of Cherokee Indian ancestry increased, the representation of Alzheimer’s disease decreased (Rosenberg et al. 1996). The low incidence of Alzheimer’s disease in high genetic Cherokee Indians was not affected by the ApoE A4 allele. These data suggest that complex protective and disease-causal genetic factors may be involved in a variable manner in Alzheimer’s disease. If Native American tribes are descended from southeastern Asian peoples, a lower prevalence rate for Alzheimer’s disease in the Chinese of Hong Kong compared with white populations might be noteworthy (Mak et al. 1996). Also, Pericak-Vance et al. (1996) have reported a low prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease in another outbred population, the Amish of Indiana, and concluded that this finding is only partially explained by the decreased frequency of the ApoE A4 allele in this population.
Molecular Linkages APP processing produces >-amyloid 40 and >-amyloid 42 molecules that polymerize to form plaques. As noted earlier, there is a direct relationship between >-amyloid burden and cognitive impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. The microtubule-stabilizing protein tau is hyperphosphorylated to form neurofibrillary tangles. ApoE A4 promotes increased plaque and tangle formation in the Alzheimer’s disease brain, and in sporadic Alzheimer’s disease, degradation and clearance of >-amyloid 42 is impaired. How these various molecular events interrelate is uncertain. As mentioned earlier, ApoE knock-out mice having the human APP717 codon mutation produced minimal amyloid plaque compared with wild type mice.
444
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
If almost no amyloid is produced when there is no ApoE, then dysregulated cell surface ApoE-R2 receptors might alter amyloidogenesis, resulting in excessive amyloid plaque formation. ApoE A4 binding to ApoE-R2 may facilitate the altered membrane signal transduction. Altered signaling via this pathway may direct transported >-amyloid internalized by way of the LRP or released from the endoplasmic reticulum into the incorrect subset of endosomes. >-Amyloid that is directed into the wrong endosomal circuit might be secreted by cells and subsequently form plaques. Thus, this cell surface membrane receptor model of Alzheimer’s disease provides a potential linkage of plaque and tangle formation through alteration of signal transduction from cell surface ApoE-R2, VLDL, and LRP receptors to cytosolic targets, including Dab1, which activates nonreceptor tyrosine kinases and their substrates. The downstream targets of this hypothetical kinase cascade initiated by Dab1 are unknown but may be concerned with >-amyloid trafficking. Tomiyama et al. (1999) have found evidence for endocytosis of ApoE– >-amyloid complexes. These lysosomal ApoE–>-amyloid aggregates may accumulate, alter APP metabolism, and produce neuronal degeneration. With neuronal breakdown and death, intracellular >-amyloid could contribute to extracellular deposits of amyloid. All proteins genetically associated with Alzheimer’s disease are linked through a single pathway to the LRP. Thus, the LRP may be central to Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis (Hyman et al. 2000). Transgenic mice overexpressing PS1 or PS2 mutations show decreased levels of LRP expression in neurons in which PS1 and the LRP are colocalized or coexpressed. Thus, downregulation of the LRP would decrease internalization of =2-macroglobulin–>-amyloid complexes and increase serum >-amyloid levels, as occurs in PS1 transgenic mice and in humans with PS1 mutations (Van Uden et al. 1999). Linkages between cell membrane receptors (LRP, ApoE, and VLDLR, among others) suggest that amyloidogenesis and tau hyperphosphorylation may be tied together. Common polymorphisms found at many points in the genome may be risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease, including the LRP, =2-macroglobulin, ApoE, and FE65, among others (Tanzi 1999, 2000). These potential additive risk factors combined with environmental risk factors could trigger the chain of events that result in Alzheimer’s disease.
Molecular Therapy Targets for molecular therapy include increased >-amyloid production and aggregation, decreased degradation and clearance of >-amyloid, and block-
Advances in the Basis of Alzheimer’s Disease
445
age of >-amyloid–induced toxicity. One possible approach is the use of a vaccine. However, the amyloid in plaques has long been regarded as insoluble in vivo, being resistant to harsh detergents and requiring formic acid treatment to dissolve in vitro. For this reason, the notion of creating an anti-antibody to amyloid to reduce amyloid burden did not seem a reasonable approach. How would macrophages formerly unable to digest amyloid become able to do so? It was found that an antibody raised against the N-terminal region of >-amyloid led in cell cultures to disaggregation of amyloid fibrils and partial restoration or solubility (Solomon et al. 1997). More recently, monthly immunization for 11 months with injections of >-amyloid 42 was found to prevent the development of >-amyloid plaque formation, neuritic dystrophy, and astrogliosis in mice transfected with the V717F mutant human APP gene (Schenk et al. 1999). Partial regression and clearance of plaques was seen in animals that had already developed them; substantial reduction of reactive gliosis also occurred. Similar results have been reported in V717F transgenic mice by chronic intranasal administration of synthetic >-amyloid, which was applied weekly for 7 months (H.L. Weiner et al. 2000). There is evidence in these transgenic mice of both age-related progressive spatial learning deficit that correlates with plaque burden and ageindependent learning deficit (the latter in spatial learning but not object recognition). The age-independent learning deficit may be due to glucose hypometabolism or smaller hippocampal size in these animals (Chen et al. 2000). In another transgenic mouse model, 8 months of immunization therapy prevented learning and age-related deficits and reduced amyloid burden in animals that had already undergone plaque formation (Morgan et al. 2000). Whether the cognitive deficits that occur in mice are analogous to the cognitive deficits that occur in Alzheimer’s disease is unknown. Safety and efficacy trials of the injectable vaccine were initiated in humans but have been stopped because of CNS inflammatory complications (see also Chapter 15 of this book). Another issue is that these transgenic mouse models do not show neurofibrillary tangles. It may be sufficient to prevent or reduce >-amyloid plaque formation in these mice, but this may not be adequate to reverse tau pathology in humans. In another mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease (Tg2576) that overexpresses the 695–amino acid form of APP that contains a double APP mutation found in familial Alzheimer’s disease, administration of the nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drug (NSAID) ibuprofen reduced >-amyloid deposition, dystrophic neurites, and microglial infiltration of plaques (Lim et al. 2000). The investigators proposed that this effect, consistent with epidemiological findings suggesting neuroprotection by NSAIDs (Stewart et al. 1997), may be due to attenuated production of inflammatory cyto-
446
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
kines such as IL-1> and products of reactive glia such as ApoE, which is associated with increased >-amyloid deposition. Ibuprofen and other NSAIDs inhibit the formation of cyclooxygenase 2, a prostaglandin precursor, which has been found to increase in the hippocampal formation as a function of the clinical progression of Alzheimer’s disease (Ho et al. 2001). Increased production of >-amyloid can be related to overexpression of both > and C secretase. The > secretase molecule has been identified and cloned. It is a pepsin-insensitive transmembrane aspartyl protease (BACE, Asp2) (Vassar et al. 1999). Identification of this >-site APP-cleaving enzyme enables the development of agents to inhibit the expression of this gene or the protease it encodes. Inhibition of C secretase has been achieved in vitro and in vivo with N-[N-(3,5-difluorophenaceytl)-L-alanyl]-Sphenylglycine t-butyl ester. Treatment with this substance reduces brain >-amyloid in a dose-dependent manner (Dovey et al. 2001). PS1 may be a cofactor for C secretase or may be C secretase (Wolfe et al. 1999), and both PS1 and PS2 have C secretase activities (Wolfe et al. 1999). Both > secretase and C secretase inhibitors will prevent >-amyloid synthesis and plaque formation. There is a threefold increase of soluble >-amyloid in the brain in Alzheimer’s disease, with a higher proportion of >-amyloid 42 in the soluble fractions, possibly representing a more neurotoxic intracellular pool (McLean et al. 1999). Reduction of soluble >-amyloid might block disease progression as well. Therapies may also be directed at the aggregation of amyloid into toxic fibrils. A small peptide that inhibits >-amyloid fibrillogenesis, disassembles preformed fibrils in vitro, and prevents neuronal death induced by fibrils in cell culture has been described by Soto et al. (1998). This peptide reduced >-amyloid deposition in vivo and blocked the formation of amyloid fibrils in a rat model. >-Amyloid induces apoptosis in part through inducing caspase 12 (Nakagawa et al. 2000). Caspase 12 is thus another potential target for protease inhibitor therapy. Polymorphisms in IL-1= IL-1> genes are strongly associated with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Nicoll et al. 2000). These Alzheimer’s disease–associated polymorphisms are common population polymorphisms that may be susceptibility factors (Tanzi 2000). Transforming growth factor >1, a key regulator of brain responses to injury and inflammation, promotes microglial >-amyloid clearance and reduces plaque formation in transgenic mice (Wyss-Coray et al. 2001). There also may be differences in sporadic and genetically determined Alzheimer’s disease that will involve different treatment. Because there is not a differential expression of APP mRNA isoforms in Alzheimer’s disease, posttranslational events in APP metabolism appear to be the most important in amyloidogenesis in Alzheimer’s disease (Panegyres et al. 2000).
Advances in the Basis of Alzheimer’s Disease
447
Thus, research in therapies that promote >-amyloid degradation and clearance from brain may be more important in treating sporadic Alzheimer’s disease than those related to differential gene expression. An understanding of risk factors including ApoE and the LRP may provide insight into sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. The relationship of the =2-macroglobulin (A2M) gene on chromosome 12 to Alzheimer’s disease is supported by some studies and not others (Dodel et al. 2000; Gibson et al. 2000). However, many of the genetic polymorphisms associated with Alzheimer’s disease, including A2M, are related to the LRP or LRP ligands. It is possible that >-amyloid is cleared by forming A2M complexes via an LRP-dependent mechanism and alteration of this process due to A2M polymorphisms might lead to reduced >-amyloid turnover. The finding that >-amyloid burden correlates more strongly with dementia severity than do the number of plaques adds to the rationale for inhibiting APP processing with >-C secretase inhibitors early in the disease. Statins may help prevent Alzheimer’s disease by reducing brain cholesterol or its precursors. Low cholesterol content in turn activates = secretase and thus reduces >-amyloid synthesis. Thus, lowered cholesterol content in neural cells promotes the nonamyloidogenic pathway (Kojro et al. 2001). Statins are clinically available, are reliable, and have minimal side effects. Their role in Alzheimer’s disease prevention may become increasingly important.
Conclusions Explorations in the molecular biology and genetics of Alzheimer’s disease have led to a number of potential therapeutic approaches, including immunization, secretase inhibitors, caspase inhibitors, statins, and compounds to disaggregate >-amyloid. Immunization therapy is under investigation; the secretase inhibitors have advanced to animal models of Alzheimer’s disease. Based on epidemiological and immunological evidence, treatment directed at the inflammatory cascade may be useful for both prevention and treatment. As the interaction between cholesterol metabolism and Alzheimer’s disease unfolds, there is evidence that substances reducing cholesterol and ApoE levels may have an impact on plaque formation in Alzheimer’s disease. Testing patients with Alzheimer’s disease for high-risk IL-1 polymorphisms may also be a means to develop genotype-specific guided treatments.
References Bales KR, Verina T, Dodel RC, et al: Lack of apolipoprotein E dramatically reduces amyloid >-peptide deposition. Nat Genet 17:263–264,1997
448
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Baskin F, Rosenberg R, Davis R: Morphological differentiation and proteoglycan synthesis regulate Alzheimer amyloid precursor protein processing in PC12 and human astrocyte cultures. J Neurosci Res 32:274–279, 1992 Baskin F, Rosenberg RN, Iyer L, et al: Platelet APP isoform ratios correlate with declining cognition in AD. Neurology 54:1907–1909, 2000 Beffert U, Arguin C, Poirier J: The polymorphism in exon 3 of the low-density lipoprotein receptor-related protein gene is weakly associated with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett 259:29–32, 1999 Beyreuther K, Masters CL: Serpins on the road to dementia and death. Nat Med 3:723–725, 1997 Bird TD, Lampe TH, Nemens EJ, et al: Familial Alzheimer’s disease in American descendants of the Volga Germans: probable genetic founder effect. Ann Neurol 23:25–31, 1988 Blacker D, Wilcox MA, Laird NM, et al: =-2 macroglobulin is genetically associated with Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Genet 19:357–360, 1998 Cao X, Südhof TC: A transcriptively active complex of APP with Fe65 and histone acetyltransferase Tip60. Science 293:115–120, 2001 Chen G, Chen KS, Knox J, et al: A learning deficit related to age and beta-amyloid plaques in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 408:975–978, 2000 Cooper JA, Howell BW: Lipoprotein receptors: signaling functions in the brain? Cell 97:671–674, 1999 Corder E, Saunders A, Strittmatter W, et al: Genetic dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset families. Science 261:921–923, 1993 Cruts M, Van Broeckhoven C: Presenilin mutations in Alzheimer’s disease. Hum Mutat 11:183–190, 1998 Dickson DW, Lee SC, Mattiace LA, et al: Microglia and cytokines in neurological disease, with special reference to AIDS and Alzheimer’s disease. Glia 7:75–83, 1993 Dodel RC, Du Y, Bales KR, et al: =2 macroglobulin and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 54:438–442, 2000 Dovey HF, John V, Anderson JP, et al: Functional gamma-secretase inhibitors reduce beta-amyloid peptide levels in brain. J Neurochem 76:173–181, 2001 Games D, Adams D, Alessandrini R, et al: Alzheimer-type neuropathology in transgenic mice overexpressing V717F >-amyloid precursor protein. Nature 373:523–527, 1995 Geula C Wu C-K, Daroff D, et al: Aging renders the brain vulnerable to amyloid >-protein neurotoxicity. Nat Med 4:827–831, 1998 Gibson AM, Singleton AB, Smith G, et al: Lack of association of the =2-macroglobulin locus on chromosome 12 in AD. Neurology 54:433–438, 2000 Goate A, Chartier-Harlin M-C, Mullan M, et al: Segregation of a missense mutation in the amyloid precursor protein gene with familial Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 349:704–706, 1991 Gong C-X, Shaikh S, Wang J-Z, et al: Phosphatase activity toward abnormally phosphorylated tau: decrease in Alzheimer disease brain. J Neurochem 65:732–738, 1995
Advances in the Basis of Alzheimer’s Disease
449
Götz J, Chen F, van Dorpe J, et al: Formation of neurofibrillary tangles in P301L tau transgenic mice induced by A> 42 fibrils. Science 293:1491–1495, 2001 Griffin WS, Sheng JG, Royston MC, et al: Glial-neuronal interactions in Alzheimer’s disease: the potential role of a “cytokine cycle” in disease progression. Brain Pathol 8:65–72, 1998 Haan J, Hardy JA, Roos RAAC: Hereditary cerebral hemorrhage with amyloidosis– Dutch type: its importance for Alzheimer research. Trends Neurosci 14:231– 234, 1991 Henderson JN, Crook R, Crook J, et al: Apolipoprotein E4 and tau allele frequencies among Choctaw Indians. Neurosci Lett 324:77–79, 2002 Hendrie H, Hall K, Dillay N: Alzheimer’s disease is rare in Cree. Int Psychogeriatr 5:5–14, 1993 Hendriks L, vanDuijn CM, Cras P, et al: Presenile dementia and cerebral hemorrhage linked to a mutation at codon 692 of the >-amyloid precursor protein gene. Nat Genet 1:218–221, 1992 Hiesberger T, Trommsdorff M, Howell B, et al: Direct binding of Reelin to VLDL receptor and ApoE receptor 2 induces tyrosine phosphorylation of disabled-1 and modulates tau phosphorylation. Neuron 24: 481–489, 1999 Ho L, Purohit D, Haroutunian V, et al: Neuronal cyclooxygenase 2 expression in the hippocampal formation as a function of the clinical progression of Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 58:487–492, 2001 Holtzman DM, Bales KR, Wu S, et al: Expression of human apolipoprotein E reduces amyloid-> deposition in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. J Clin Invest 103:R15–R21, 1999 Holtzman DM, Bales KR, Tenkova T, et al: Neuritic degeneration in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease: requirement for ApoE and isoform-dependent effects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:2892–2897, 2000 Hyman BT, Strickland D, Robeck GW: Role of the low-density lipoprotein receptor-related protein (LPR) in >-amyloid metabolism and Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 57:646–650, 2000 Iwata N, Tsubuki S, Takaki Y, et al: Metabolic regulation of brain Abeta by neprilysin. Science 292:1550–1552, 2001 Jick H, Zornberg GL, Jick SS, et al: Statins and the risk of dementia. Lancet 356:1627–1631, 2000 Kang DE, Saitoh T, Chen X, et al: Genetic association of the low-density lipoprotein receptor-related protein gene (LPR), and apolipoprotein E receptor, with late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 49:56–61, 1997 Kang DE, Pietrzik CU, Baum L, et al: Modulation of amyloid beta-protein clearance and Alzheimer’s disease susceptibility by the LDL receptor-related protein pathway. J Clin Invest 106:1159–1166, 2000 Katayama T, Imaizumi K, Sato N, et al: Presenilin-1 mutations downregulate the signaling pathway of the unfolded-protein response. Nat Cell Biol 1:479–485, 1999 Kojro E, Gimpl G, Lammich S, et al: Low cholesterol stimulates the nonamyloidogenic pathway by its effect on the alpha-secretase. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:5815–5820, 2001
450
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Lee MK, Borchelt DR, Kim G, et al: Hyperaccumulation of FAD-linked presenilin 1 variants in vivo. Nat Med 3:756–759, 1997 Levy E, Carman MD, Fernandez-Madrid IJ, et al: Mutation of the Alzheimer’s disease amyloid gene in hereditary cerebral hemorrhage, Dutch type. Science 248:1124–1126, 1990 Lewis J, Dickson DW, Lin WL, et al: Enhanced neurofibrillary degeneration in transgenic mice expressing mutant tau and APP. Science 293:1487–1491, 2001 Mak Y, Chiu H, Woo J: Apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer’s disease in Hong Kong elderly Chinese. Neurology 146:146–149, 1996 Markesbery WR: The role of oxidative stress in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 56:1449–1452, 1999 Masters CL, Simms G, Weinman NA, et al: Amyloid plaque core protein in Alzheimer disease and Down syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 82:4245–4249, 1985 Mayeux R, Tang M-X, Jacobs DM, et al: Plasma amyloid >-peptide 1–42 and incipient Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 46:412–416, 1999 McLean CA, Cherny RA, Fraser FW, et al: Soluble pool of Abeta amyloid as a determinant of severity of neurodegeneration in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 46:860–866, 1999 Meda L, Cassatella MA, Szendrei GI, et al: Activation of microglial cells by betaamyloid protein and interferon-gamma. Nature 374:647–650, 1995 Morgan D, Diamond DM, Gottschall PE, et al: Abeta peptide vaccination prevents memory loss in an animal model of Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 408:982–985, 2000 Mullan M, Crawford F, Axelman K, et al: A pathogenic mutation for probable Alzheimer’s disease in the APP gene at the N-terminus of beta amyloid. Nat Genet 1:345–347, 1992 Nakagawa T, Zhu H, Morishima N, et al: Caspase-12 mediates endoplasmicreticulum–specific apoptosis and cytotoxicity by amyloid-beta. Nature 403:98– 103, 2000 Näslund A, Haroutunian V, Mohs RC, et al: Correlation between elevated levels of amyloid beta-peptide in the brain and cognitive decline. JAMA 288:1571– 1577, 2000 Nicoll JA, Mrak RE, Graham DI, et al: Association of interleukin-1 gene polymorphisms with Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 47:365–368, 2000 Nishikawa O, Mune M, Mivano M, et al: Effect of simvastatin on the lipid profile of hemodialysis patients. Kidney Int 71 (suppl):S219–S221, 1999 Nordoy A, Bonaa KH, Lilsen H, et al: Effects of simvastatin and omega-3 fatty acids on plasma lipoproteins and lipid peroxidation in patients with combined hyperlipidaemia. J Intern Med 243:163–170, 1998 Notkola IL, Sulkava R, Pekkanin J, et al: Total serum cholesterol, apoliprotein E epsilon 4 allele, and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroepidemiology 7:14–20, 1998 Panegyres PK, Zafiris-Toufexis K, Kakulas BA: Amyloid precursor protein gene isoforms in Alzheimer’s disease and other neurodegenerative disorders. J Neurol Sci 173:81–92, 2000
Advances in the Basis of Alzheimer’s Disease
451
Patrick GN, Zukerberg L, Nikolic M, et al: Conversion of p35 to p25 deregulates Cdk5 activity and promotes neurodegeneration. Nature 402:615–622, 1999 Pericak-Vance MS, Johnson C, Rimmler JB, et al: Alzheimer’s disease and apolipoprotein epsilon4 allele in an Amish population. Ann Neurol 39:700–704, 1996 Refolo LM, Pappolla MA, Malester B, et al: Hypercholesterolemia accelerates the Alzheimer’s amyloid pathology in a transgenic mouse model. Neurobiol Dis 7:321–331, 2000 Rogaeva EI, Sherrington R, Rogaeva EA, et al: Familial Alzheimer’s disease in kindreds with missense mutations in a gene on chromosome 1 related to the Alzheimer’s disease type 3 gene. Nature 376:775–778, 1995 Rosenberg RN: The molecular and genetic basis of AD: the end of the beginning: the 2000 Wartenberg lecture. Neurology 54:2045–2054, 2000 Rosenberg RN, Richter RW, Risser RC, et al: Genetic factors for the development of Alzheimer’s disease in the Cherokee Indian. Arch Neurol 53:997–1000, 1996 Rosenberg RN, Baskin F, Fosmire JA, et al: Altered amyloid protein processing in platelets of patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 54:139–144, 1997 Saunders A, Strittmatter W, Schmechel D, et al: Association of apolipoprotein E allele epsilon4 with late-onset familial and sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 43:1467–1472, 1993 Schenk D, Barbour R, Junn W, et al: Immunization with amyloid-beta attenuates Alzheimer-disease–like pathology in the PDAPP mouse. Nature 400:173–177, 1999 Selkoe DJ: Molecular pathology of Alzheimer’s disease: the role of amyloid, in Blue Books of Practical Neurology. The Dementias. Edited by Growdon JH, Rossor MN. Boston, MA, Butterworth-Heinemann, 1998, pp 257–283 Sherrington R, Rogaev EI, Liang Y, et al: Cloning of a gene bearing missense mutations in early onset familial Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 375:754–760, 1995 Simons M, Keller P, DeStrooper B, et al: Cholesterol depletion inhibits the generation of beta-amyloid in hippocampal neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:6460–6464, 1998 Simons M, Keller P, Dichgans J, et al: Cholesterol and Alzheimer’s disease: is there a link? Neurology 57:1089–1093, 2001 Solomon B, Koppel R, Frankel D, et al: Disaggregation of Alzheimer beta-amyloid by site-directed mAb. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 94:4109–4112, 1997 Sontag E, Nunbhakdi-Craig V, Lee G, et al: Regulation of the phosphorylation state and microtubule-binding activity of tau by protein phosphatase 2A. Neuron 17:1201–1207, 1996 Sontag E, Nunbhakdi-Craig V, Lee G, et al: Molecular interactions among protein phosphatase 2A, tau, and microtubules. J Biol Chem 274:25490–25498, 1999 Soto C, Sigurdsson EM, Morrelli L, et al: Beta-sheet breaker peptides inhibit fibrillogenesis in a rat brain model of amyloidosis: implications for Alzheimer’s therapy. Nat Med 4:822–826, 1998 St. George-Hyslop PH, Tanzi RE, Polinski PJ, et al: The genetic defect causing familial Alzheimer’s disease maps on chromosome 21. Science 235:885–890, 1987 Stewart WF, Kawas C, Corrada M, et al: Risk of Alzheimer’s disease and duration of NSAID use. Neurology 4:626–632, 1997
452
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Tanzi RE: A genetic dichotomy model for the inheritance of Alzheimer's disease and common age-related disorders. J Clin Invest 104:1175–1179, 1999 Tanzi RE: Alzheimer’s disease risk and the interleukin-1 genes. Ann Neurol 47:283–285, 2000 Tanzi RE, Dovacs DM, Kim TW, et al: The gene defects responsible for familial Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Dis 3:159–168, 1996 Tomiyama T, Corder EH, Mori H: Molecular pathogenesis of apolipoprotein E– mediated amyloidosis in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Cell Mol Life Sci 56:268–279, 1999 Trommsdorff M, Borg J-P, Margolis B, et al: Interaction of cytosolic adapter proteins with neuronal apolipoprotein E receptors and the amyloid precursor protein. J Biol Chem 273:33556–33560, 1998 Van Uden E, Carlson G, St. George-Hyslop P, et al: Aberrant presenilin-1 expression down regulates LDL receptor–related protein (LRP): is LRP central to Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis? Mol Cell Neurosci 14:129–140, 1999 Vassar R, Bennett B, Babu-Kahn S, et al: Beta-secretase cleavage of Alzheimer’s amyloid precursor protein by the transmembrane aspartic protease BACE. Science 286:735–741, 1999 Weiner HL, Lemere CA, Maron R, et al: Nasal administration of amyloid-beta peptide decreases cerebral amyloid burden in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 48:567–579, 2000 Weiner MF, Vega GL, Risser RC, et al: Apolipoprotein E4, other risk factors, and course of Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 45:633–638, 1999 Wilhelmsen KC, Lynch T, Pavlou E, et al: Localization of disinhibition-dementiaparkinsonism-amyotrophy complex to 17q21-22. Am J Hum Genet 55:1159– 1165, 1994 Wolfe MS, Xia W, Ostaszewski BL, et al: Two transmembrane aspartates in presenilin-1 required for presenilin endoproteolysis and gamma-secretase activity. Nature 398:513–517, 1999 Wolozin B, Kellman W, Rousseau P, et al: Deceased prevalence of Alzheimer disease associated with 3-hydroxy-3-methylglutaryl coenzyme A reductase inhibitors. Arch Neurol 57:1439–1443, 2000 Wu WS, Holmans P, Wavrant-DeVrieze F, et al: Genetic studies on chromosome 12 in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA 280:619–622, 1998 Wyss-Coray T, Lin C, Yan F, et al: TGF-beta1 promotes microglial amyloid-beta clearance and reduces plaque burden in transgenic mice. Nat Med 7:612–618, 2001 Yamazaki T, Koo EH, Selkoe DJ: Multiple fates of cell-surface amyloid betaprotein precursor. Secretion, endocytosis, recycling and lysosomal targeting. J Cell Sci 109:999–1008, 1996 Yasojima K, Akiyama H, McGeer E, et al: Reduced neprilysin in high plaque areas of Alzheimer’s brain: a possible relationship to deficient degradation of betaamyloid peptide. Neurosci Lett 297:97–100, 2001 Yu G, Nishimura M, Arawaka S, et al: Nicastrin modulates presenilin-modulated notch/glp-1 signal transduction and beta APP processing, Nature 407:34–35, 2000
CHAPTER
15
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease Steven C. Samuels, M.D. Kenneth L. Davis, M.D.
Progressive cognitive deterioration, the clinical hallmark of Alzheimer’s disease, is the main target of current therapeutic efforts. As with other diseases characterized by progressive deterioration, therapeutic goals can include alleviating symptoms, slowing or arresting the deterioration, or reversing the damage. The neurotransmitter-based approaches described in this chapter offer modest palliation by augmenting deficient neurotransmission. There is preliminary evidence that some of the cholinesterase inhibitors may also alter disease course. Many other empirically derived approaches have been assessed for potential palliative or deterioration-retarding properties in Alzheimer’s disease. Recent advances in understanding Alzheimer’s disease may permit the development of new strategies for retarding or halting the progression of the illness. These strategies, derived from various pathophysiological models of the illness, attempt to alter putative neurotoxic mechanisms such as the excitotoxic effects of glutamate, free radical–mediated neuronal damage, chronic brain inflammation, amyloid production, metal accumulation, and others. A vaccine against >-amyloid had intriguing initial results in animals but proved toxic in humans. Cholinergic Hypothesis of Alzheimer’s Disease and Cholinergic Enhancement Strategies Several lines of evidence support the hypothesis that enhancing cholinergic neurotransmission ameliorates cognitive dysfunction in persons with Alzheimer’s disease (see also Chapter 7). It has been repeatedly demonstrated that learning and memory impairment due to chemical, surgical, and phar453
454
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
macological lesions of the cerebral cholinergic system are reversed in laboratory animals by cholinomimetic agents (Bartus 1982; Olton and Wenk 1987). Human trials with cholinesterase inhibitors also support the cholinergic hypothesis (Davis and Mohs 1982; Raskind et al. 2000; S.L. Rogers et al. 1998a, 1998b; Rosler et al. 1999; Tariot et al. 2000). The cholinergic cell loss in the septal nuclei and the nucleus basalis of Meynert in patients with Alzheimer’s disease is accompanied by a decrease in the cholinergic markers choline acetyltransferase and acetylcholinesterase, but this drop in cholinergic activity does not occur until the middle stage of the disease (Davis et al. 1999a). Therapeutic trials in Alzheimer’s disease aimed at augmenting cerebral cholinergic neurotransmission have employed acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, cholinergic agonists, acetylcholine precursors, acetylcholine-releasing agents, and drugs with less well-defined mechanisms. Despite the large number of known centrally active cholinomimetic agents, few are usable. Most such agents have a short biological half-life, poor blood-brain barrier penetration, instability in plasma, unpredictable absorption, frequent side effects, or a dangerously narrow therapeutic range. Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors have been the most extensively studied drugs and have had the best clinical results. There are four such drugs currently approved by the FDA: donepezil, rivastigmine, galantamine, and tacrine.
Acetylcholinesterase Inhibitor Donepezil Donepezil hydrochloride is a noncompetitive, reversible inhibitor of acetylcholinesterase with very little effect on butyrylcholinesterase. The drug is 95% protein bound and is metabolized through the 2D6 and 3A4 isoenzymes of the P450 system. The half-life of donepezil may exceed 100 hours, longer than any of the other FDA-approved cholinesterase inhibitors. This allows for once-daily dosing, but it may also explain the potential for this agent to disturb sleep in Alzheimer’s disease patients. Superiority of the 10-mg/day dosage to the 5-mg/day dosage was supported by one study (Burns et al. 1999) but not by others (S.L. Rogers et al. 1998a, 1998b). Higher dosages increase the risk of central and peripheral cholinergic effects.
Acetylcholinesterase Inhibitor Rivastigmine Rivastigmine is a nonselective cholinesterase inhibitor with activity against acetylcholinesterase and butyrylcholinesterase. The inhibition of acetylcholinesterase is considered pseudoirreversible, because the rivastigmineacetylcholinesterase combination is not hepatically metabolized but
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
455
requires hydrolysis at the covalent esteratic site by other cholinesterases. The plasma half-life is 1.5 hours, but the effective half-life is 9 hours because of the pseudoirreversible inhibition described above (Jann 2000). The drug is not extensively protein bound and has minimal potential interaction with other agents. Clinical trial data revealed that this agent had efficacy for cognition, function, and behavior but was associated with adverse gastrointestinal side effects that appeared to be related to absolute dosage and rate of titration. There is a warning in the package insert about gastrointestinal side effects, anorexia, and weight loss. A “Dear Doctor” letter from the manufacturer reported a death from vomiting and esophageal rupture when the medication was restarted after a hiatus at a dosage higher than the suggested initiating dosage. As indicated in Chapter 7, rivastigmine requires titration to achieve an effective dosage.
Acetylcholinesterase Inhibitor Galantamine Galantamine hydrobromide is an alkaloid derived from the daffodil (snowdrop plant) that competitively and reversibly inhibits acetylcholinesterase. Galantamine is 10% protein bound and has a half-life of 7 hours. Metabolism occurs hepatically through the P450 2D6 isoenzyme. Galantamine differs from the other cholinesterase inhibitors in its competitive inhibition of acetylcholinesterase and its effect on nicotinic receptors. The competitive inhibition of acetylcholinesterase may translate to fewer side effects because the drug could have greater activity in brain areas with relative acetylcholine deficiencies. The mechanism of action of galantamine involves allosteric modulation of the presynaptic and postsynaptic nicotinic receptors, potentiating the release of acetylcholine (Maelicke 2000). In addition, galantamine may alter the transcriptional properties and stabilization of the nicotinic receptors (Samochocki et al. 2000).
Acetylcholinesterase Inhibitor Tetrahydroacridinamine (Tacrine) The synthetic aminoacridine 1,2,3,4,-tetrahydro-9-acridinamine, known as tacrine, was synthesized more than 40 years ago. The U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) approved tacrine (Cognex) in 1993 for treatment of cognitive impairment in persons with mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease. Despite a number of trials showing superiority to placebo (Conway 1998), tacrine has very limited clinical use because of four-timesa-day dosing, the potential for hepatotoxicity, gastrointestinal side effects, and the availability of alternative FDA-approved cholinesterase inhibitors that have an improved side-effect profile and preferred dosing schedule.
456
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Other Cholinesterase Inhibitors Huperzine A Huperzine A is a natural alkaloid from the plant Huperzia serrata. It is a reversible acetylcholinesterase inhibitor that is more selective and more potent than physostigmine (Wang et al. 1986). In a double-blind, placebocontrolled study in patients with multi-infarct dementia and Alzheimer’s disease, huperzine A was administered either 0.05 mg intramuscularly twice a day or 0.03 mg intramuscularly three times a day and was shown to have positive effects (S.L. Zhang et al. 1986). In a cell culture of rat pheochromocytoma cells that were exposed to >-amyloid, preincubation with huperzine A increased cell survival (Xiao et al. 2000). In addition, huperzine A appears to have some activity as an antagonist of N-methyl-Daspartic acid (NMDA), although the clinical relevance of this activity on dissociated rat hippocampal neurons remains unclear (J.M. Zhang and Hu 2001). Huperzine A has very low bioavailability, a factor that may limit its clinical utility.
Comparison of Cholinesterase Inhibitors Although the cholinesterase inhibitors have not been directly compared in clinical trials, they may have different mechanisms of action.
Butyrylcholinesterase Rivastigmine and tacrine nonselectively inhibit butyrylcholinesterase and acetylcholinesterase. The lytic effect of butyrylcholinesterase on acetylcholine is one-tenth that of acetylcholinesterase, and it depends on substrate concentration. Butyrylcholinesterase activity is found in glial cells and plaques and appears to be more pronounced in advanced Alzheimer’s disease. However, the presence of butyrylcholinesterase does not necessarily explain the class effect benefit of cholinesterase inhibitors in more advanced Alzheimer’s disease. In fact, for butyrylcholinesterase to have clinical activity in patients with more advanced Alzheimer’s disease, a selective acetylcholinesterase inhibitor must increase synaptic acetylcholine concentrations to the micromolar range and the acetylcholine has to diffuse the glial cell protoplasm near the synapse and ultimately be degraded. (Giacobini 2000b, p. 186). Butyrylcholinesterase inhibition also results in increased levels of acetylcholinesterase in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) (Kennedy et al. 1999). The additional acetylcholinesterase activity may be therapeutically countertherapeutic and lead to pathological changes as well (Inestrosa et al. 1998;
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
457
Munoz and Inestrosa 1999). In addition, there is an abundance of evidence against an association between the butyrylcholinesterase K-variant and Alzheimer’s disease (Ki et al. 1999; Kim et al. 2001; Lee et al. 2000). Thus, the clinical meaningfulness of butyrylcholinesterase inhibition on Alzheimer’s disease remains unclear.
Competitive or Noncompetitive Inhibition Cholinesterase inhibitors also may be distinguished based on their cholinesterase inhibition profile. The competitive inhibitors of acetylcholinesterase depend on acetylcholine concentration. At the acetylcholinesterase binding site, competitive inhibitors compete with acetylcholine, whereas noncompetitive inhibitors bind to the receptor site independent of acetylcholine concentration. In brain areas high in acetylcholine, competitive inhibitors are less likely to bind to the enzymatic site. In brain areas low in acetylcholine, competitive cholinesterase inhibitors will have a higher likelihood of binding to acetylcholinesterase compared with acetylcholine. This profile of the competitive inhibitors of acetylcholinesterase may offer an advantage for Alzheimer’s disease patients. Competitive inhibitors are more likely to act in brain areas with low acetylcholine levels and less likely to act in areas with higher acetylcholine levels. Alzheimer’s disease patients with low acetylcholine levels will more likely benefit from competitive inhibition. In contrast, a noncompetitive inhibitor binding to acetylcholinesterase where there are high levels of acetylcholine may have an increased propensity to produce adverse central cholinergic effects.
Nicotinic Mechanisms Nicotine has a theoretical benefit in Alzheimer’s disease (Davis 2001). Alzheimer’s disease patients have losses of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors that are distinct from aging alone (Court et al. 2001). Transdermal nicotine administration to Alzheimer’s disease patients results in short-term cognitive benefit (White and Levin 1999). The neurotoxicity of >-amyloid is blocked by nicotine in vivo and in vitro (Zeng et al. 2001). Enhancement of nicotinic transmission may benefit memory and learning, possibly by means of neuroprotective and neurotrophic effects (Coyle and Kershaw 2001). Stimulation of the presynaptic nicotinic acetylcholine receptor may augment acetylcholine, glutamate, and serotonin, which are attenuated in Alzheimer’s disease (Maelicke et al. 2001). In addition, when nicotine is administered directly to rat hippocampus, an increase of nerve growth factor results (Rattray 2001). Galantamine is unique among the cholinesterase inhibitors in that it has a modulating effect on the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor at a distinct allosteric binding site from acetylcholine (Schratten-
458
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
holz et al. 1996). Taken together, this evidence supports the potential for upregulation or stimulation of nicotinic acetylcholine receptor as a therapeutic modality in Alzheimer’s disease.
Alteration of Course Clinical trial data suggest that cholinesterase inhibitors are both palliative and disease altering. All of the FDA-approved cholinesterase inhibitors demonstrated an initial increase from baseline in scores on the cognitive portion of the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS-Cog) (Rosen et al. 1984). On average, Alzheimer’s disease patients worsen by 4 points over 6 months on the ADAS-Cog, although there is great individual variation. Patients receiving cholinesterase inhibitors remain at or above their baseline ADAS-Cog scores for as long as 24 months (Giacobini 2000a). However, in one clinical trial, half of the subjects were treated with a cholinesterase inhibitor for 6 months while a comparison group received placebo treatment. At the end of 6 months, the placebo group was started on active drug. These individuals responded to treatment but never achieved the same scores as those who had been receiving active treatment from baseline (Raskind et al. 2000). The fact that the group whose treatment was delayed 6 months did not achieve the same scores as those with ongoing treatment suggests that cholinesterase inhibitors have disease-modifying effects.
Limitations to the Approach Other means of stimulating the cholinergic system in Alzheimer’s disease have been tried and have failed; they include cholinergic agonists (Adamus et al. 1995; Thal et al. 2000), acetylcholine-enhancing agents (Huff et al. 1996; Rockwood et al. 1997), and acetylcholine precursors (Amenta 2001). Adverse effects of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors are primarily due to the enhancement of peripheral cholinergic tone. Cholinergic toxicity is often manifested by gastrointestinal symptoms (nausea, vomiting, abdominal cramping, and diarrhea), diaphoresis, lightheadedness, and occasionally cardiovascular symptoms such as bradycardia and hypotension. Depression and agitation have also been encountered. Individual susceptibility to such side effects varies widely, but clinical trial data support a relationship between dosage titration rate and gastrointestinal symptoms. There are limitations to the postsynaptic agonist strategy as well. Postsynaptic muscarinic agonists provide a nonphysiological tonic stimulation, whereas phasic processes characterize the physiological state. Also, it appears that the central nervous system contains spare cholinergic
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
459
receptor sites that have lost functional innervation during development and maturation. The stimulation of such spare receptors and the resulting untoward effects might present one of the more significant impediments to the agonist approach. Augmenting both muscarinic and nicotinic cholinergic transmission at hypothesized specific nicotinic receptor subtypes could possibly further enhance the therapeutic efficacy of cholinergic agonists. This possibility is supported by the finding that single photon emission computed tomography performed after administration of scopolamine in young volunteers reveals a brain scan pattern of predominantly frontal flow reduction (Honer et al. 1988), unlike the pattern found in Alzheimer’s disease patients, which is characterized by temporoparietal flow reduction. The difference in these two patterns could be explained by the fact that scopolamine primarily induces a central cholinergic muscarinic blockade, whereas the Alzheimer’s disease pattern might be produced by additional deficits such as cholinergic nicotinic or other noncholinergic deficits. The pharmacology of the cholinergic system is far from fully understood. Research may reveal variables that have not yet been recognized, and the clinical implications of the various parameters need more accurate assessment.
Noncholinergic Neurotransmitter Replacement Strategies Alzheimer’s disease is characterized by marked noradrenergic and serotoninergic deficits (reviewed in Stern and Davis 1996). In addition, deficits in neuropeptidergic neurotransmission have been found (somatostatin in particular) (Davis et al. 1999b; Gabriel et al. 1996). On the basis of these findings and on encouraging experimental results, investigators have attempted to manipulate these neurotransmitter systems.
Monoaminergic Drugs Selegiline, or deprenyl (Eldepryl), is a monoamine oxidase B inhibitor with good brain permeability that is used in the treatment of Parkinson’s disease (Olanow and Koller 1998). Selegiline has been evaluated in several studies with Alzheimer’s disease patients. In the most recent of these, a large multicenter study involving patients with moderate to severe Alzheimer’s disease, selegiline was compared with =-tocopherol, the combination of selegiline and =-tocopherol, and placebo (Sano et al 1997). Both agents were superior to placebo in delaying progression to one of three end points:
460
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
death, institutionalization, or loss of an activity of daily living. Of interest, there was no benefit of the combination therapy on the end points of the study. The centrally active adrenoreceptor agonists clonidine and guanfacine have been assessed for their therapeutic potential in Alzheimer’s disease but they produced no significant improvement (Crook et al. 1992; Mohr et al. 1989). These negative results do not mean that adrenergic enhancement is ineffective in Alzheimer’s disease or that it might not be a critical complement to a cholinergic therapy. It is possible that agonists have a significant activity at presynaptic receptors and inhibit transmitter release to an extent that yields a net diminution in noradrenergic activity. Therefore, an alternative strategy could employ an adrenoreceptor antagonist, selectively active at the presynaptic receptor, which would interrupt the feedback inhibition path and could lead to increased norepinephrine release. Both agonists and antagonists require further investigation.
Cholinergic-Monoaminergic Combinations Animal experiments have demonstrated that noradrenergic brain lesions block cholinomimetic enhancement of memory and that the efficacy of cholinomimetic treatment can be restored by administration of clonidine (Haroutunian et al. 1990). Combination strategies using cholinesterase inhibitors and presynaptic =-adrenergic antagonists are supported in animal studies (Camacho et al. 1996; Santucci et al. 1991). Clinical trials in humans using this strategy are under way (Friedman et al. 1999).
Miscellaneous Neuropeptides Corticotropin-releasing hormone has shown benefit in protecting neurons in cell culture from death caused by >-amyloid, lipid peroxidation, and glutamate (Pedersen et al. 2001). The benefit was associated with stabilization of calcium homeostasis. Neuropathological studies in Alzheimer’s disease patients have suggested that low levels of corticotropin-releasing hormone immunoreactivity may be an early marker for Alzheimer’s disease (Davis et al. 1999b). Animal studies are needed as a next step with corticotropin-releasing hormone, a compound whose receptor also has the potential as a drug target for depression (Holsboer 2001). Cerebrolysin (FPF-1070) is a mammalian tissue extract with neurotrophic activity but unclear mechanism of action. The agent has promoted neuron survival in vivo and in vitro (Deigner et al. 2000) A randomized, placebo-controlled trial supports the cognitive benefit of cerebrolysin at 28
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
461
weeks, 3 months after drug withdrawal (Ruether et al. 2001). Long-term studies with cerebrolysin examining measures of cognition and function are needed.
Glutamatergic Drugs Excitatory amino acid (EAA)–mediated toxicity may produce lesions similar to the neurofibrillary tangles of Alzheimer’s disease. Furthermore, the glutamatergic system has been implicated in learning and memory. Blockade of NMDA receptors disrupts spatial learning and prevents long-term potentiation, which is thought to be the physiological basis of memory (Squire 1992). Several glutamate receptor subtypes exist. NMDA receptor antagonists may benefit Alzheimer’s disease patients because of the role of NMDA receptor stimulation in apoptosis. The relative role of caspasemediated apoptosis or necrosis in Alzheimer’s disease pathophysiology remains unclear (Behl 2000). NMDA receptor blockers have been shown to provide significant protection against ischemic and EAA-mediated cortical insults (Greenamyre and Young 1989). Glycine, an NMDA receptor modulator, is thought to enhance the effects of glutamate at the NMDA receptor site and thus to augment longterm potentiation processes and memory. It appears that the blockade of certain glutamate receptor subtypes may protect against EAA excitotoxicity, whereas activation of other subtypes may enhance cognitive functions in Alzheimer’s disease. Memantine, an NMDA receptor antagonist, demonstrated benefit in moderate to severely demented patients at 12 weeks as measured by a rating of caregiver time spent and a global impression of change (Winblad and Poritis 1999). An earlier trial had failed to demonstrate benefit (Fleischhacker et al. 1986), and an in vitro study demonstrated no attenuation of cholinesterase-inhibiting effects by the coadministration of memantine (Wenk et al. 2000). Muscarinic agonists may also have theoretical benefit for Alzheimer’s disease and are being developed for clinical trials (Cain et al. 2000). Animal models have demonstrated benefit with ampakines, allosteric modulators of the AMPA (=-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methylisoxazole-4-propionate) receptor (Yamada 2000), and AMPA potentiators are currently in drug development (Lynch et al. 1997). Cycloserine, a partial glycine agonist, enhances learning in rodents. In human volunteers, D-cycloserine was shown to ameliorate scopolamineinduced cognitive impairment (Jones et al. 1991), and there is evidence that at dosages of 50 or 100 mg/day, cognitive benefit can be achieved with this agent in Alzheimer’s disease patients (Tsai et al. 1999).
462
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Glutamatergic-modulating agents require further studies. The pharmacology of the EAA is a rapidly developing field. New NMDA receptor blockers, as well as possible EAA receptor subtype–specific partial agonists may open new avenues in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease.
Antioxidants For more than 40 years, it has been proposed that free radicals produced during aerobic respiration cause cumulative oxidative damage that results in aging, disease, and death (Harman 1993; Harman et al. 1966). Aging and Alzheimer’s disease may be associated with increased production of free radicals (Harman et al. 1966). Increased superoxide dismutase–derived hydrogen peroxide fluxes, metal ions, and damaged mitochondria can contribute to cell damage mediated by free radicals (Multhaup et al. 1997). Free radical production in Alzheimer’s disease may also be caused by >-amyloid and glutamate (Behl et al. 1994). For example, >-amyloid may alter glucose metabolism in hippocampal neuronal cell lines via lipid peroxidation, an alteration that leads to cell membrane damage (Mark et al. 1997). In addition, fibrillar >-amyloid is associated with oxidative stress markers in transgenic mice models for Alzheimer’s disease (Matsuoka et al. 2001). Alteration in the oxidative metabolism may also increase the production of the soluble, aggregating form of >-amyloid (Gabuzda et al. 1994; Gasparini et al. 1997) Therefore, it has been hypothesized that antioxidant agents could have beneficial effects in Alzheimer’s disease by reducing free radical production and preventing subsequent cell injury. Selegiline is thought to be neuroprotective by quenching free radicals. Vitamin E and idebenone are also potential antioxidant treatments for Alzheimer’s disease because they prevent cell death caused by glutamate and >-amyloid protein (Oka et al. 1993). Idebenone is no longer in development in the United States, although clinical trial data were suggestive of its potential efficacy and relative safety in phase 3 trials (Gutzmann and Hadler 1998; Weyer et al. 1997).
Vitamins Epidemiological studies and some clinical trials suggest that vitamins may reduce the risk of developing cognitive impairment, and perhaps dementing illness (Chandra 2001). Dietary intake of vitamins C and E was associated with reduced risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease in a cohort from East Boston (Morris et al. 1998). Another large epidemiological trial of elderly men found that vitamin E and C supplements were associated with decreased risk of developing vascular dementia but no reduction in the risk of develop-
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
463
ing Alzheimer’s disease (Masaki et al. 2000). Moreover, lower plasma levels of =-tocopherol were associated with lower cognition on a dementia rating scale (Schmidt et al. 1998). In a cohort study of elderly retirement-community residents in Australia, intake of vitamin C was associated with a lower prevalence of cognitive impairment (Paleologos et al. 1998). In a large epidemiological study, low dietary intake of >-carotene was associated with impaired cognitive function in Alzheimer’s disease patients (Jama et al. 1996). Thiamine has demonstrated no benefit in Alzheimer’s disease (Rodriguez-Martin et al. 2000). Pyritinol (Encephabol), a pyridoxine (vitamin B6) derivative, was administered orally in dosages of 600 mg/day to 100 Alzheimer’s disease patients in a 12-week double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel study (Fischhof et al. 1992). Although pyritinol appeared to be superior to placebo, the differences between placebo and the active agent were not significant. Mecobalamin (vitamin B12) (0.5 mg intravenously every other day) was given to 10 Alzheimer’s disease patients for 8 weeks. Cognitive functions and communication abilities improved, and the improvements correlated with high levels of vitamin B12 in CSF (Ikeda et al. 1992). In another study, decreased vitamin B12 levels were associated with increased behavioral disturbance in Alzheimer’s disease (Meins et al. 2000).
Gingko Gingko, an extract of the Gingko biloba tree, is a popular preparation of unknown chemical constitution that may have scavenging effects on free radicals. It has been studied in double-blind, placebo-controlled trials. One study in a cohort of persons with Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, or age-associated memory impairment did not demonstrate an effect on neuropsychological and behavioral measures (van Dongen et al. 2000). In another study, modest differences were found between gingko (120 mg/ day) and placebo groups on measures of cognition and function. (Le Bars et al. 1997). The utility of gingko for Alzheimer’s disease requires further investigation before it is recommended to patients.
Chelating Agents An association between aluminum and Alzheimer’s disease has been suspected for several decades. Aluminum is toxic to the cholinergic system (Clayton et al. 1992) and leads to >-amyloid conformational change and increase in aggregation in vitro (Kawahara et al. 2001). In a 2-year doubleblind, controlled trial, patients who received intramuscular desferrioxamine treatment showed less decline in daily living skills than did the placebo
464
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
group (Crapper McLachlan et al. 1991). The therapeutic effects observed with this agent may not be due to its chelating action, because it has been shown to inhibit free radical formation and inflammation as well (Crapper McLachlan et al. 1991). The required intramuscular administration and toxic side effects of this compound might limit its clinical utility. Replication studies are necessary to confirm the efficacy of this approach. In addition, since desferrioxamine chelates iron very strongly, the possibility that iron overload may have a role in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease needs continued consideration (Savory et al. 1996). Agents that bind to copper and zinc may also be potential therapeutic agents for Alzheimer’s disease (Bush et al. 1994; Gouras and Beal 2001). Transgenic mice models had reduction in amyloid burden when treated with clioquinol, an antibiotic and a copper-zinc chelator (Cherny et al. 2001). Caution is needed in extrapolating from animal models for Alzheimer’s disease. Human trials are still required to validate the effectiveness of compounds that have demonstrated benefit in transgenic mice.
New Outlooks Nerve Growth Factor Another potential therapeutic approach for Alzheimer’s disease is the administration of neurotrophic factors. Nerve growth factor (NGF) is a 118– amino acid polypeptide that does not cross the blood-brain barrier. Other substances with neurotrophic activity—such as epidermal growth factor, brain-derived neurotrophic factor, and gangliosides—might have therapeutic potential as well. Intraventricular administration of NGF partially reverses lesion-induced deficits in the activity of cortical acetylcholinesterase and choline acetyltransferase (Haroutunian et al. 1985) and promotes survival of septal cholinergic neurons after fimbrial transection in adult rats (Hefti 1986). Animals with reduced levels of NGF demonstrate impairment in spatial learning (K.S. Chen et al. 1997). Human NGF has been produced by recombinant techniques, and in the near future sufficient amounts of pure human NGF may be available for experimental assessment and therapeutic trials in Alzheimer’s disease. There are promising pharmacological strategies—such as gene transfer, cell grafting, synthesis of liposomes, drug lipidization, and development of lipid-soluble prodrugs and chimeric nutrients or peptides—that might provide ways for improved noninvasive drug delivery to the central nervous system (reviewed in Martinez-Serrano and Bjorklund 1998). Other routes of administration could also be considered. Genetically modified NGF-secreting fibroblast grafts have been shown to prevent
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
465
degeneration of cholinergic neurons after surgical lesions of the fimbriafornix in rats (Rosenberg et al. 1988). This opens the possibility of grafting genetically engineered cells to secrete in situ the required factor or neurotransmitter. For example, a fibroblast cell line was genetically modified to express choline acetyltransferase. The altered cells produced and released acetylcholine in vitro and in vivo after grafting into rat hippocampus. Thus, specially designed fibroblasts could provide a vehicle for delivering various substances to the brain. It is conceivable that in the future neuronal stem cells or neurotransmitter-secreting cell implants will become available for clinical trials in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. NGF conjugated to an antibody to the transferring receptor was shown to cross the blood-brain barrier and to increase the survival of cholinergic and noncholinergic neurons after peripheral injection in rats (Frieden et al. 1993). Pharmacological modulation of NGF within the central nervous system may be a future method to deal with the limitations of intracerebral administration of NGF (reviewed in Rattray 2001).
Estrogen Support for use of estrogen in Alzheimer’s disease comes from laboratory and epidemiological findings. Cholinergic and estrogen receptors are colocalized (Burton-Jones et al. 1999). Estrogen activates the cholinergic system, blocks anticholinergic effects, encourages neuron arborization, and attenuates the toxicity of >-amyloid in cell culture (Gibbs 1999; Luine et al. 1980; Segarra and McEwen 1991). In addition, estrogen has antioxidant properties and neurotrophic effects and promotes neuronal survival (Inestrosa et al. 1998; Nathan and Chaudhuri 1998). The women in the tacrine clinical trial who were receiving estrogen responded better to tacrine than the women who were not taking estrogen (Schneider and Farlow 1997). One explanation for this finding may be the effect of estrogen on increasing 1-hydroxy tacrine, an active metabolite of tacrine (Laine et al. 1999). In addition, the trial was not designed to ascertain the effect of estrogen on cholinesterase inhibitors, and the variable dosage of estrogen and the lack of randomization limit the conclusions that can be drawn from the report. Epidemiological support is consistent for the use of estrogen as a preventive for Alzheimer’s disease in postmenopausal women (Baldereschi et al. 1998; Kawas et al. 1997; LeBlanc et al. 2001; Paganini-Hill and Henderson 1996; Tang et al. 1996). The optimal duration of use and the preferred estrogen preparation and dosage could not be deduced from these epidemiological studies. The utility of estrogen as a therapeutic agent for Alzheimer’s disease is uncertain (Asthana et al. 1999, 2001; Henderson et al. 2000). In a large
466
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
1-year randomized trial with estrogen, there was no benefit for Alzheimer’s disease in measures of cognition or function. The estrogen-treated group actually worsened in cognitive measures compared with the placebo group over the duration of the study (Mulnard et al. 2000). Longer-term trials are under way to clarify the effect of estrogen on the development and progression of dementia (Shumaker et al. 1998).
Anti-Inflammatory Agents Epidemiological, neuropathological, preclinical, and clinical data support involvement of the immune system and of inflammation in the pathophysiology of Alzheimer’s disease (Anthony et al. 2000; McGeer and McGeer 1995; McGeer et al. 1996). Activation of microglia and inflammatory markers are found in neurons and axons near amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. Acute-phase reactants are present in CSF, along with elevated levels of various cytokines, including tumor necrosis factor in serum and interleukins 1 and 6 in brain. Cellular immune responses include T4 and T8 lymphocytes and microglia and the production of complement. Several studies suggest that nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs) such as indomethacin may have protective effects against Alzheimer’s disease (Breitner et al. 1994). Interestingly, the benefit of NSAIDs in reducing risk of Alzheimer’s disease may require at least 2 years of exposure to the agents (Beard et al. 1998; in’t Veld et al. 1998, 2001; Stewart et al. 1997). The dosage of NSAIDs needed to achieve the risk reduction is not yet clear (Broe et al. 2000; McGeer et al. 1996). In a 6-month double-blind, placebo-controlled study of Alzheimer’s disease, oral indomethacin 100–150 mg/day was found to improve cognitive function and to delay further deterioration. These results are promising, but the toxicity of indomethacin limits its use (J. Rogers et al. 1993). Finally, NSAIDS are not alike in their effects on amyloid precursor protein (APP) processing. Some, such as ibuprofen and indomethacin, decrease >-amyloid 42 and increase the less toxic and less aggregating >-amyloid 38 fragment by alteration of C-secretase activity (De Strooper and Konig 2001; Weggen et al. 2001). Use of corticosteroids is another possible strategy for anti-inflammatory agents in Alzheimer’s disease. These agents are widely used for the treatment of central nervous system inflammatory diseases, including lupus cerebritis and multiple sclerosis. Unfortunately, the systemic toxicity of steroids limits the dosage and length of treatment that are possible with these agents. Treatment with prednisone 10 mg/day orally for 1 year was not effective in Alzheimer’s disease and was associated with adverse behavioral effects and worsening cognition compared with placebo (Aisen et al. 2000). Colchicine is another possible candidate among anti-inflammatory
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
467
agents for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. This drug effectively treats familial Mediterranean fever, a condition in which recurrent inflammation and amyloidosis occur. Although the amyloid constituents in familial Mediterranean fever and Alzheimer’s disease differ, both illnesses involve chronic inflammation, elevated levels of acute-phase proteins, and abnormal processing of a precursor protein leading to deposition of insoluble amyloid fragments. Hydroxychloroquine, an antimalarial agent and an effective second-line drug for the treatment of rheumatoid arthritis and lupus erythematosus, is thought to affect the immune response by interacting with lysosomal functioning. This agent suppresses cytokine and acutephase reactant levels. The drug is relatively safe when administered chronically for the treatment of rheumatoid arthritis but was not found to slow the rate of decline in patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease over 18 months (Van Gool et al. 2001). Mouse studies suggest that these agents may vary in their ability to alter APP processing independent of cyclooxygenase activity (Weggen et al. 2001). Multicenter clinical trials with cyclooxygenase-2 inhibitors and other NSAIDs are currently in progress (Breitner and Zandi 2001; McGeer 2000). When these findings are available, the interpretation of the results should consider that differential effects of anti-inflammatory agents may reflect the specific agent used, dosage and duration of exposure, severity of cognitive impairment when exposed to the agent, and medical comorbidities. A study of celecoxib has been abandoned by the manufacturer, and both rofecoxib and naprosyn have been found ineffective over a 1-year course of treatment.
APP Metabolism There is abundant evidence that accumulation and aggregation of >-amyloid may be involved in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease (Selkoe 2000). The APP may or may not be cleaved to form the neurotoxic >-amyloid 42–43. Therapeutic targets for drug discovery include the secretase enzymes responsible for the alternative cleavage pathways. >-Secretase and C-secretase must both cleave APP to produce toxic >-amyloid (Vassar et al. 1999). =-Secretase cleaves within the >-amyloid domain and results in a nonneurotoxic fragment. Therefore, minimizing production of toxic >-amyloid could be accomplished by inhibition of >- or C-secretase or potentiation of =-secretase (see Selkoe 2001). Several manufacturers have C-secretase inhibitors under development. There is the possibility that C-secretase is closely linked to presenilins (Li et al. 2000; Wolfe et al. 1999) and that presenilins may become a therapeutic target. However, the importance of presenilins in the critical Notch pathway may limit the applicability of C-secretase inhibitors. In vivo studies have demonstrated the ability
468
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
of C-secretase inhibitor to reduce brain >-amyloid levels in transgenic animals. (Dovey et al. 2001). An alternative therapeutic approach could involve an antibody to the toxic >-amyloid. In fact, this is the basis for an Alzheimer’s “vaccine.”
Interfering With Tangle Formation Hyperphosphorylation of tau protein leads to the formation of neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) (Alonso et al. 1996). NFTs are present in the brains of individuals with Alzheimer’s disease irrespective of cognitive status, but the density of the NFTs is correlated with the clinical severity of Alzheimer’s disease (Haroutunian et al. 1999). Tauopathies are diseases that may include Alzheimer’s disease, and an understanding of the tauopathies may also translate into therapeutic interventions (Forman et al. 2000). The cholinergic system may also interface with tangle formation. Tau is phosphorylated by glycogen synthase kinase–3> and is inhibited via protein kinase C. Many muscarinic agonists attenuate the expression of phosphorylated tau (Forlenza et al. 2000). In addition, lithium has been shown to block tau hyperphosphorylation by inhibition of glycogen synthase kinase–3> (Lovestone et al. 1999). Inhibitors of glycogen synthase kinase–3>, such as lithium and valproic acid (G. Chen et al. 1999), may be future therapeutic agents for Alzheimer’s disease.
Immunological Approaches: “Vaccine” Transgenic mice that possess the mutated human gene for APP develop intracerebral amyloid deposits. The development of these deposits was attenuated in these genetically altered mice (compared with control mice) by vaccinating the mice with >-amyloid (Bard et al. 2000; Barinaga 1999). Vaccination of mice that had already developed the amyloid deposits led to a reduction in plaque formation in the transgenic mouse group compared with control mice (Janus et al. 2000). The effectiveness of the vaccine in transgenic mice may be dependent on >-amyloid load before the vaccination; the vaccine appears to work best in mice with low or no levels of >-amyloid at the time of the vaccinations (Das et al. 2001). To determine whether the vaccine has any beneficial or deleterious effects, in one study the transgenic mice that developed learning deficits were observed for 8 months while they were receiving the vaccination (Morgan et al. 2000). During that time there were no functional changes in the vaccinated group of mice compared with the control mice. The vaccinated group performed better than the control transgenic mice on a task of spatial memory. Pre-
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
469
liminary work in vaccinating humans (Thatte 2001) was terminated in early 2002 because 4/360 persons exposed to multiple doses of the vaccine AN-1972 developed clinical signs of CNS inflammation (Imbimbo 2002). The cause of this inflammation remains to be determined. Other unanswered questions about this approach are the degree to which elderly humans will develop and maintain an immune response to antibodies against amyloid protein, whether humans will reject the foreign epitope (see Frenkel et al. 2001), which epitope will be ideal for humans (see Klein et al. 2001), whether the vaccine will be able to (or will need to) cross the bloodbrain barrier (see Poduslo and Curran 2001), what schedule of vaccines will be optimal (e.g., giving it to subjects at risk or to subjects who already have the disease), whether reduction in amyloid plaques will result in any meaningful clinical change in presentation or course of the disease, and whether reduction in plaque burden will result in an increase in other pathology (a shift in equilibrium).
Apolipoprotein E There is a threefold higher prevalence of the apolipoprotein E (ApoE) A4 allele in persons with late-onset familial Alzheimer’s disease than in control subjects. ApoE is present in senile plaques, vascular amyloid, and neurofibrillary tangles of Alzheimer’s disease. Based on experimental data, it has been suggested that the protein generated by the ApoE A4 allele (unlike the proteins generated by the alleles ApoE A3 or ApoE A2) lacks microtubule-protective properties. Thus, the absence of the alternative alleles ApoE A3 or ApoE A2 may be amyloidogenic and associated with an increased risk for Alzheimer’s disease (Strittmatter et al. 1994). This hypothesis suggests that new pharmacological strategies in Alzheimer’s disease could consist of agents with ApoE A3- or ApoE A2-like microtubule–stabilizing properties or of compounds that alter the expression of the ApoE gene. The protein product of the ApoE A4 gene, ApoE4, is involved in lipid transport and may affect lipid balance in the brains of Alzheimer’s disease patients. Phospholipid, fatty acid, and cholesterol metabolism may be adversely affected by low apolipoprotein concentrations in ApoE A4 Alzheimer’s disease patients. Response to drug treatment in Alzheimer’s disease patients may be dependent on ApoE genotype. In addition, there is an inverse relationship between the number of ApoE A4 alleles and nicotinic receptor binding as well as choline acetyltransferase activity. For all these reasons, ApoE A4 may be a potential target for therapeutic intervention in Alzheimer’s disease (Poirier 1999).
470
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Statins Cholesterol has been associated with increased risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease (Marx 2001). Rabbits fed high-cholesterol diets demonstrated increased intracellular >-amyloid (Sparks et al. 1994). Guinea pigs given statin drugs demonstrated lower levels of >-amyloid 42 in CSF and brain (Fassbender et al. 2001). In another study with >-amyloid phenotype transgenic mice, a cholesterol-lowering agent (BM15.766) resulted in lower levels of plaque formation, lower serum cholesterol levels, and a correlation between serum cholesterol levels and >-amyloid levels (Refolo et al. 2001). In a cell culture experiment, another possible link in cholesterol metabolism and APP processing was observed. The drug lovastatin appeared to stimulate =-secretase and resulted in the increased production of the nontoxic, soluble =-secretase cleaved APP fragments (Kojro et al. 2001). Another study demonstrated that cholesterol acyltransferase, the enzyme responsible for altering the balance between cholesterol and cholesterol esters, may be a potential drug target in Alzheimer’s disease (Puglielli et al. 2001). A series of epidemiological studies support the possibility that statins reduce the risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease. One study found that individuals over age 50 who were taking statins had a substantially lower risk of developing dementia independent of exposure to nonstatin lipid–powering agents or hyperlipidemia (Jick et al. 2000). Another study found that the prevalence of probable Alzheimer’s disease in a cohort of persons taking statins was 60%–73% lower than the total population or compared with patients taking other medications used in the treatment of hypertension or cardiovascular disease (Wolozin et al. 2000). Basic studies have demonstrated a reduction in the secreted form of >-amyloid in various cells exposed to lovastatin. A dose response was evident in each cell type tested (Friedhoff et al. 2001). Clinical trials are planned that will test the efficacy and safety of statins for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. In addition, a large clinical trial with pravastatin is examining the effects of this drug on major vascular events over 3.5 years in an elderly cohort of more than 5,000 individuals. Additional analysis will examine the cognitive effects of pravastatin in these individuals (Shepherd et al. 1999).
Summary and Conclusions Multiple empirical pharmacological strategies have failed in Alzheimer’s disease, but the neurotransmission-enhancing strategies (and in particular the cholinomimetic strategies) have shown some promise. The secondgeneration cholinesterase inhibitors (donepezil, galantamine, and rivastig-
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
471
mine) have been FDA approved for mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease. Some evidence suggests that these agents may have not only palliative effects, but also have a disease modifying effect. Evidence that patients with more advanced disease may benefit from cholinesterase inhibitors is intriguing and requires further investigation. An Alzheimer’s disease vaccine may offer future hope of decreasing >-amyloid burden, but crucial safety and efficacy questions remain. Newer strategies are based on an increasing understanding of the pathophysiological processes involved in this illness. Strategies aimed at slowing or arresting the progression of the illness are beginning to be explored and will undoubtedly lead to therapeutic breakthroughs in the near future. Protease inhibitors, agents that block tangle formation, antioxidants, anti-inflammatory agents, cholesterolreducing agents, and cholinesterase inhibitors may be a future therapeutic cocktail for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease.
References Adamus WS, Leonard JP, Troger W: Phase I clinical trials with WAL 2014, a new muscarinic agonist for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Life Sci 56:883– 890, 1995 Aisen PS, Davis KL, Berg JD, et al: A randomized controlled trial of prednisone in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study. Neurology 54:588–593, 2000 Alonso AC, Grundke-Iqbal I, Iqbal K: Alzheimer’s disease hyperphosphorylated tau sequesters normal tau into tangles of filaments and disassembles microtubules. Nat Med 2:783–787, 1996 Amenta F, Parnetti L, Gallai V, et al: Treatment of cognitive dysfunction associated with Alzheimer’s disease with cholinergic precursors: ineffective treatments or inappropriate approaches? Mech Ageing Dev 122:2025–2040, 2001 Anthony JC, Breitner JC, Zandi PP, et al: Reduced prevalence of AD in users of NSAIDs and H2 receptor antagonists: the Cache County study. Neurology 54:2066–2071, 2000 Asthana S, Craft S, Baker LD, et al: Cognitive and neuroendocrine response to transdermal estrogen in postmenopausal women with Alzheimer’s disease: results of a placebo-controlled, double-blind, pilot study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 24:657–677, 1999 Asthana S, Baker LD, Craft S, et al: High-dose estradiol improves cognition for women with AD: results of a randomized study. Neurology 57:605–612, 2001 Baldereschi M, Di Carlo A, Lepore V, et al: Estrogen-replacement therapy and Alzheimer’s disease in the Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Neurology 50:996–1002, 1998
472
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Bard F, Cannon C, Barbour R, et al: Peripherally administered antibodies against amyloid beta-peptide enter the central nervous system and reduce pathology in a mouse model of Alzheimer disease. Nat Med 6:916–919, 2000 Barinaga M: An immunization against Alzheimer’s? Science 285:175, 177, 1999 Bartus RT: Effects of cholinergic agents on learning and memory in animal models of aging, in Alzheimer’s Disease: A Report of Progress in Research. Edited by Corkin S, Davis KL, Growdon JH. New York, Raven, 1982, pp 271–280 Beard CM, Waring SC, O’Brien PC, et al: Nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drug use and Alzheimer’s disease: a case-control study in Rochester, Minnesota, 1980 through 1984. Mayo Clin Proc 73:951–955, 1998 Behl C: Apoptosis and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neural Transm 107:1325–1344, 2000 Behl C, Davis J, Lesley R, et al: Hydrogen peroxide mediates amyloid beta protein toxicity. Cell 77:817–827, 1994 Breitner JCS, Zandi PP: Do nonsteroidal antiinflammatory drugs reduce the risk of Alzheimer’s disease? N Engl J Med 345:1567–1568, 2001 Breitner JCS, Gau BA, Welsh KA, et al: Inverse association of anti-inflammatory treatments and Alzheimer’s disease: initial results of a co-twin control study. Neurology 44:227–232, 1994 Broe GA, Grayson DA, Creasey HM, et al: Anti-inflammatory drugs protect against Alzheimer disease at low doses. Arch Neurol 57:1586–1591, 2000 Burns A, Rossor M, Hecker J, et al: The effects of donepezil in Alzheimer’s disease—results from a multinational trial. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 10:237– 244, 1999 Burton-Jones MM, Roberts JA, Tuszynski MH: Estrogen receptor immunoreactivity in the adult primate brain: neuronal distribution and association with p75, trkA, and choline acetyltransferase. J Comp Neurol 405:529–542, 1999 Bush AI, Pettingell WH Jr, Paradis MD, et al: Modulation of A beta adhesiveness and secretase site cleavage by zinc. J Biol Chem 269:12152–12158, 1994 Cain DP, Ighanian K, Boon F: Individual and combined manipulation of muscarinic, NMDA, and benzodiazepine receptor activity in the water maze task: implications for a rat model of Alzheimer dementia. Behav Brain Res 111:125– 137, 2000 Camacho F, Smith CP, Vargas HM, et al: Alpha 2-adrenoceptor antagonists potentiate acetylcholinesterase inhibitor effects on passive avoidance learning in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 124:347–354, 1996 Chandra RK: Effect of vitamin and trace-element supplementation on cognitive function in elderly subjects. Nutrition 17:709–712, 2001 Chen G, Huang LD, Manji HK: The mood-stabilizing agent valproate inhibits the activity of glycogen synthase kinase-3. J Neurochem 72:1327–1330, 1999 Chen KS, Nishimura MC, Armanini MP, et al: Disruption of a single allele of the nerve growth factor gene results in atrophy of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons and memory deficits. J Neurosci 17:7288–7296, 1997 Cherny RA, Atwood CS, Xilinas ME, et al: Treatment with a copper-zinc chelator markedly and rapidly inhibits beta-amyloid accumulation in Alzheimer’s disease transgenic mice. Neuron 30:665–676, 2001
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
473
Clayton RM, Sedowofia SKA, Rankin JM, et al: A long-term effect of aluminum in the fetal mouse brain. Life Sci 51:1921–1928, 1992 Conway EL: A review of the randomized controlled trials of tacrine in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease: methodologic considerations. Clin Neuropharmacol 21:8–17, 1998 Court J, Martin-Ruiz C, Piggott M, et al: Nicotinic receptor abnormalities in Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 49:175–184, 2001 Coyle J, Kershaw P: Galantamine, a cholinesterase inhibitor that allosterically modulates nicotinic receptors: effects on the course of Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 49:289–299, 2001 Crapper McLachlan DR, Dalton AJ, Kruck TP, et al: Intramuscular desferrioxamine in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 337:1304–1308, 1991 Crook T, Wilner E, Rohwell A, et al: Noradrenergic intervention in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychopharmacol Bull 28:67–70, 1992 Das P, Murphy MP, Younkin LH, et al: Reduced effectiveness of Abeta1-42 immunization in APP transgenic mice with significant amyloid deposition. Neurobiol Aging 22:721–727, 2001 Davis KL: Nicotinic mechanisms: potential for therapeutic exploitation (editorial). Biol Psychiatry 49:165, 2001 Davis KL, Mohs R: Enhancement of memory processes in Alzheimer’s disease with multiple-dose intravenous physostigmine. Am J Psychiatry 139:1421–1424, 1982 Davis KL, Mohs RC, Marin D, et al: Cholinergic markers in elderly patients with early signs of Alzheimer disease. JAMA 281:1401–1406, 1999a Davis KL, Mohs RC, Marin DB, et al: Neuropeptide abnormalities in patients with early Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56:981–987, 1999b De Strooper B, Konig G: Alzheimer’s disease: an inflammatory drug prospect. Nature 414:159–160, 2001 Deigner HP, Haberkorn U, Kinscherf R: Apoptosis modulators in the therapy of neurodegenerative diseases. Expert Opin Investig Drugs 9:747–764, 2000 Dovey HF, John V, Anderson JP, et al: Functional gamma-secretase inhibitors reduce beta-amyloid peptide levels in brain. J Neurochem 76:173–181, 2001 Fassbender K, Simons M, Bergmann C, et al: Simvastatin strongly reduces levels of Alzheimer’s disease beta-amyloid peptides Abeta 42 and Abeta 40 in vitro and in vivo. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:5856–5861, 2001 Fischhof PK, Saletu B, Ruether E, et al: Therapeutic efficacy of pyritinol in patients with SDAT and MID. Neuropsychobiology 26:65–70, 1992 Fleischhacker WW, Buchgeher A, Schubert H: Memantine in the treatment of senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 10:87–93, 1986 Forlenza OV, Spink JM, Dayanandan R, et al: Muscarinic agonists reduce tau phosphorylation in non-neuronal cells via GSK-3beta inhibition and in neurons. J Neural Transm 107:1201–1212, 2000 Forman MS, Lee VM, Trojanowski JQ: New insights into genetic and molecular mechanisms of brain degeneration in tauopathies. J Chem Neuroanat 20:225– 244, 2000
474
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Frenkel D, Kariv N, Solomon B: Generation of auto-antibodies towards Alzheimer’s disease vaccination. Vaccine 19:2615–2619, 2001 Frieden PM, Walus LR, Watson P, et al: Blood-brain barrier penetration and in vivo activity of an NGF conjugate. Science 259:373–377, 1993 Friedhoff LT, Cullen EI, Geoghagen NS, et al: Treatment with controlled-release lovastatin decreases serum concentrations of human beta-amyloid (A beta) peptide. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 4:127–130, 2001 Friedman JI, Adler DN, Davis KL: The role of norepinephrine in the pathophysiology of cognitive disorders: potential applications to the treatment of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia and Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 46:1243–1252, 1999 Gabriel SM, Davidson M, Haroutunian V, et al: Neuropeptide deficits in schizophrenia vs. Alzheimer’s disease cerebral cortex. Biol Psychiatry 39:82–91, 1996 Gabuzda D, Busciglio J, Chen LB, et al: Inhibition of energy metabolism alters the processing of amyloid precursor protein and induces a potentially amyloidogenic derivative. J Biol Chem 269:13623–13628, 1994 Gasparini L, Racchi M, Benussi L, et al: Effect of energy shortage and oxidative stress on amyloid precursor protein metabolism in COS cells. Neurosci Lett 231:113–117, 1997 Giacobini E: Cholinesterase inhibitors stabilize Alzheimer’s disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 920:321–327, 2000a Giacobini E: Cholinesterases and cholinesterase inhibitors. London, Martin Dunitz, 2000b Gibbs RB: Estrogen replacement enhances acquisition of a spatial memory task and reduces deficits associated with hippocampal muscarinic receptor inhibition. Horm Behav 36:222–233, 1999 Gouras GK, Beal MF: Metal chelator decreases Alzheimer beta-amyloid plaques. Neuron 30:641–642, 2001 Greenamyre JT, Young AB: Review: excitatory amino acids and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 10:593–602, 1989 Gutzmann H, Hadler D: Sustained efficacy and safety of idebenone in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease: update on a 2-year double-blind multicentre study. J Neural Transm Suppl 54:301–310, 1998 Harman D: Free radical involvement in aging: pathophysiology and therapeutic implications. Drugs Aging 3:60–80, 1993 Harman D, Piette LH: Free radical theory of aging: free radical reactions in serum. J Gerontol 21:560–565, 1966 Haroutunian V, Kanof PD, Davis KL: Pharmacologic alleviation of cholinergic lesion induced memory deficits in rats. Life Sci 37:945–952, 1985 Haroutunian V, Kanof PD, Tsuboyama G, et al: Restoration of cholinomimetic activity by clonidine in cholinergic plus adrenergic lesioned rats. Brain Res 507:251–256, 1990 Haroutunian V, Purohit DP, Perl DP, et al: Neurofibrillary tangles in nondemented elderly subjects and mild Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 56:713–718, 1999
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
475
Hefti F: Nerve growth factor promotes survival of septal cholinergic neurons after fimbrial transsections. J Neurosci 6:2155–2162, 1986 Henderson VW, Paganini-Hill A, Miller BL, et al: Estrogen for Alzheimer’s disease in women: randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Neurology 54:295–301, 2000 Holsboer F: Prospects for antidepressant drug discovery. Biol Psychol 57:47–65, 2001 Honer WG, Prohovnik I, Smith G, et al: Scopolamine reduces frontal cortex perfusion. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 8:635–641, 1988 Huff FJ, Antuono PG, Delagandara JE, et al: A treatment and withdrawal trial of besipirdine in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 10:93–102, 1996 Ikeda T, Yamamoto K, Takahashi K, et al: Treatment of Alzheimer-type dementia with intravenous mecobalamin. Clin Ther 14:426–437, 1992 Imbimbo BP: Toxicity of beta-amyloid vaccination in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 51:794, 2002 Inestrosa NC, Marzolo MP, Bonnefont AB: Cellular and molecular basis of estrogen’s neuroprotection: potential relevance for Alzheimer’s disease. Mol Neurobiol 17:73–86, 1998 in’t Veld BA, Launer LJ, Hoes AW, et al: NSAIDs and incident Alzheimer’s disease: the Rotterdam Study. Neurobiol Aging 19:607–611, 1998 in’t Veld BA, Ruitenberg A, Hofman A, et al: Nonsteroidal antiinflammatory drugs and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 345:1515–1521, 2001 Jama JW, Launer LJ, Witteman JC, et al: Dietary antioxidants and cognitive function in a population-based sample of older persons: the Rotterdam Study. Am J Epidemiol 144:275–280, 1996 Jann MW: Rivastigmine: a new-generation cholinesterase inhibitor for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Pharmacotherapy 20:1–12, 2000 Janus C, Pearson J, McLaurin J, et al: A beta peptide immunization reduces behavioural impairment and plaques in a model of Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 408:979–982, 2000 Jick H, Zornberg GL, Jick SS, et al: Statins and the risk of dementia. Lancet 356:1627–1631, 2000 Jones RW, Wesnes KA, Kirby J: Effects of NMDA modulation in scopolamine dementia. Ann N Y Acad Sci 640:241–244, 1991 Kawahara M, Kato M, Kuroda Y: Effects of aluminum on the neurotoxicity of primary cultured neurons and on the aggregation of beta-amyloid protein. Brain Res Bull 55:211–217, 2001 Kawas C, Resnick S, Morrison A, et al: A prospective study of estrogen replacement therapy and the risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease: the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neurology 48:1517–1521, 1997 Kennedy JS, Polinsky RJ, Johnson B, et al: Preferential cerebrospinal fluid acetylcholinesterase inhibition by rivastigmine in humans. J Clin Psychopharmacol 19:513–521, 1999 Ki CS, Na DL, Kim JW, et al: No association between the genes for butyrylcholinesterase K variant and apolipoprotein E4 in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Med Genet 88:113–115, 1999
476
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Kim KW, Jhoo JH, Lee JH, et al: Neither the butyrylcholinesterase K variant nor transferrin C2 variant confers a risk for Alzheimer’s disease in Koreans. J Neural Transm 108:1159–1166, 2001 Klein WL, Krafft GA, Finch CE: Targeting small Abeta oligomers: the solution to an Alzheimer’s disease conundrum? Trends Neurosci 24:219–224, 2001 Kojro E, Gimpl G, Lammich S, et al: Low cholesterol stimulates the nonamyloidogenic pathway by its effect on the alpha-secretase ADAM 10. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:5815–5820, 2001 Laine K, Palovaara S, Tapanainen P, et al: Plasma tacrine concentrations are significantly increased by concomintant hormone replacement therapy. Clin Pharmacol Ther 66:602–608, 1999 Le Bars PL, Katz MM, Berman N, et al: A placebo-controlled, double-blind, randomized trial of an extract of Ginkgo biloba for dementia. North American EGb Study Group. JAMA 278:1327–1332, 1997 LeBlanc ES, Janowsky J, Chan BK, et al: Hormone replacement therapy and cognition: systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA 285:1489–1499, 2001 Lee DW, Liu HC, Liu TY, et al: No association between butyrylcholinesterase K– variant and Alzheimer disease in Chinese. Am J Med Genet 96:167–169, 2000 Li YM, Xu M, Lai MT, et al: Photoactivated gamma-secretase inhibitors directed to the active site covalently label presenilin 1. Nature 405:689–694, 2000 Lovestone S, Davis DR, Webster MT, et al: Lithium reduces tau phosphorylation: effects in living cells and in neurons at therapeutic concentrations. Biol Psychiatry 45:995–1003, 1999 Luine V, Park D, Joh T, et al: Immunochemical demonstration of increased choline acetyltransferase concentration in rat preoptic area after estradiol administration. Brain Res 191:273–277, 1980 Lynch G, Granger R, Ambros-Ingerson J, et al: Evidence that a positive modulator of AMPA-type glutamate receptors improves delayed recall in aged humans. Exp Neurol 145:89–92, 1997 Maelicke A: Allosteric modulation of nicotinic receptors as a treatment strategy for Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 11 (suppl 1):11–18, 2000 Maelicke A, Samochocki M, Jostock R, et al: Allosteric sensitization of nicotinic receptors by galantamine, a new treatment strategy for Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 49:279–288, 2001 Mark RJ, Pang Z, Geddes JW, et al: Amyloid beta-peptide impairs glucose transport in hippocampal and cortical neurons: involvement of membrane lipid peroxidation. J Neurosci 17:1046–1054, 1997 Martinez-Serrano A, Bjorklund A: Ex vivo nerve growth factor gene transfer to the basal forebrain in presymptomatic middle-aged rats prevents the development of cholinergic neuron atrophy and cognitive impairment during aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:1858–1863, 1998 Marx J: Alzheimer’s disease: bad for the heart, bad for the mind? Science 294:508– 509, 2001
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
477
Masaki KH, Losonczy KG, Izmirlian G, et al: Association of vitamin E and C supplement use with cognitive function and dementia in elderly men. Neurology 54:1265–1272, 2000 Matsuoka Y, Picciano M, La Francois J, et al: Fibrillar beta-amyloid evokes oxidative damage in a transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroscience 104:609–613, 2001 McGeer PL: Cyclo-oxygenase-2 inhibitors: rationale and therapeutic potential for Alzheimer’s disease. Drugs Aging 17:1–11, 2000 McGeer PL, McGeer EG: The inflammatory response system of brain: implications for therapy of Alzheimer and other neurodegenerative diseases. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 21:195–218, 1995 McGeer PL, Schulzer M, McGeer EG: Arthritis and anti-inflammatory agents as possible protective factors for Alzheimer’s disease: a review of 17 epidemiologic studies. Neurology 47:425–432, 1996 Meins W, Muller-Thomsen T, Meier-Baumgartner HP: Subnormal serum vitamin B12 and behavioural and psychological symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 15:415–418, 2000 Mohr E, Schlegel J, Fabbrini G, et al: Clonidine treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 46:376–378, 1989 Morgan D, Diamond DM, Gottschall PE, et al: A beta peptide vaccination prevents memory loss in an animal model of Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 408:982–985, 2000 Morris MC, Beckett LA, Scherr PA, et al: Vitamin E and vitamin C supplement use and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 12:121– 126, 1998 Mulnard RA, Cotman CW, Kawas C, et al: Estrogen replacement therapy for treatment of mild to moderate Alzheimer disease: a randomized controlled trial. Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study. JAMA 283:1007–1015, 2000 Multhaup G, Ruppert T, Schlicksupp A, et al: Reactive oxygen species and Alzheimer’s disease. Biochem Pharmacol 54:533–539, 1997 Munoz FJ, Inestrosa NC: Neurotoxicity of acetylcholinesterase amyloid beta-peptide aggregates is dependent on the type of Abeta peptide and the AChE concentration present in the complexes. FEBS Lett 450:205–209, 1999 Nathan L, Chaudhuri G: Antioxidant and prooxidant actions of estrogens: potential physiological and clinical implications. Semin Reprod Endocrinol 16:309–314, 1998 Oka A, Beilliveau MF, Rosenberg PA, et al: Vulnerability of oligodendroglia to glutamate: pharmacology, mechanisms, and prevention. J Neurosci 13:141– 153, 1993 Olanow CW, Koller WC: An algorithm (decision tree) for the management of Parkinson’s disease: treatment guidelines. Neurology 50 (suppl 3):1–57, 1998
478
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Olton DS, Wenk GL: Dementia: animal models of the cognitive impairments produced by the degeneration of the basal forebrain cholinergic system, in Psychopharmacology: The Third Generation of Progress. Edited by Meltzer HY. New York, Raven, 1987, pp 941–953 Paganini-Hill A, Henderson VW: Estrogen replacement therapy and risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch Intern Med 156:2213–2217, 1996 Paleologos M, Cumming RG, Lazarus R: Cohort study of vitamin C intake and cognitive impairment. Am J Epidemiol 148:45–50, 1998 Pedersen WA, McCullers D, Culmsee C, et al: Corticotropin-releasing hormone protects neurons against insults relevant to the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Dis 8:492–503, 2001 Poduslo JF, Curran GL: Amyloid beta peptide as a vaccine for Alzheimer’s disease involves receptor-mediated transport at the blood-brain barrier. Neuroreport 12:3197–3200, 2001 Poirier J: Apolipoprotein E: a pharmacogenetic target for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Mol Diagn 4:335–341, 1999 Puglielli L, Konopka G, Pack-Chung E, et al: Acyl-coenzyme A: cholesterol acyltransferase modulates the generation of the amyloid beta-peptide. Nat Cell Biol 3:905–912, 2001 Raskind MA, Peskind ER, Wessel T, et al: Galantamine in AD: a 6-month randomized, placebo-controlled trial with a 6-month extension. Neurology 54:2261– 2268, 2000 Rattray M: Is there nicotinic modulation of nerve growth factor? Implications for cholinergic therapies in Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 49:185–193, 2001 Refolo LM, Pappolla MA, LaFrancois J, et al: A cholesterol-lowering drug reduces beta-amyloid pathology in a transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Dis 8:890–899, 2001 Rockwood K, Beattie BL, Eastwood MR, et al: A randomized, controlled trial of linopirdine in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Can J Neurol Sci 24:140– 145, 1997 Rodriguez-Martin JL, Lopez-Arrieta JM, Qizilbash N: Thiamine for Alzheimer’s disease. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2:CD001498, 2000 Rogers J, Kirby LC, Hempelman SR, et al: Clinical trial of indomethacin in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 43:1609–1611, 1993 Rogers SL, Doody RS, Mohs RC, et al: Donepezil improves cognition and global function in Alzheimer disease: a 15-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Donepezil Study Group. Arch Intern Med 158:1021–1031, 1998a Rogers SL, Farlow MR, Doody RS, et al: A 24-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of donepezil in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Donepezil Study Group. Neurology 50:136–145, 1998b Rosen WG, Mohs RC, Davis KL: A new rating scale for Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 14:1356–1364, 1984 Rosenberg MB, Friedmann T, Robertson RO, et al: Grafting genetically modified cells to the damaged brain: restorative effects of NGF expression. Science 242:1575–1577, 1988
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
479
Rosler M, Anand R, Cicin-Sain A, et al: Efficacy and safety of rivastigmine in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: international randomised controlled trial. BMJ 318:633–638, 1999 Ruether E, Husmann R, Kinzler E, et al: A 28-week, double-blind, placebocontrolled study with Cerebrolysin in patients with mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 16:253–263, 2001 Samochocki M, Zerlin M, Jostock R, et al: Galantamine is an allosterically potentiating ligand of the human alpha4/beta2 nAChR. Acta Neurol Scand 176:68–73, 2000 Sano M, Ernesto C, Thomas RG, et al: A controlled trial of selegiline, alphatocopherol, or both as treatment for Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 336:1216–1222, 1997 Santucci AC, Haroutunian V, Davis KL: Pharmacological alleviation of combined cholinergic/noradrenergic lesion–induced memory deficits in rats. Clin Neuropharmacol 14 (suppl 1):1–8, 1991 Savory J, Exley C, Forbes WF, et al: Can the controversy of the role of aluminum in Alzheimer’s disease be resolved? What are the suggested approaches to this controversy and methodological issues to be considered? J Toxicol Environ Health 48:615–635, 1996 Schmidt R, Hayn M, Reinhart B, et al: Plasma antioxidants and cognitive performance in middle-aged and older adults: results of the Austrian Stroke Prevention Study. J Am Geriatr Soc 46:1407–1410, 1998 Schneider LS, Farlow M: Combined tacrine and estrogen replacement therapy in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 826:317–322, 1997 Schrattenholz A, Periera EF, Roth U, et al: Agonist responses of neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors are potentiated by a novel class of allosterically acting ligands. Mol Pharmacol 49:1–6, 1996 Segarra AC, McEwen BS: Estrogen increases spine density in ventromedial hypothalamic neurons of peripubertal rats. Neuroendocrinology 54:365–372, 1991 Selkoe DJ: Toward a comprehensive theory for Alzheimer’s disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 924:17–25, 2000 Selkoe DJ: Alzheimer’s disease: genes, proteins, and therapy. Physiol Rev 81:741– 766, 2001 Shepherd J, Blauw GJ, Murphy MB, et al: The design of a prospective study of Pravastatin in the Elderly at Risk (PROSPER). PROSPER Study Group. Prospective Study of Pravastatin in the Elderly at Risk. Am J Cardiol 84:1192– 1197, 1999 Shumaker SA, Reboussin BA, Espeland MA, et al: The Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study (WHIMS): a trial of the effect of estrogen therapy in preventing and slowing the progression of dementia. Control Clin Trials 19:604–621, 1998 Sparks DL, Scheff SW, Hunsaker JC III, et al: Induction of Alzheimer-like betaamyloid immunoreactivity in the brains of rabbits with dietary cholesterol. Exp Neurol 126:88–94, 1994 Squire LR: Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol Rev 99:195–231, 1992
480
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Stern RG, Davis KL: Research in treating cognitive impairment in Alzheimer’s disease, in The Dementias: Diagnosis, Management, and Research, 2nd Edition. Edited by Weiner MF. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1996, pp 331–354 Stewart WF, Kawas C, Corrada M, et al: Risk of Alzheimer’s disease and duration of NSAID use. Neurology 48:626–632, 1997 Strittmatter WJ, Weisgraber KH, Goedert M, et al: Hypothesis: microtubule instability and paired helical filament formation in the Alzheimer disease brain are related to apolipoprotein E genotype. Exp Neurol 152:163–171, 1994 Tang MX, Jacobs D, Stern Y, et al: Effect of oestrogen during menopause on risk and age at onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 348:429–432, 1996 Tariot PN, Solomon PR, Morris JC, et al: A 5-month, randomized, placebocontrolled trial of galantamine in AD. Neurology 54:2269–2276, 2000 Thal LJ, Forrest M, Loft H, et al: Lu 25–109, a muscarinic agonist, fails to improve cognition in Alzheimer’s disease. Lu25–109 Study Group. Neurology 54:421– 426, 2000 Thatte U: AN-1792 (Elan). Curr Opin Investig Drugs 2:663–667, 2001 Tsai GE, Falk WE, Gunther J, et al: Improved cognition in Alzheimer’s disease with short-term D-cycloserine treatment. Am J Psychiatry 156:467–469, 1999 van Dongen MC, van Rossum E, Kessels AG, et al: The efficacy of ginkgo for elderly people with dementia and age-associated memory impairment: new results of a randomized clinical trial. J Am Geriatr Soc 48:1183–1194, 2000 Van Gool WA, Weinstein HC, Scheltens PK, et al: Effect of hydroxychloroquine on progression of dementia in early Alzheimer’s disease: an 18-month randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Lancet 358:455–460, 2001 Vassar R, Bennett BD, Babu-Khan S, et al: Beta-secretase cleavage of Alzheimer’s amyloid precursor protein by the transmembrane aspartic protease BACE. Science 286:735–741, 1999 Wang YE, Yue DX, Tang XC: Anti-cholinesterase activity of huperzine A. Acta Pharmacolica Sinensis 7:110–113, 1986 Weggen S, Eriksen JL, Das P, et al: A subset of NSAIDs lower amyloidogenic Abeta42 independently of cyclooxygenase activity. Nature 414:212–216, 2001 Wenk GL, Quack G, Moebius HJ, et al: No interaction of memantine with acetylcholinesterase inhibitors approved for clinical use. Life Sci 66:1079–1083, 2000 Weyer G, Babej-Dolle RM, Hadler D, et al: A controlled study of 2 doses of idebenone in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychobiology 36:73– 82, 1997 White HK, Levin ED: Four-week nicotine skin patch treatment effects on cognitive performance in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 143:158– 165, 1999 Winblad B, Poritis N: Memantine in severe dementia: results of the 9M-Best Study (benefit and efficacy in severely demented patients during treatment with memantine). Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 14:135–146, 1999
Advances in the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease
481
Wolfe MS, Xia W, Moore CL, et al: Peptidomimetic probes and molecular modeling suggest that Alzheimer’s gamma-secretase is an intramembrane-cleaving aspartyl protease. Biochemistry 38:4720–4727, 1999 Wolozin B, Kellman W, Ruosseau P, et al: Decreased prevalence of Alzheimer disease associated with 3-hydroxy-3-methylglutaryl coenzyme A reductase inhibitors. Arch Neurol 57:1439–1443, 2000 Xiao XQ, Wang R, Tang XC: Protective effects of huperzine A on beta-amyloid (25–35) induced oxidative injury in rat pheochromocytoma cells. Neurosci Lett 286:155–158, 2000 Yamada KA: Therapeutic potential of positive AMPA receptor modulators in the treatment of neurological disease. Expert Opin Investig Drugs 9:765–778, 2000 Zeng H, Zhang Y, Peng L, et al: Nicotine and amyloid formation. Biol Psychiatry 49:248–257, 2001 Zhang JM, Hu GY: Huperzine A, a nootropic alkaloid, inhibits N-methyl-Daspartate–induced current in rat dissociated hippocampal neurons. Neuroscience 105:663–669, 2001 Zhang SL: Therapeutic effects of huperzine A on the aged with memory impairment. New Drugs and Clinical Remedies 5:260–262, 1986
This page intentionally left blank
APPENDIX
A
Dementia Questionnaire
Patient Name _________________________ Age __________ Date __________________ Informant ___________________________________________ Relation to patient _____________________________________ MEMORY Does _________________________________________________________________________ have any problem with: Yes
No
Don’t know
Date
1) Memory
______ ______ ______ ______
2) Remembering people’s names
______ ______ ______ ______
3) Recognizing familiar faces
______ ______ ______ ______
4) Finding way about indoors
______ ______ ______ ______
5) Finding way on familiar streets
______ ______ ______ ______
6) Remembering a short list of items
______ ______ ______ ______
7) Did trouble with memory begin suddenly
______ or slowly ______
8) Has the course of the memory problems been a steady downhill progression or have there been abrupt declines ______ ______ ______ EXPRESSION 9) Ever have trouble finding the right word or expressing self ______ ______ ______ ______ 10) Talking become less over time
______ ______ ______ ______
11) Tendency to dwell in the past
______ ______ ______ ______
DAILY FUNCTIONING 12) Trouble with household tasks
______ ______ ______ ______
13) Trouble handling money
______ ______ ______ ______
14) Trouble grasping situations or explanations
______ ______ ______ ______
15) Difficulty at work (check if N/A____)
______ ______ ______ ______
16) Trouble dressing or caring for self
______ ______ ______ ______
17) Trouble feeding self
______ ______ ______ ______
18) Trouble controlling bladder and bowels
______ ______ ______ ______
19) Agitation and nervousness
______ ______ ______ ______
483
484
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Yes
No
Don’t know
Date
OTHER PROBLEMS 20) High blood pressure
______ ______ ______ ______
21) Stroke
______ ______ ______ ______
22) More than one stroke
______ ______ ______ ______
23) Is one side of the body weaker than other side
______ ______ ______ ______
24) Parkinson’s disease (tremors, shuffling gait, rigidity of limbs) ______ ______ ______ ______ 25) Injury to the head resulting in a loss of consciousness for more than a second or two ______ ______ ______ ______ 26) Seizure or fits
______ ______ ______ ______
27) Syphilis
______ ______ ______ ______
28) Diabetes
______ ______ ______ ______
29) Drinking problem (describe type and amount consumed per day, duration, blackouts, DTs) _______________________________________ 30) Did memory problems coincide with drinking
______ ______ ______ ______
31) Ever depressed or sad for 2 weeks or more
______ ______ ______ ______
32) If yes, ever seek treatment
______ ______ ______ ______
33) Ever very high, euphoric, top of the world
______ ______ ______ ______
34) If yes, ever seek treatment
______ ______ ______ ______
35) Ever seek psychiatric or psychological help for any reason
______ ______ ______ ______
36) If yes, ever hospitalized for psychiatric illness
______ ______ ______ ______
Where? ____________________________________________________________ 37) Down syndrome (subject or family member)
______ ______ ______ ______
38) Other medical problems we have not talked about ________________________________________________________________ MEDICAL CONTACTS 39) Name and address of doctors seen for memory or related problems: ____________________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________________ 40) Ever receive medications
______ ______ ______ ______
41) A neurological exam or a psychiatric exam
______ ______ ______ ______
42) CAT scan or MRI scan
______ ______ ______ ______
43) What was diagnosis given for problems ___________________________________________ RECOGNITION OF PROBLEM 44) Who was the first person to notice something wrong ________________________________ 45) What was noticed ___________________________________________________________ 46) When was the last time (the subject seemed to be really well, or his old self) ____________________________________________________________________________
Dementia Questionnaire
485
MEDICATIONS 47) What medications are currently being taken ________________________________________ FAMILY HISTORY 48) Anyone else in the family with similar problems ______ Yes ______ No. If yes, what is the name of the person ______________________________________________; relationship __________________________; age __________. How can we obtain history about that person _______________________________________ _____________________________________________________________________________ Source. Adapted from Breitner JCS, Folstein MF: “Familial Alzheimer Dementia: A Prevalent Disorder With Specific Features.” Psychological Medicine 14:63–80, 1984. Used with permission.
This page intentionally left blank
APPENDIX
B
Mental Status Examination
Patient name _________________________________ Handedness ___ R ___ L
Age ____
Years of education_____ 1.
Sex ___
Date:___/___/___
Marital status _________
Occupation _________________________
APPEARANCE AND BEHAVIOR
Norm Abn Unk
Grooming and dress (circle one):
0
1
Hearing
0
1
9
Eyesight
0
1
9
Wandering
0
1
9
Verbal aggression
0
1
9
Sundowning
0
1
9
2.
9
BEHAVIORAL HISTORY OR OBSERVATION
Physical aggression
0
1
9
Apathy
0
1
9
Crying
0
1
9
Blunted
0
1
9
Shallow
0
1
9
Labile
0
1
9
Appropriateness
0
1
9
Other
0
1
9
3.
ORIENTATION
Time
day
date
month
Place
city
hospital
home address
Person
name
age
birth date
4.
year
EMOTION (AFFECT)
487
488
5.
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
MOOD
Norm Abn Unk
Depressed
0
1
9
Euphoric
0
1
9
Other
0
1
9
6.
THOUGHT PROCESS
Associations Loose
0
1
9
Klang
0
1
9
Other
0
1
9
Flow Tangential
0
1
9
Circumstantial
0
1
9
Flight of ideas
0
1
9
Blockage
0
1
9
Derailment
0
1
9
Perseveration
0
1
9
Other
0
1
9
Delusions
0
1
9
Hallucinations
0
1
9
Illusions
0
1
9
Suspicions
0
1
9
Misidentification syndrome
0
1
9
7.
8.
THOUGHT CONTENT
ATTENTION
3-7 2-4-9
Digits forward:
0
1
9
8-5-2-7 2-9-6-8-3
Digits reversed:
0
1
9
0
1
9
5-7-1-9-4-6 8-1-5-9-3-6-2 3-9-8-2-5-1-4-7 CONCENTRATION Subtraction of 3s (20, 17, 14, 11, 8, 5, 2, -1) 9. LANGUAGE Articulation
0
1
9
Fluency overall
0
1
9
increase
decrease
neologisms
paraphasias
other
delayed word finding
Mental Status Examination
489
Norm Abn Unk 10. Name as many animals as you can in 1 minute:
0
1
9
(normal=18±6)
11. EXPRESSIVE LANGUAGE
None
Word finding difficulty in
Mild– mod Severe Unk
0
1
2
9
0
1
2
9
spontaneous speech: Global rating of amount of spoken language:
Norm Abn Unk 0 1 9
12. COMPREHENSION Point to floor
ceiling
desk, chair, door
Are you 150 years old?
Is the sky green?
Do you put on your coat before or after your shirt/blouse? If you cross from the north to the south side of the street, which side are you on? Is my cousin’s mother a man or a woman? 13. REPETITION walk
hospital
0
1
9
0
1
9
Mississippi River
the little boy next door I saw the train arrive yesterday 14. NAMING watch
back
crystal
band
stem
15. READING ALOUD (see p. 492) P
0
1
9
I am going to a movie
G
R
0
1
9
It is a thriller and bound to be scary
0
1
9
16. WRITING (see p. 494) Dictated sentence
0
1
9
Spontaneous sentence
0
1
9
0
1
9
book
0
1
9
chair
0
1
9
green
0
1
9
17. MEMORY Remote: Recent:
Name four presidents during your lifetime unrelated words
5 minutes
cued
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
490
18. PRAXIS
Norm Abn Unk
Imitation
0
1
9
Ideomotor: Salute flag
0
1
9
Comb hair
0
1
9
Blow out match
0
1
9
Construction (see p. 493):
0
1
9
intersecting rectangles
0
1
9
Greek cross
0
1
9
cube
0
1
9
0
1
9
0
1
9
Don’t cry over spilled milk.
0
1
9
People who live in glass houses shouldn’t throw stones.
0
1
9
Overall rating
0
1
9
19. HIGHER COGNITIVE FUNCTION Fund of information How many weeks in a year? Why do people have lungs? Name four states Where is Denmark? How far is it from New York to Los Angeles? Who wrote the Odyssey?
Why are light colored clothes cooler in the summer than dark colored clothes? Who was president during the American Civil War? What causes rust? What is the Koran?
20. CALCULATION 2´2=4, 2´4=8, 2´8=16, … (through 1,024) 21. PROVERBS: What do people mean when they say...
22. SIMILARITIES: An apple and a banana are similar in that they are fruits. What is the similarity between... potato cat
— carrot — rabbit
airplane — motorcycle dancing — swimming hot
— cold — tongue
nose Overall rating
0
1
9
0
1
9
0
1
9
0
1
9
0
1
9
0
1
9
0
1
9
23. JUDGMENT—What would you do if... You found a sealed, addressed, stamped envelope on the street?
0
1
9
You were in a crowded theater when a fire broke out?
0
1
9
Overall assessment
0
1
9
Mental Status Examination
24. INSIGHT: 0=normal; total insight into illness and implications 1=partial awareness of disease or implications 2=unaware of or denial of symptoms or illness 3=uncertain or irrelevant response or not applicable DIAGNOSIS (DSM-IV-TR) Axis I:
Clinical syndromes or conditions that are the focus of treatment:
Axis II:
Personality or specific developmental disorders:
Axis III: Physical disorders and conditions:
491
492
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Mental Status Examination
493
494
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Dictated sentence: Spontaneous sentence: Draw figures below:
APPENDIX
C
Neurological Examination
Patient Name: Handedness: R Age:
L Sex:
Date: __/__/__
Date: Cranial nerves
Normal
Abnormal
N/A
1.
Olfaction:
0
1
9
2.
Visual acuity:
0
1
9
3.
Pupils (ERRLA):
0
N=neurologic
9
P=physical 4.
Visual fields:
0
1
9
5.
Fund:
0
1
9
6.
EOMs:
0
1
9
7.
Facial sensation:
0
1
9
8.
Muscles of mastication:
0
1
9
9. 10.
Jaw jerk:
0
1
9
Facial muscles:
0
N=neurologic
9
P=physical 11.
Auditory acuity:
0
1=unilateral
9
2=bilateral 12.
Palate elevation:
0
N=neurologic
9
P=physical 13.
Cough, gag, swallow:
0
1
9
14.
Traps and SCM (CNXI):
0
1
9
15.
Tongue:
0
1
9
F=focal
9
Motor 16.
Bulk:
0
D=diffuse 17.
Spasticity (tone):
0
F=focal D=diffuse
495
9
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
496
18.
Normal
Abnormal
N/A
0
F=focal
9
Fasciculations:
D=diffuse 19.
Paratonia:
0
F=focal
9
D=diffuse 20.
“Lead pipe” rigidity
None Mild Moderate Severe N/A 0 1 2 3 9
a) Right extremities: b) Left extremities:
0
1
2
3
9
Note: Do not consider cogwheel rigidity here. 21.
Strength: 5=normal 4=reduced strength 3=unable to overcome
22.
2=unable to overcome gravity 1=trace of movement only 0=no strength 9=N/A
Muscle stretch reflexes (circle one for each side) a) Right
b) Left reflexes reduced but present
1 2
normal
1 2
3
increased reflexes
3
4
nonsustained clonus
4
5
sustained clonus
5
0
absent reflexes
0
9
N/A
9 Normal
Abnormal
N/A
Primitive reflexes (check all that apply) palmomental snout grasp suck
0
1
9
24.
Extensor plantar response:
0
1
9
25.
Finger-nose-finger:
0
1
9
26.
Heel-knee-shin:
0
1
9
27.
Luria maneuver:
0
1
9
23.
28.
Go–no-go:
0
1
9
29.
Motor impersistence:
0
1
9
30.
Perseveration:
0
1
9
31.
Reciprocal motor command:
0
1
9
32.
Rapid alternating movements Right Impairment
None Mild Moderate Severe N/A
a) Finger or hand clasps b) Heel taps
0 0
1 1
2 2
3 3
9 9
c) Prone/supine
0
1
2
3
9
Neurological Examination
Left Impairment
33.
34.
36.
None Mild Moderate Severe N/A
d) Finger or hand clasps e) Heel taps
0 0
1 1
2 2
3 3
9 9
f) Prone/supine
0
1
2
3
9
Abnormal movements
Normal
Abnormal
N/A
a) Benign essential tremor:
0
1
9
b) Myoclonus:
0
1
9
c) Dyskinesia
0
1
9
d) Rest tremor–right extremities:
0
1
9
e) Rest tremor–left extremities:
0
1
9
f) Action tremor–right extremities:
0
1
9
g) Action tremor–left extremities:
0
1
9
Normal
Abnormal
N/A
0
F=focal
9
Sensation (decreased) (mark all that apply)
35.
Light touch
Cold
Vibration Pinprick Joint position sense
Gait and station Gait and posture:
D=diffuse
Normal
Abnormal
N/A
0
1
9
Other extrapyramidal symptoms a) Body bradykinesia:
0
1
9
0
1
9
b) Postural stability:
0
1
9
c) Arising from chair:
0
1
9
d) Voice (hypovocalization):
0
1
9
e) Facial movement (hypomimia):
0
1
9
f) Turning en bloc:
0
1
9
g) Cogwheel rigidity:
0
1
9
37.
Patient cooperativeness (mark one) 0=fully cooperative 1=mildly to moderately cooperative 2=very uncooperative 9=couldn’t examine/don’t know
38.
Overall neurological assessment
39.
497
Normal
Abnormal
N/A
(excluding mental status)
0
1
9
Overall mental status
0
1
9
Notes:
This page intentionally left blank
APPENDIX
D 1)
Blessed Dementia Rating Scale
Memory and performance of everyday activities
Loss of ability (Score: 0=none; 0.5=sometimes; 1=frequently) A. Ability to perform household tasks B. Ability to cope with small sums of money C. Ability to remember a short list of items (e.g., shopping list) D. Ability to find way about indoors (patient's home or other familiar locations) E. Ability to find way around familiar streets F.
2)
Ability to grasp situations or explanations
0
0.5
1
0
0.5
1
0
0.5
1
0
0.5
1
0
0.5
1
0
0.5
1
G. Ability to recall recent events
0
0.5
1
H. Tendency to dwell in the past
0
0.5
1
Habits A. Eating 0=Feeds self without assistance 1=Feeds self with minor assistance 2=Feeds self with much assistance 3=Has to be fed B. Dressing 0=Unaided 1=Occasionally misplaces buttons, etc., requires minor help 2=Wrong sequence, forgets items, requires much assistance 3=Unable to dress C. Toilet 0=Clean, cares for self at toilet 1=Occasional incontinence, or needs to be reminded 2=Frequent incontinence, or much assistance 3=No control
3)
TOTAL SCORE OF ALL ITEMS (maximum score 17)
Source. Adapted from Blessed G, Tomlinson BE, Roth M: “The Association Between Quantitative Measures of Dementia and of Senile Change in the Cerebral Gray Matter of Elderly Subjects.” British Journal of Psychiatry 114:797–811, 1968. Used with permission.
499
This page intentionally left blank
APPENDIX
E
Washington University Clinical Dementia Rating Scale
Healthy CDR 0 Memory
Questionable dementia CDR 0.5
No memory loss or slight inconsistent forgetfulness
Orientation
Mild consistent forgetfulness, partial recollection of events; “benign” forgetfulness
Fully oriented Only doubtful impairment in solving problems, similarities, differences
Judgment—problem solving
Solves everyday problems well; judgment good in relation to past performance
Community affairs
Only doubtful or mild Independent function at usual level in job, shopping, impairment in these activities business, and financial affairs, volunteer and social groups
Home—hobbies
Life at home, hobbies, intellectual interests well maintained
Personal care
Fully capable of self-care
Score only impairment due to cognitive loss, not impairment due to other factors.
501
Life at home, hobbies, intellectual interests slightly impaired
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
502
Mild dementia CDR 1
Moderate dementia CDR 2
Severe dementia CDR 3
Severe memory loss: only Severe memory loss: only Moderate memory loss: fragments remain highly learned material more marked for recent retained; new material events; defect interferes rapidly lost with everyday activities Some difficulty with time Usually disoriented in time, often to place relationships: oriented for place and person at examination but may have geographic disorientation Moderate difficulty in handling complex problems; social judgment usually maintained
Orientation to person only
Unable to make Severely impaired in judgments or solve handling problems, problems similarities, differences; social judgment usually impaired
No pretense of independent function outside home Unable to function independently at these Appears well enough to be Appears too ill to be taken activities though may to functions outside a still be engaged in some; taken to functions family home outside a family home may still appear normal to casual inspection Only simple chores Mild but definite preserved; very impairment of function restricted interests, at home; more difficult chores abandoned; more poorly sustained complicated hobbies and interests abandoned
No significant function in home outside of own room
Needs prompting
Requires much help with personal care; often incontinent
Requires assistance in dressing, hygiene, keeping of personal effects
Source. Reprinted from Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL: “A New Clinical Scale for the Staging of Dementia.” British Journal of Psychiatry 140:566–572, 1982. Used with permission.
APPENDIX
F
Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale
The examiner is seated facing the subject across a small table. The word recall task is administered first and is followed by the remaining cognitive tasks. Some time (as long as 10 minutes) is spent in open-ended conversation to assess various aspects of expressive speech. Noncognitive behaviors are evaluated from reports of the patient and/or reliable informants and from observations made during the interview. It is best, but not absolutely necessary, to interview subject or informant separately. The rating scale of 0–5 indicates the severity of dysfunction. 0 = no impairment on a task or absence of a particular behavior 1 = very mild 2 = mild 3 = moderate 4 = moderately severe 5 = the most severe degree of impairment or a very high frequency of a behavior Ratings of many cognitive behaviors correspond to specific levels of performance on tasks.
COGNITIVE BEHAVIORS 1. WORD RECALL TASK. The subject reads 10 high-imagery words exposed for two seconds each. Each word is printed in 5/8" (16-mm) letters on a separate 4"´6" card. The subject is asked to recall the words aloud. The cards are then shuffled and re-presented. Three trials of reading and recall are given. The score equals the mean number of words NOT recalled on three trials (maximum=10). If subject is unable to comprehend or perform task, the score is 10. Note: Language abilities are evaluated throughout the interview and on specific tests. Questions eliciting “yes” and “no” answers assess basic 503
504
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
comprehension. Other questions should require specific information and well-developed communication skills commensurate with educational level or estimated premorbid intelligence level. Do NOT rate word finding here. 2. SPOKEN LANGUAGE ABILITY. This is a global rating of speech quality such as clarity and ability to make oneself understood. Do NOT rate quantity of speech or word-finding difficulty here. 0 = no difficulty 1 = one instance of lack of understanding 2 = subject understandable >75% of the time 3 = subject understandable 50%–75% of the time 4 = subject NOT understandable 50% of the time 5 = one or two word utterances; fluent but empty speech; mute 3. COMPREHENSION. Evaluate the subject’s ability to understand speech. Do not include responses to commands. 0 = no misunderstandings 1 = one instance of misunderstanding 2 = 3–5 instances of misunderstanding 3 = requires several repetitions and rephrasing 4 = occasional correct response, such as yes-no questions 5 = rarely responds to questions appropriately, not due to poverty of speech 4. REMEMBERING TEST INSTRUCTIONS. The subject’s ability to remember the requirements of the recognition task is evaluated. On each recognition trial, the subject is asked prior to presenting the first two words, “Did you see this word before or is it a new word?” For the third word, the subject is asked, “How about this one?” If subject responds appropriately, i.e., “yes” or “no,” recall of instructions is accurate. Failure to respond indicates that instructions have been forgotten. The instruction is then repeated. The procedure for the third word is repeated for words 4– 24; each instance of recall failure is noted. 5. WORD-FINDING DIFFICULTY IN SPONTANEOUS SPEECH. Subjects have difficulty finding the desired word in spontaneous speech. They may deal with this by circumlocution or giving explanatory phrases or nearly satisfactory synonyms. Do NOT include finger and object naming in this rating.
Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale
505
0 = remembers well 1 = forgets once 2 = must be reminded twice 3 = must be reminded 3–4 times 4 = must be reminded 5–6 times 5 = must be reminded 7 or more times 0 = none 1 = 1–2 instances 2 = noticeable circumlocution or word substitution 3 = occasional loss of words without compensation 4 = frequent loss of words without compensation 5 = nearly total loss of content words; speech sounds empty; 1- to 2-word utterances 6. COMMANDS. Receptive speech is also assessed by subject’s ability to carry out 1- to 5-step commands. 1 = Make a FIST 2 = Point to the CEILING and then to the FLOOR. Line up a pencil, watch, and 4"´6" card, in that order, on the table in front of the patient. 3 = Put the PENCIL ON TOP OF THE CARD and then PUT IT BACK. 4 = Put the WATCH on the OTHER SIDE OF THE PENCIL and then TURN OVER THE CARD. 5 = Tap EACH SHOULDER TWICE, WITH TWO FINGERS, KEEPING YOUR EYES SHUT. Each capitalized element is a single step. The command may be repeated once in its entirety. Each command is scored as a whole. The rating is the number of commands performed correctly. 0 = accomplishes 5-step command 1 = accomplishes 4-step command 2 = accomplishes 3-step command 3 = accomplishes 2-step command 4 = accomplishes 1-step command 5 = cannot follow 1-step command
506
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
7. NAMING OBJECTS AND FINGERS. The subject is asked to name 12 randomly presented real objects whose frequency values (2) are high, medium, and low. Then, subjects are asked to place the dominant hand on the table and to name the fingers: thumb, index (pointer, first finger forefinger), middle, ring, and little (pinky), or other culturally acceptable name. Standard clues may be used to assist subjects having difficulty. The objects, their frequency, and the clues are: High Frequency: Flower (plastic): grows in the garden Bed (dollhouse furniture): used for sleeping Whistle: makes sound when blown Pencil: used for writing Medium Frequency: Rattle: a baby’s toy Mask: hides your face Scissors: cuts paper Comb: used on hair Low Frequency: Wallet: holds your money Harmonica: a musical instrument Stethoscope: doctor uses it to listen to your heart Tongs: picks up food 0 = 0–2 objects named incorrectly (objects + fingers named) 1 = 3–5 objects named incorrectly 2 = 6–8 objects named incorrectly 3 = 9–11 objects named incorrectly 4 = 12–14 objects named incorrectly 5 = 15–17 objects named incorrectly 8. CONSTRUCTIONAL PRAXIS. The ability to copy four geometric forms is assessed. These forms, in order of presentation, are: 1. Circle, approximately 20 cm in diameter. 2. Two overlapping rectangles forming a cross. The vertical rectangle is 20´25 cm; the horizontal, 10´35 cm. 3. Rhombus, each side 20 cm, acute £50 deg, obtuse ³130 deg. 4. Cube, each side=20 cm, with internal lines present.
Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale
507
Each form is located in the upper middle of a 5"´8" sheet of white paper. The subject is asked, “Do you see this figure?” and then told, “Make one that looks like this anywhere on the paper.” Two attempts are permitted. Scoring criteria are: 1. Circle. A closed, curved figure. 2. Two overlapping rectangles. Figures must be four-sided and overlap similar to presented form. Do NOT score for size. 3. Rhombus. Figure must be four-sided, obliquely oriented, and the sides of approximately equal length. Four measurements are taken: ac, a´c, bc, b´c. The ratio of ac/a´c ranges from 0.75 to 1.0. The ratio of bc/b´c ranges from 0.6 to 1.0. The ratio of bb´/aa´ ranges from 3 to 0.75. Figure is incorrect if any ratio is outside these ranges. 4. Cube. The form is three-dimensional with front face in the correct orientation, internal lines drawn correctly between corners. Opposite sides of faces not parallel by more than 10 deg is incorrect.
9. IDEATIONAL PRAXIS. The subject is given an 8½"´11" sheet of paper and a long envelope. Subjects are instructed to pretend to send the letter to themselves. Subjects are told to fold the paper, put the paper in the envelope, seal it, address it to themselves, and to indicate where the stamp goes. If the subject forgets part of the task or has difficulty, reinstruction is given. Impairment on this task should reflect dysfunction in executing an overlearned task and NOT recall difficulty. Task components are: 1) fold letter, 2) put letter in envelope, 3) seal envelope, 4) address envelope, and 5) mark where stamp goes. Any address containing name, street, city and state (ZIP code is optional). 0 = performs all components 1 = failure to perform 1 component 2 = failure to perform 2 components 3 = failure to perform 3 components 4 = failure to perform 4 components 5 = failure to perform 5 components 10. ORIENTATION. The components of orientation are date, month, year, day of the week, season, time of day, place, and person. One point is
508
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
given for each INCORRECT response (maximum=8). Acceptable answers include ±1 for the date, ±1 for the hour, partial name for place, naming of upcoming season within 1 week of its beginning, and name of previous season for 2 weeks after its end. 11. WORD RECOGNITION TASK. The subject reads aloud 12 high-imagery words printed in 16-mm letters on 4" ´ 6" cards. These cards are then randomly mixed with 12 words the subject has not seen. The subject indicates whether the word was shown previously. Then two more trials of reading the original words and of recognition are given. The score is the mean number of incorrect responses for the three trials (maximum = 12). If the subject is unable to perform the task, a score of 12 is given.
NONCOGNITIVE BEHAVIORS 12. TREMORS. Subject extends both hands in front of body and extends the fingers, holding this position for approximately 10 seconds. 0 = none 1 = very slight tremor, barely noticeable 2 = noticeable tremor that does not interfere with activities 3 = tremor that interferes with activities such as holding pencil or buttoning shirt 4 = tremor that interferes with gross activities such as holding a glass of water 5 = very rapid movements with sizable displacement 13. TEARFUL. Subject/informant is asked about the frequency of tearfulness in the prior week. 0 = no tears or crying 1 = occurs one time during week or testing session 2 = occurs 2–3 times during the week 3 = occasional outbursts of short duration 4 = frequent crying spells nearly every day 5 = frequent and prolonged crying spells every day 14. DEPRESSION. Subject/informant is asked about sadness, discouragement, being down. If response is positive, inquire as to severity and pervasiveness of mood, loss of interest or pleasure in activities, and reactivity
Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale
509
to environmental events. Interviewer notes presence of depressed faces and ability to respond to encouragement and jokes. Include past week. 0 = feels good 1 = feels slightly dysphoric 2 = appears and reports mild dysphoric mood, reactivity present, some loss of interest 3 = feels moderately dysphoric often 4 = feels dysphoric almost all the time with considerable loss of reactivity and interest 5 = pervasive and severe dysphoric mood; total lack of reactivity; pervasive loss of interest and/or pleasure 15. CONCENTRATION/DISTRACTIBILITY. Rate the frequency with which subject is distracted by irrelevant stimuli and/or must be reoriented to the ongoing task. Information from informant may also be used, for example, ability to watch TV, finish meals, have conversations. A score of 1 may be given subjects who are reported by the informant as distractible but do not exhibit this behavior during the interview. 0 = concentrates well 1 = one instance of poor concentration 2 = 2–3 instances of poor concentration/distractibility 3 = 4–5 instances during interview; frequent episodes at home 4 = poor concentration; highly distractible throughout much of the interview and at home 5 = extreme difficulty in concentration; extremely distractible, unable to complete tasks 16. UNCOOPERATIVE TO TESTING. Rate the amount of subject’s resistance to various aspects of the interview. 0 = no instance of uncooperativeness 1 = one instance of uncooperativeness 2 = few instances of uncooperativeness; readily complies if asked to continue 3 = several instances of uncooperativeness 4 = needs constant cajoling to complete interview 5 = refuses to complete interview 17. DELUSIONS. Rate the subject’s belief in ideas that appear untrue, for example, persons entering house, stealing possessions. In rating severity
510
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
consider conviction in delusions, preoccupation, and effect on subject’s actions. Include prior week’s observations. 0 = none 1 = one transient delusional belief 2 = delusion present, but subject questions own belief 3 = patient convinced of delusion, but belief doesn’t affect behavior 4 = delusion affects behavior 5 = significant actions based on delusions 18. HALLUCINATIONS. Inquiry about visual, auditory, and tactile hallucinations. The frequency and degree of disruptiveness of hallucinations are rated. 0 = none 1 = very mild; hears voice saying one word; visual hallucination once 2 = mild 3 = moderate; hallucinates numerous times during day, which interferes with normal functioning 4 = moderately severe 5 = severe; nearly constant hallucinating, which totally disrupts normal functioning 19. PACING. Distinguish between normal physical activity and excessive walking back and forth. Include the prior week. 0 = none 1 = very rare occurrence 2 = paces for short intervals at certain times of the day 3 = paces frequently each day 4 = paces for the greater portion of the day, but can stop for activities such as meals 5 = cannot sit still and paces excessively 20. INCREASED MOTOR ACTIVITY. Rate in relation to the subject’s normal level of motor activity or a previously obtained baseline. Include the prior week. A score of 1–2 is permissible if increased motor activity is reported by the informant but does not occur during the interview.
Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale
511
0 = none 1 = very slight increase 2 = noticeable fidgeting and restlessness 3 = moves frequently; fidgets very often 4 = significant increase in movement that interferes with other activity 5 = must be moving constantly; rarely sits still 21. INCREASED/DECREASED APPETITE. Rate relative to subject’s normal appetite or previously obtained baseline. Include prior week. 0 = none 1 = slight appetite change without weight change 2 = appetite change with weight change, but eats without encouragement 3 = appetite and weight change with encouragement needed to eat OR asks for more food 4 = moderately severe 5 = will not eat/requires forced feeding OR complains of constant hunger despite adequate food intake
512
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE ASSESSMENT SCALE SCORING Subject
Rater
Date
COGNITIVE BEHAVIORS 1. Word recall Average Error Score 2. Spoken language ability 3. Comprehension spoken language 4. Recall test instructions 5. Word finding difficulty 6. Commands: 1 2 3 4 5 7. Naming H: 1 2 3 4 Fingers: M: 1 2 3 4 TIMRP L: 1 2 3 4 8. Constructions Figures: 0 1 2 3 4 9. Ideational praxis Steps: 0 1 2 3 4 5 10. Orientation Day Year Person Time of Day Date Month Season Place 11. Word recognition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Average Error Score NONCOGNITIVE BEHAVIORS 12. Tremor 13. Tearful 14. Depressed mood 15.Concentration 16. Uncooperative to testing
17. 18. 19. 20. 21.
Delusions Hallucinations Pacing Increased motor activity Increased/decreased appetite
TOTAL SCORES Word Recall (Item 1) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Word Recognition (Item 11) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Total Cognitive Behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Other Cognitive Behaviors (Items 2–10) ....... Noncognitive Behaviors (Items 12–21) ........ Total . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
. . . . . .
. . . . . .
. . . . . .
. . . . . .
. . . . . .
. . . . . .
. . . . . .
. . . . . .
. . . . . .
. . . . . .
Source. Reprinted from Rosen WG, Mohs RC, Davis KL: “A New Rating Scale for Alzheimer’s Disease.” American Journal of Psychiatry 14:1356–1364, 1984. Used with permission. Copyright 1984 American Psychiatric Association.
APPENDIX
G
Neuropsychiatric Inventory: Community Dwelling Version
Scoring Sheet
Name (L, F, MI)
Item
Date (M/D/Y)
N/A Absent Frequency
Severity
F×S
Disruption
A. Delusions
x
0
1 2 3 4
1 2 3
___
012345
B. Hallucinations
x
0
1 2 3 4
1 2 3
___
012345
C. Agitation
x
0
1 2 3 4
1 2 3
___
012345
D. Depression/dysphoria
x
0
1 2 3 4
1 2 3
___
012345
E. Anxiety
x
0
1 2 3 4
1 2 3
___
012345
F.
x
0
1 2 3 4
1 2 3
___
012345
G. Apathy/indifference
Euphoria/elation
x
0
1 2 3 4
1 2 3
___
012345
H. Disinhibition
x
0
1 2 3 4
1 2 3
___
012345
I.
Irritability/lability
x
0
1 2 3 4
1 2 3
___
012345
J.
Aberrant motor behavior
x
0
1 2 3 4
1 2 3
___
012345
K. Night-time behavior
x
0
1 2 3 4
1 2 3
___
012345
L. Appetite/eating change
x
0
1 2 3 4
1 2 3
___
012345
1.
Informant/Caregiver
2.
Knowledge of the informant/caregiver about (S): 1)
very familiar/provides daily care
2)
somewhat familiar/often cares for (S)
3)
not very familiar, has minimal interaction with (S)
3.
If caregiver does not live with patient, how many hours per week do they see him/her?
4.
Medications of (S)
513
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
514
Instructions: Ask the informant/caregiver to indicate whether any of the (S)’s behaviors listed below occurred during the previous four weeks. If so, use the following scales to rate the frequency, severity, and amount of distress the behaviors caused the caregiver. A. Delusions Read to the caregiver: Does (S) have beliefs that you know are not true? For example, insisting that people are trying to harm him/her or steal from him/her. Has he/she said that family members are not who they say they are, or that the house is not their home? I’m not asking about mere suspiciousness: I am interested if (S) is convinced that these things are happening to him/her. Yes No N.A. _____ _____ _____ _____ _____ _____ _____ _____ _____
(If yes, proceed to subquestions 1–10). (If no, proceed to next screening question, “B. Hallucinations”). (Not applicable)
1. Does (S) believe that he/she is in danger—that others are planning to hurt him/ her? 2. Does (S) believe that others are stealing from him/her? 3. Does (S) believe that his/her spouse is having an affair? 4. Does (S) believe that unwelcome guests are living in his/her house? 5. Does (S) believe that his/her spouse or others are not who they claim to be? 6. Does (S) believe that his/her home is not his/her home? 7. Does (S) believe that family members plan to abandon him/her? 8. Does (S) believe that television or magazine figures are actually present in the home/room? [Does he/she try to talk or interact with them?] 9. Does (S) believe any other unusual things that I haven’t asked about? 10. Have the caregiver rate the global frequency, severity, and their own distress for the preceding items.
10a. Frequency Ratings (check one): 1. Occasionally—less than once per week. 2. Often—about once per week. 3. Frequently—several times per week, but less than every day. 4. Very frequently—once or more per day. 10b. Severity (check one): 1. Mild—delusions present but seem harmless, and do not upset (S) that much. 2. Moderate—delusions are stressful and upsetting to (S) and cause unusual or strange behavior. 3. Marked—delusions are very stressful and upsetting to (S) and cause a major amount of unusual or strange behavior. [PRN medications may be required to control them.] 10c. Caregiver Distress (How emotionally distressing do you find this behavior? Check one): 0. Not at all 1. Minimally 2. Mildly 3. Moderately 4. Severely 5. Very severely or extremely
Neuropsychiatric Inventory
515
B. Hallucinations Read to the caregiver: Does (S) have hallucinations such as false visions or voices? Does he/she see, hear, or experience things that are not present? By this we do not mean just mistaken beliefs such as stating that someone who has died is still alive; rather we are asking if (S) actually has abnormal experiences of sounds, or visions. Yes No N/A _____ 1. _____ 2. _____ 3. _____ 4. _____ 5. _____ 6. _____ 7. 8.
(If yes, proceed to subquestions 1–8). (If no proceed to next screening question, “C. Agitation/Aggression”). (not applicable)
Does (S) describe hearing voices or act as if he/she hears voices? Does (S) talk to people who are not there? Does (S) describe seeing things not seen by others, or behave as if he/she is seeing things not seen by others (people, animals, lights, etc.) Does (S) report smelling odors not smelled by others? Does (S) describe feeling things on his/her skin, or otherwise appear to be feeling things crawling or touching him/her? Does (S) describe tastes that are without known cause? Does (S) describe any other unusual sensory experiences? Have the caregiver rate the global frequency, severity, and their own distress for the preceding items.
8a. Frequency Ratings (check one): 1. Occasionally—less than once per week. 2. Often—about once per week. 3. Frequently—several times per week, but less than every day. 4. Very frequently—once or more per day. 8b. Severity (check one): 1. Mild—hallucinations are present but seem harmless and cause little distress for (S). 2. Moderate—hallucinations are distressing and are disruptive to (S). 3. Marked—hallucinations are very disruptive and are a major source of behavioral disturbance. PRN medications may be required to control them. 8c. Caregiver Distress (How emotionally distressing do you find this behavior? Check one): 0. Not at all 1. Minimally 2. Mildly 3. Moderately 4. Severely 5. Very severely or extremely C. Agitation/Aggression Read to the caregiver: Does (S) have periods when he/she refuses to cooperate or won’t let people help him/her? Is he/she hard to handle? Yes
(If yes, proceed to subquestions 1–9).
No
(If no, proceed to next screening question, “D. Depression/Dysphoria”).
N.A.
(not applicable)
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
516
_____ 1. _____ _____ _____ _____ _____ _____ _____
2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9.
Does (S) get upset when people are trying to care for him/her or resist activities such as bathing or changing clothes? Is (S) stubborn, having to have things his/her way? Is (S) uncooperative, resistive to help from others? Does (S) have any other behaviors that make him/her hard to handle? Does (S) shout or curse angrily? Does (S) slam doors, kick furniture, throw things? Does (S) attempt to hurt or hit others? Does (S) have any other aggressive or agitated behaviors? Have the caregiver rate the global frequency, severity, and their own distress for the preceding items.
9a. Frequency Ratings (check one): 1. Occasionally—less than once per week. 2. Often—about once per week. 3. Frequently—several times per week, but less than every day. 4. Very frequently—once or more per day. 9b. Severity Ratings (check one): 1. Mild—behavior is disruptive but can be managed with redirection or reassurance. 2. Moderate—behaviors disruptive and difficult to redirect or control. 3. Marked—agitation is very disruptive and a major source of difficulty; there may be a threat of personal harm. Medications are often required. 9c. Caregiver Distress (How emotionally distressing do you find this behavior? Check one): 0. Not at all 1. Minimally 2. Mildly 3. Moderately 4. Severely 5. Very severely or extremely D. Depression/Dysphoria Read to the caregiver: Does (S) seem sad or depressed? Does he/she say that he/she feels sad or depressed? Yes No N.A. _____ _____ _____ _____ _____ _____
1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6.
_____ 7. _____ 8. 9.
(If yes, proceed to subquestions 1–9). (If no, proceed to next screening question, “E. Anxiety”). (Not applicable)
Does (S) have periods of tearfulness or sobbing that seem to indicate sadness? Does (S) say or act as if he/she is sad or in low spirits? Does (S) put him/herself down or say that he/she feels like a failure? Does (S) say that he/she is a bad person or deserves to be punished? Does (S) seem very discouraged or say that he/she has no future? Does (S) say he/she is a burden to the family or that the family would be better off without him/her? Does (S) express a wish for death or talk about killing him/herself? Does (S) show any other signs of depression or sadness? Have the caregiver rate the global frequency, severity, and their own distress for the preceding items.
Neuropsychiatric Inventory
517
9a. Frequency Ratings (check one): 1. Occasionally—less than once per week. 2. Often—about once per week. 3. Frequently—several times per week, but less than every day. 4. Very frequently—essentially continuously present. 9b. Severity Ratings (check one): 1. Mild—depression is distressing, but usually responds to redirection or reassurance. 2. Moderate—depression is distressing, depressive symptoms are spontaneously voiced by (S) and difficult to alleviate. 3. Marked—depression is very distressing and a major source of suffering for (S). 9c. Caregiver Distress (How emotionally distressing do you find this behavior? Check one): 0. Not at all 1. Minimally 2. Mildly 3. Moderately 4. Severely 5. Very severely or extremely E. Anxiety Read to the caregiver: Is (S) very nervous, worried, or frightened for no reason? Does he/she seem very tense or fidgety? Is (S) afraid to be apart from you? Yes No N.A _____ 1. _____ 2. _____ 3. _____ 4. _____ 5. _____ 6. _____ 7. 8.
(If yes, proceed to subquestions 1–8). (If no, proceed to next screening question, “F. Elation/Euphoria”). (not applicable) Does (S) say that he/she is worried about planned events? Does (S) have periods of feeling shaky, unable to relax, or feeling excessively tense? Does (S) have periods of or complain of shortness of breath, gasping, or sighing for no reason other than nervousness? Does (S) complain of butterflies in his/her stomach, or of racing or pounding of the heart in association with nervous? [Symptoms not explained by ill health]. Does (S) avoid certain places or situations that make him/her more nervous such as riding in the car, meeting with friends, or being in crowds? Does (S) become nervous or upset when separated from you [or his/her caregiver]? [Does he/she cling to you to keep from being separated?] Does (S) show any other signs of anxiety? Have the caregiver rate the frequency, severity, and their own distress for the preceding items.
8a. Frequency Ratings (check one): 1. Occasionally—less than once per week. 2. Often—about once per week. 3. Frequently—several times per week, but less than every day. 4. Very frequently—once or more per day. 8b. Severity Ratings (check one): 1. Mild—anxiety is stressful for, but usually responds to redirection or reassurance. 2. Moderate—anxiety is stressful, anxiety symptoms are spontaneously voiced by (S) and difficult to alleviate. 3. Marked—anxiety is very distressing and a major source of suffering.
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
518
8c. Caregiver Distress (How emotionally distressing do you find this behavior? Check one): 0. Not at all 1. Minimally 2. Mildly 3. Moderately 4. Severely 5. Very severely or extremely F. Elation/Euphoria Read to the caregiver: Does (S) seem too cheerful or too happy for no reason? I don’t mean the normal happiness that comes from seeing friends, receiving presents, or spending time with family members. I am asking if (S) has a persistent and abnormally good mood or finds humor where others do not. Yes No N.A. _____ 1. _____ 2. _____ 3. _____ 4. _____ 5. _____ 6. _____ 7. 8.
(If yes, proceed to subquestions 1–8). (If no, proceed to next screening question, “G. Apathy/Indifference”). (Not applicable)
Does (S) appear to feel too good or to be too happy, different from his/her usual self? Does (S) find humor and laugh at things that others do not find funny? Does (S) seem to have a childish sense of humor with a tendency to giggle or laugh inappropriately (such as when something unfortunate happens to others)? Does (S) tell jokes or make remarks that have little humor? Does he/she play childish pranks such as pinching or playing “keep away” for the fun of it? Does (S) “talk big” or claim to have more abilities or wealth than is true? Does (S) show any other signs of feeling too good or being too happy? Have the caregiver rate the global frequency, severity, and their own distress for the preceding items.
8a. Frequency Ratings (check one): 1. Occasionally—less than once per week. 2. Often—about once per week. 3. Frequently—several times per week, but less than every day. 4. Very frequently—essentially continuously present. 8b. Severity Ratings (check one): 1. Mild—(S) is too happy at times. 2. Moderate—(S) is too happy at times, and this sometimes causes strange behavior. 3. Marked—(S) is almost always too happy and finds nearly everything to be funny. 8c. Caregiver Distress (How emotionally distressing do you find this behavior? Check one): 0. Not at all 1. Minimally 2. Mildly 3. Moderately 4. Severely 5. Very severely or extremely
Neuropsychiatric Inventory
519
G. Apathy/Indifference Read to the caregiver: Does (S) sit quietly without paying attention to things going on around him/ her? Has (S) lost interest in the world around him/her? Has (S) lost interest in doing things or lack motivation for participating in activities? Is it difficult to involve (S) in conversation or in doing chores? Yes No N.A. _____ _____ _____ _____ _____ _____ _____ _____
1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9.
(If yes, proceed to subquestions 1–9). (If no, proceed to next screening question, “H. Disinhibition”). (Not applicable)
Does (S) seem less spontaneous and less active than usual? Is (S) less likely to initiate a conversation? Is (S) less affectionate or lacking in emotions when compared to his/her usual self? Does (S) contribute less to household chores? Does (S) seem less interested in the activities and plans of others? Has (S) lost interest in friends and family members? Is (S) less enthusiastic about his/her usual interests? Does (S) show any other signs that he/she doesn’t care about doing new things? Have the caregiver rate the global frequency, severity, and their own distress for the preceding items.
9a. Frequency Ratings (check one): 1. Occasionally—less than once per week. 2. Often—about once per week. 3. Frequently—several times per week, but less than every day. 4. Very frequently—nearly always present. 9b. Severity Ratings (check one): 1. Mild—apathy is notable but produces little interference with daily routines; only mildly different from (S)’s usual behavior; (S) responds to suggestions to engage in activities. 2. Moderate—apathy is very evident; may be overcome with coaxing and encouragement; responds spontaneously only to powerful events such as visits from close relatives or family members. 3. Marked—apathy is very evident and usually fails to respond to any encouragement or external events. 9c. Caregiver Distress (How emotionally distressing do you find this behavior? Check one): 0. Not at all 1. Minimally 2. Mildly 3. Moderately 4. Severely 5. Very severely or extremely H. Disinhibition Read to the caregiver: Does (S) seem to act impulsively without thinking? Does he/she do or say things that are not usually done or said in public? Does he/she say things that are embarrassing to you or others? Yes (If yes, proceed to subquestions 1–8). No (If no, proceed to next screening question, “I. Irritability/Lability”). N. A. (Not applicable)
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
520 _____ _____ _____ _____
1. 2. 3. 4.
_____ 5. _____ 6. _____ 7. 8.
Does (S) act impulsively without appearing to consider the consequences? Does (S) talk to total strangers as if he/she knew them? Does (S) say things to people that are insensitive or hurt their feelings? Does (S) say crude things or make sexual remarks that they would not usually have said? Does (S) talk openly about very personal or private matters not usually discussed in public? Does (S) take liberties or touch or hug others in a way that is out of character for him/her? Does (S) show any other signs of loss of control of his/her impulses? Have the caregiver rate the global frequency, severity, and their own distress for the preceding items.
8a. Frequency Ratings (check one): 1. Occasionally—less than once per week. 2. Often—about once per week. 3. Frequently—several times per week, but less than every day. 4. Very frequently—essentially continuously present. 8b. Severity Ratings (check one): 1. Mild—disinhibition is notable, but usually responds to redirection and guidance. 2. Moderate—disinhibition is very evident and difficult to overcome by the caregiver. 3. Marked—disinhibition usually fails to respond to any interventions by the caregiver and is a source of embarrassment or social distress. 8c Caregiver Distress (How emotionally distressing do you find this behavior? Check one): 0. Not at all 1. Minimally 2. Mildly 3. Moderately 4. Severely 5. Very severely or extremely I. Irritability/Lability Read to the caregiver: Does (S) get irritated and easily disturbed? Are his/her moods very changeable? Is he/she abnormally impatient? We do not mean frustration over memory loss or inability to perform usual tasks; we are interested to know if (S) has abnormal irritability, impatience, or rapid emotional changes different from his/her usual self. Yes No N.A. _____ 1. _____ 2. _____ _____ _____ _____ _____
3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8.
(If yes, proceed to subquestions 1–8). (If no, proceed to next screening question, “J. Aberrant Motor Behavior”). (Not applicable)
Does (S) have a bad temper, “flying off the handle” easily over little things? Does (S) rapidly change moods from one to another, being fine one minute and angry the next? Does (S) have sudden flashes of anger? Is (S) impatient, having trouble coping with delays or waiting for planned activities? Is (S) cranky and irritable? Is (S) argumentative and difficult to get along with? Does (S) show any other signs of irritability? Have the caregiver rate the global frequency, severity, and their own distress for the preceding items.
Neuropsychiatric Inventory
521
8a. Frequency (check one): 1. Occasionally—less than once per week. 2. Often—about once per week. 3. Frequently—several times per week, but less than every day. 4. Very frequently—essentially continuously present. 8b. Severity (check one): 1. Mild—irritability or lability is notable but usually responds to redirection and reassurance. 2. Moderate—irritability and lability are very evident and difficult to overcome by the caregiver. 3. Marked—irritability and lability are very evident, they usually fail to respond to any intervention by the caregiver, and they are a major source of distress. 8c. Caregiver Distress (How emotionally distressing do you find this behavior? Check one): 0. Not at all 1. Minimally 2. Mildly 3. Moderately 4. Severely 5. Very severely or extremely J. Aberrant Motor Behavior Read to the caregiver: Does (S) pace, do things over and over such as opening closets or drawers, or repeatedly pick at things, or wind string or threads? Yes No N.A. _____ _____ _____ _____ _____
1. 2. 3. 4. 5.
_____ 6. _____ 7. 8.
(If yes, proceed to subquestions 1–8). (If no, proceed to next screening question, “K. Sleep”) (Not applicable)
Does (S) pace around the house without purpose? Does (S) rummage around opening and unpacking drawers or closets? Does (S) repeatedly put on and take off clothing? Does (S) have repetitive activities or “habits” that he/she performs over and over? Does (S) engage in repetitive activities such as handling buttons, picking, wrapping string, etc.? Does (S) fidget excessively, seem unable to sit still, or bounce his/her feet or tap his/her fingers a lot? Does (S) do any other activities over and over? Have the caregiver rate the global frequency, severity, and their own distress for the preceding items.
8a. Frequency (check one): 1. Occasionally—less than once per week. 2. Often—about once per week. 3. Frequently—several times per week, but less than every day. 4. Very frequently—essentially continuously present. 8b. Severity (check one): 1. Mild—abnormal motor activity is notable but produces little interference with daily routines. 2. Moderate—abnormal motor activity is very evident; can be overcome by the caregiver. 3. Marked—abnormal motor activity is very evident, it usually fails to respond to any intervention by the caregiver, and is a major source of distress.
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
522
8c. Caregiver Distress (How emotionally distressing do you find this behavior? Check one): 0. Not at all 1. Minimally 2. Mildly 3. Moderately 4. Severely 5. Very severely or extremely K. Sleep Read to the caregiver: Does (S) have difficulty sleeping (do not count as present if (S) simply gets up once or twice per night to go to the bathroom and falls back asleep immediately)? Is he/she up at night? Does he/she wander at night, get dressed, or disturb your sleep? Yes No N.A. _____ 1. _____ 2. _____ 3. _____ 4. _____ 5. _____ 6. _____ 7. _____ 8. 9.
(If yes, proceed to subquestions 1–9). (If no, proceed to next screening question, “L. Appetite and Eating Disorders”). (Not applicable)
Does (S) have difficulty falling asleep? Does (S) get up during the night (do not count if the patient gets up once or twice per night only to go to the bathroom and falls back asleep immediately.) Does (S) wander, pace, or get involved in inappropriate activities at night? Does (S) awaken you during the night? Does (S) wake up at night, dress, and plan to go out, thinking that it is morning and time to start the day? Does (S) wake up too early in the morning (earlier than was his/her habit)? Does (S) sleep excessively during the day? Does (S) have any other night-time behaviors that bother you that we haven’t talked about? Have the caregiver rate the global frequency, severity, and their own distress for the preceding items.
9a. Frequency (check one): 1. Occasionally—less than once per week. 2. Often—about once per week. 3. Frequently—several times per week, but less than every day. 4. Very frequently—essentially continuously present. 9b. Severity (check one): 1. Mild—night-time behaviors occur but they are not particularly disruptive. 2. Moderate—night-time behaviors occur and disturb (S) and the sleep of the caregiver; more than one type of night-time behavior may be present. 3. Marked—night-time behaviors occur; several types of behavior may be present; (S) is very distressed during the night and the caregiver’s sleep is markedly disturbed. 9c. Caregiver Distress (How emotionally distressing do you find this behavior? Check one): 0. Not at all 1. Minimally 2. Mildly 3. Moderately 4. Severely 5. Very severely or extremely
Neuropsychiatric Inventory
523
L. Appetite and Eating Disorders Read to the caregiver: Has he/she had any changes in appetite, weight, or eating habits? (Count as NA if (S) is incapacitated and has to be fed.) Has there been any change in type of food he/she prefers? Yes No N.A. _____ _____ _____ _____ _____
1. 2. 3. 4. 5.
_____ 6. _____ 7. _____ 8. 9.
(If yes, proceed to subquestions 1–9). (Not applicable)
Does (S) have a poor appetite (loss of appetite). Does (S) have an abnormally good appetite (increase in appetite)? Has (S) lost weight? Has (S) gained weight? Does (S) have unusual eating behavior, such as putting too much food in his/her mouth at once? Has (S) had a change in the kind of food he/she likes, such as wanting too many sweets or other specific types of food? Has (S) developed eating behaviors, such as eating exactly the same types of food each day, or eating the food in exactly the same order? Have there been any other changes in appetite or eating that I haven’t asked about? Have the caregiver rate the global frequency, severity, and their own distress for the preceding items.
9a. Frequency (check one): 1. Occasionally—less than once per week. 2. Often—about once per week. 3. Frequently—several times per week, but less than every day. 4. Very frequently—essentially continuously present. 9b. Severity (check one): 1. Mild—irritability or lability is notable but usually responds to redirection and reassurance. 2. Moderate—irritability and lability are very evident and difficult to overcome by the caregiver. 3. Marked—irritability and lability are very evident, they usually fail to respond to any intervention by the caregiver, and they are a major source of distress. 9c. Caregiver Distress (How emotionally distressing do you find this behavior? Check one): 0. Not at all 1. Minimally 2. Mildly 3. Moderately 4. Severely 5. Very severely or extremely Source. Copyright J.L. Cummings, 1994. Used with permission. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory is copyrighted and its use must be negotiated with the author, Jeffrey L. Cummings, UCLA School of Medicine, 710 Westwood Plaza, Los Angeles, CA 90097-1769.
This page intentionally left blank
APPENDIX
H
Agitated Behavior in Dementia Scale
Instructions for Administration and Scoring of Agitated Behavior in Dementia Scale (ABID) The ABID is administered to the caregiver about the Alzheimer’s disease patient. Instructions should be read to the caregiver, and the interviewer should ensure that the caregiver understands the instructions. Each item is rated according to its occurrence during the past week, and according to its occurrence during the week before last, using the following format: Was (subject) verbally threatening or aggressive towards others during the past week? If yes, mark the frequency, according to instructions. How about the week before last? If yes, mark the frequency. If the behavior occurred during either of the past 2 weeks, ask the caregiver to rate how much the behavior bothered him/her, according to instructions. If the behavior did not occur during either week, mark the reaction as “9” (not applicable). Continue for each item. Scoring Algorithm for Outpatient Agitation Inventory: Reaction total: sum (r0l to r16) range=0 to 64 items scored as “9” (not applicable) receive a reaction score of “0” Frequency total: first, compute composite score for each item (f01 to f16) by adding (week 1+week 2) for each item range for item scores=0 to 6 then, sum composite scores (f01 to f16) range for total score=0 to 96 Interview Date:
Month___________ Day ___________ Year__________________
Instructions: The following is a list of problems people/patients sometimes have. Please indicate if any of these problems have occurred during the last two weeks. If so, how much has this bothered or upset you when it happened? Use the following scales for the frequency of the problem and your reaction to it. Please read the description of the ratings carefully.
525
526
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Frequency Ratings: 0=did not occur in the week 1=occurred 1 to 2 times in the week 2=occurred 3 to 6 times in the week 3=occurred daily or more often 9=don’t know/not applicable
Reaction Ratings: 0=not at all 1=a little 2=moderately 3=very much 4=extremely 9=don’t know/not applicable
Please answer all the questions below. Check one box from 0–9 for both Frequency and Reaction. WEEK 1 WEEK 2
WEEK 1
Refers to the past week Refers to the week before last Frequency
Reaction
0 1 2 3 9
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 9
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 9
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 9
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 9
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 9
WEEK 2
2. Physically threatening or aggressive toward others.
3. Harmful to self (e.g., biting, pinching self).
4. Inappropriate screaming or crying out.
5. Destroying property.
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
0 1 2 3 9
1. Verbally threatening or aggressive toward others.
0 1 2 3 4 9
6. Refusing to accept appropriate help (e.g., with personal care).
7. Trying to leave (or leaving) home inappropriately.
Agitated Behavior in Dementia Scale
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 9
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 9
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 9
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 9
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 9
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 9
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
WEEK 1
0 1 2 3 9
WEEK 2
9. Socially inappropriate behavior (e.g., loud offensive remarks).
10. Inappropriate sexual behavior (e.g., unwanted sexual advances, public sexual behavior).
11. Restlessness, fidgetiness, inability to sit still.
12. Worrying, anxiety, and/or fearfulness.
13. Easily agitated or upset.
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
0 1 2 3 9
8. Arguing, irritability, or complaining.
0 1 2 3 4 9
WEEK 2
0 1 2 3 9
527
0 1 2 3 4 9
14. Waking and getting up at night (other than trips to the bathroom).
15. Incorrect, distressing beliefs or delusions (e.g., about being threatened or harmed).
16. Seeing, hearing, or sensing distressing people or things that are not really present (e.g., a strange man in the house, insects crawling on the walls—hallucinations).
Source. Reprinted from Logsdon RG, Teri L, Weiner MF, et al.: “Assessment of Agitation in Alzheimer’s Disease: The Agitated Behavior in Dementia Scale: Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 47:1354–1358, 1999. Used with permission.
This page intentionally left blank
APPENDIX
I
Quality of Life in Alzheimer’s Disease Scale
Instructions for Interviewers The QOL-AD is administered in interview format to caregivers of individuals with dementia, following the instructions below. Hand the form to the caregiver, so that he or she may look at it as you give the following instructions (instructions should closely follow the wording given in bold type): I want to ask you some questions about [S’s] quality of life and have you rate different aspects of [S’s] life using one of four words: poor, fair, good, or excellent Point to each word (poor, fair, good, and excellent) on the form as you say it. When you think about [S’s] life, there are different aspects, like physical health, energy, family, money, and others. I’m going to ask you to rate each of these areas. We want to find out what you think [S]feels about his/her current situation in each area. If you’re not sure about what a question means, you can ask me about it. If you have difficulty rating any item, just give it your best guess. It is usually apparent whether an individual understands the questions, and most individuals who are able to communicate and respond to simple questions can understand the measure. If the caregiver answers all questions the same, or says something that indicates a lack of understanding, the interviewer is encouraged to clarify the question. However, under no circumstances should the interviewer suggest a specific response. Each of the four possible responses should be presented, and the caregiver should pick one of the four. 529
530
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
If a caregiver is unable to choose a response to a particular item or items, this should be noted in the comments. If the caregiver is unable to comprehend and/or respond to two or more items, the testing may be discontinued and this should be noted in the comments. As you read the items listed below, ask the caregiver to circle her/his response. If the caregiver has difficulty circling the word, you may ask her/ him to point to the word or say the word, and you may circle it for him or her. You should let the participant hold his or her own copy of the measure and follow along as you read each item. 1. First of all, how do you think [S] feels about [S’s] physical health? Would you say it’s poor, fair, good, or excellent? Circle whichever word you think best describes [S’s] estimate of his/her physical health right now. 2. How do you think [S] feels about [S’s] energy level? Do you think it is poor, fair, good, or excellent? If the participant says that some days are better than others, ask him or her to rate how she/he thinks [S] has been feeling most of the time lately. 3. How would [S] describe [S’s] mood lately? Would [S] say [S’s] spirits have been good, or would [S] report feeling down? Would [S] rate [S’s] mood as poor, fair, good, or excellent? 4. How do you think [S] feels about [S’s] living situation; the place where [S] lives now? Would you say it’s poor, fair, good, or excellent? 5. How about [S’s] feelings about [S’s] memory? Would [S] describe it as poor, fair, good, or excellent? 6. How about [S’s] family and [S’s] relationship with family members? Would you describe it as poor, fair, good, or excellent? If the respondent says [S] has no family, ask about brothers, sisters, children, nieces, nephews. 7. How do you think [S] would rate [S’s] (your ) marriage? Do you feel [S] would describe the relationship as poor, fair, good, or excellent? Some patients will be accompanied by a non-spousal caregiver. In that case, ask [S’s] rating of the relationship with him/her. When this is the case, ask the caregiver how they seem to feel about him/her. 8. How would you think [S] would describe [S’s] current relationship with friends? Would you say it’s poor, fair, good, or excel-
Quality of Life in Alzheimer’s Disease Scale
531
lent? If the respondent answers that [S] has no friends, or all their friends have died, probe further. Is there anyone [S] appears to enjoy being with besides family? Would you call that person a friend? If the respondent still says [S] has no friends, ask how does [S] feel about having no friends—poor, fair, good, or excellent? 9. How do you think [S] feels about him/herself—when [S] thinks of [S’s] whole self, and all the different things about him/her, would you say it’s poor, fair, good, or excellent? 10. How do you think [S] feels about [S’s] ability to do things like chores around the house or other things [S] needs to do? Would you say it’s poor, fair, good, or excellent? 11. How about [S’s] feelings about [S’s] ability to do things for fun, that [S] enjoys? Would you say it’s poor, fair, good, or excellent? 12. How do you think [S] feels about [S’s] current situation with money; [S’s] financial situation? Would [S] rate it as poor, fair, good, or excellent? If the respondent hesitates, explain that you don’t want to know what [S’s] situation is (as in amount of money), just how they feel about it. 13. How do you think [S] would rate [S’s] life as a whole; everything together. Would you say it’s poor, fair, good, or excellent? SCORING INSTRUCTIONS FOR THE QOL: Points are assigned to each item as follows: poor = 1, fair = 2, good = 3, excellent=4. The total score is the sum of all 13 items.
532
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Patient ID# Patient Initials Visit Type Date
_________________ _________________ _________________ _________________
Quality of Life (Informant Version) Please rate [S’s] current situation, as you see it. Circle your responses. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13.
Physical health. Energy. Mood. Living situation. Memory. Family. Marriage. Friends. Self as a whole. Ability to do chores around the house. Ability to do things for fun. Money. Life as a whole.
Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor
Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair
Good Good Good Good Good Good Good Good Good Good Good Good Good
Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent
Comments: _________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ Thank You!
Quality of Life in Alzheimer’s Disease Scale
Patient ID# Patient Initials Visit Type Date
533
_________________ _________________ _________________ _________________
Quality of Life (Patient Version) Please rate current situation, as you see it. Circle your responses. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13.
Physical health. Energy. Mood. Living situation. Memory. Family. Marriage. Friends. Self as a whole. Ability to do chores around the house. Ability to do things for fun. Money. Life as a whole.
Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor Poor
Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair Fair
Good Good Good Good Good Good Good Good Good Good Good Good Good
Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent Excellent
Comments: ________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________ Thank You! Source. Reprinted from Logsdon RG, Gibbons LE, McCurry SM, et al.: “Quality of Life in Alzheimer’s Disease: Patient and Caregiver Reports.” Journal of Mental Health and Aging 5:21–32, 1999. Copyright 1999, Springer Publishing Company, Inc., New York 10012. Used with permission.
This page intentionally left blank
APPENDIX
J
Quality of Life in LateStage Dementia (QUALID) Scale
The Quality of Life in Late-Stage Dementia (QUALID) is administered in interview format to an informant following the instructions below. Informants may be either a family member or professional caregiver who by having regular contact is familiar with the subject’s general behavior. Informants must, in addition to being familiar with the subject, have spent a significant portion of at least 3 days out of the last 7 days with the subject, in order to accurately rate the items on the scale. The scale is scored by summing the responses. The possible scores range from 11 to 55, with 11 representing the highest quality of life. The final items on the scale require that the interviewer make a judgment about the validity of the interview. Provide both a rating of the overall quality of the interview, which includes the informant’s ability to understand the items and responses and the effort the informant put forth in answering questions, and the familiarity of the informant with the subject. These items are not included in the score, but offer information about the validity and usefulness of the ratings for that subject. Informants are handed a blank copy of the scale so that they may look at the items as they are read aloud, and the following instructions are given: I want to ask you some questions about quality of life. I want you to rate his/ her behaviors using the responses under each question on this page. (Point to the responses on the first question.) There is no one right or wrong answer. I just want to know how you would rate his/her behavior from your observations. Specifically, I want to know about his/her behavior over the past week only, not how he/she previously behaved. Remember that your answers should reflect his/her behavior over the past seven days. If you are not sure what the question means, you can ask me about it. If you have difficulty choosing a rating for an item, just make your best guess. Again, indicate your observation about his/her behavior over the past week.
Which response best describes ______________ over the past week? A. [S] smiles 1. spontaneously once or more each day 2. spontaneously less than once each day 3. only in response to external stimuli; at least once each day 4. only in response to external stimuli; less than once each day 5. rarely, if at all
535
536
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
B. [S] appears sad 1. rarely or never 2. only in response to external stimuli; less than once each day 3. only in response to external stimuli; at least once each day 4. for no apparent reason less than once each day 5. for no apparent reason once or more each day C. [S] cries 1. rarely or never 2. only in response to external stimuli; less than once each day 3. only in response to external stimuli; at least once each day 4. for no apparent reason less than once each day 5. for no apparent reason once or more each day D. [S] has a facial expression of discomfort—appears unhappy or in pain (looks worried, grimaces, furrowed or turned-down brow) 1. rarely or never 2. less than once each day 3. at least once each day 4. nearly half of each day 5. most of each day E. [S] appears physically uncomfortable—he/she squirms, writhes, frequently changes position 1. rarely or never 2. less than once each day 3. at least once each day 4. nearly half of each day 5. most of each day F.
[S] makes statements or sounds that suggest discontent, unhappiness, or discomfort (complains, groans, screams) 1. rarely or never 2. only in response to external stimuli; less than once each day 3. only in response to external stimuli; at least once each day 4. without cause less than once each day 5. without cause once or more each day
G. [S] is irritable or aggressive (becomes angry, curses, pushes, or attempts to hurt others) 1. rarely or never 2. only in response to external stimuli; less than once each day 3. only in response to external stimuli; at least once each day 4. without cause less than once each day 5. without cause once or more each day H. [S] enjoys eating 1. at most meals and snacks 2. twice a day 3. at least once a day 4. less than once each day 5. rarely or never
Quality of Life in Late-Stage Dementia Scale
537
I.
[S] enjoys touching/being touched 1. almost always; almost always initiates touching 2. more than half the time; sometimes initiates touching 3. half the time; never initiates touching, but doesn’t resist touching 4. less than half the time; often or frequently resists touching/being touched 5. rarely or never; almost always resists touching/being touched
J.
[S] enjoys interacting or being with others 1. almost always; almost always initiates interaction with others 2. more than half the time; sometimes initiates interaction with others 3. half the time; never initiates interaction, but doesn’t resist interaction with others 4. less than half the time; often or frequently resists interacting with others 5. rarely or never; almost always resists interacting with others
K. [S] appears emotionally calm and comfortable 1. most of each day 2. more than half of each day 3. half of each day 4. less than half of each day 5. rarely or never __________Total Score (sum of all items; scores range from 11 to 55 with lower scores representing higher quality of life) Quality of Interview (Administrator’s judgment): 0 Interview appeared valid 1 Some questions about interview, but probably acceptable 2 Information from interview of doubtful validity Knowledge/familiarity of caregiver with subject: 0 Very familiar; provides daily care 1 Somewhat familiar; often provides some care 2 Not very familiar; only dispenses meds, minimal contact Source.
Copyright 2000 Myron F. Weiner, M.D.
This page intentionally left blank
APPENDIX
K
Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale (AIMS)
Before conducting the formal examination below, observe patients unobtrusively at rest in a hard, firm, armless chair. Then ask the following questions or make the following requests: 1. “Is there anything in your mouth such as gum or candy? If so, please remove it.” 2. “How are your teeth?” “Do you wear dentures?” “Do your teeth or dentures bother you?” 3. “Do you notice any movements in your mouth, face, hands, or feet? If so, describe them.” “How much do they bother you at present, and how much do they interfere with your activities?” 4. “Please sit with your hands on your knees, legs slightly apart and feet flat on the floor.” (Observe for large body movements) 5. “Please let your hands to hang down between your legs” or (if wearing a dress) “Let your hands hang over your knees.” (Observe hands and other body areas) 6. “Please open your mouth.” (Do twice, observing tongue at rest) 7. “Please stick out your tongue.” (Do twice, noting abnormalities of tongue movement) 8. “Tap your thumb with each finger of the same hand as fast as you can.” (Do 15 seconds for each finger of both hands, one hand at a time. Observe face and leg movements) 9. “Please stand up.” (Observe all body areas, including hips, in profile) 10. “Please stretch your arms out in front of you with your palms down.” (Observe trunk, legs, and mouth) 11. “Please walk a few steps, turn, and walk back to the chair.” (Do twice, observing hands and gait)
539
540
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Circle the highest severity noted. 0 = none; 1 = minimal; 2 = mild; 3 = moderate; 4 = severe. 1. Muscles of facial expression (frowning, blinking, grimacing)
0 1 2 3 4
2. Lips and perioral area (puckering, pouting, smacking)
0 1 2 3 4
3. Jaw (chewing, clenching, mouth opening, lateral motion)
0 1 2 3 4
4. Tongue
0 1 2 3 4
5. Upper extremities (choreic [rapid, purposeless, spontaneous, irregular] and athetoid [slow, irregular, complex, serpentine] movements only)
0 1 2 3 4
6. Lower extremities
0 1 2 3 4
7. Trunk (squirming, rocking, twisting)
0 1 2 3 4
8. Global severity of abnormal movements
0 1 2 3 4
9. Incapacity from abnormal movements
0 1 2 3 4
10. Patient’s awareness of abnormal movements
0 1 2 3 4
0 = NONE; 1 = AWARE, NO DISTRESS; 2 = AWARE, MILD DISTRESS; 3 = AWARE, MODERATE DISTRESS; 4 = AWARE, SEVERE DISTRESS 11. Current problem with teeth or dentures? Source. Adapted from Guy 1976.
No 0
Yes 1
APPENDIX
L
Resources
National Agencies Administration on Aging, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services 330 Independence Avenue, SW Washington, DC 20201 1-800-677-1116 Web site: http://www.aoa.dhhs.gov The Administration on Aging (AOA) supports a nationwide network providing services to the elderly, especially to enable them to remain independent. AOA supports some 240 million meals for the elderly each year, including home-delivered “meals on wheels,” helps provide transportation and at-home services, supports ombudsman services for the elderly, and provides policy leadership on aging issues.
Alzheimer’s Association (National Office) 919 North Michigan Avenue, Suite 1100 Chicago IL 60611-1676 1-800-272-3900 or 1-313-335-8700; fax: 1-312-335-1110 Web site: http://www.alz.org E-mail:
[email protected] A privately funded national voluntary organization with chapters nationwide. The national office can be contacted for information on many issues regarding Alzheimer’s disease, as well as referral to the nearest local chapter.
541
542
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Alzheimer’s Disease Education and Referral (ADEAR) Center P.O. Box 8250 Silver Spring, MD 20907-8250 1-800-438-4380 Web site: http://www.alzheimers.org This agency is contracted through the National Institute on Aging. ADEAR maintains an on-line database and functions as a clearinghouse for publications and information on Alzheimer’s disease. It has publications from the federally funded Alzheimer’s Disease Centers and other sources.
American Association of Retired Persons (AARP) Programs Resources Dept./BS 601 E Street, NW Washington, DC 20049 1-800-424-3410 Web site: http://www.aarp.org AARP distributes resource kits consisting of several publications about aging and caregiving.
Brain Injury Association of America 105 North Alfred Street Alexandria, VA 22314 703-236-6000; fax: 703-236-6001 Web site: http://www.biausa.org/ An advocacy organization whose mission is to improve the quality of life for persons with head injures and their families, and to develop programs to prevent head injuries. To fulfill its mission, the BIAA focuses its efforts on education, support and information, public awareness, prevention, research, and training.
National Association of Professional Geriatric Care Managers 1604 N. County Club Road Tucson, AZ 85716-3102 1-520-881-8008 Web site: http://www.caremanager.org Private case managers are certified to provide an array of assessment and social services ranging from obtaining in-home health help to linking with financial, legal, and other long-term care services.
Resources
543
The National Council on the Aging, Inc. 409 3rd Street SW, Suite 200 Washington, DC 20024 1-202-479-1200 Web site: http://www.ncoa.org A private, nonprofit organization that serves as a resource for information, training, technical assistance, advocacy, and leadership in all aspects of aging.
National Health Information Center P.O. Box 133 Washington, DC 20013-1133 1-800-336-4797 Web site: http://www.health.gov/nhic Healthfinder series provides a starting point for consumers and health professionals looking for information on home health care. Currently available books and additional organizations that can provide further information are cited. Financial issues and how to find home health care providers are discussed.
National Stroke Association (NSA) 9707 E. Easter Lane Englewood, CO 80112 1-800-787-6537 or 1-303-649-9299; fax: 1-303-649-1328 Web site: http://www.stroke.org A nonprofit organization whose mission is to reduce the incidence and impact of stroke on individuals and on society. It supports stroke research in all areas, develops and distributes educational materials, and is an information and referral clearinghouse.
Huntington’s Disease Society of America (HDSA) 158 W. 29th Street, 7th Floor New York, NY 10001-5300 1-800-345-4372; fax: 1-212-239-3430 Web site: http://www.hdsa.org A national voluntary organization providing support and services to patients and families. HDSA also supports research and education and disseminates information. There is also a network of local chapters across the United States.
544
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
National Organization for Rare Disorders (NORD) 55 Kenosia Avenue P.O. Box 1968 Danbury, CT 06813-1968 1-800-999-6673 or 1-203-744-0100 Web site: http://www.rarediseases.org A federation of voluntary health organizations dedicated to helping people with rare “orphan” diseases. NORD is a clearinghouse for information and is committed to the identification, treatment, and cure of rare disorders through programs of education, advocacy, research, and service.
National Parkinson Foundation (NPF) Bob Hope Parkinson Research Center 1501 NW 9th Avenue Miami, FL 33136-1494 1-800-327-4545 or 1-305-547-6666 Web site: http://www.parkinson.org Provides educational services and information in the form of support groups, publications, and workshops. NPF also supports Parkinson’s disease research.
The American Parkinson Disease Association, Inc. (APDA) 1250 Hylan Blvd., Suite 4B Staten Island, NY 10305-1946 1-800-223-2732 or 1-718-981-8001 Web site: http://www.apdaparkinson.com/ APDA supports research and produces educational materials including a newsletter and various pamphlets covering issues such as nutrition in Parkinson’s disease and mobility aids.
State Agencies State Department on Aging (See state governmental listing in local telephone directories) These agencies in each state funnel federal dollars to local area agencies on aging for disbursement to community agency services and programs for elders. Some programs may be dementia specific. This agency is a good entry point for information and referral for aging services.
Resources
545
State Department of Human Services Provide linkage to state-administered and subsidized programs, including community care and adult day care, elder abuse and neglect programs, and nursing home and other residential care programs under Title XX. Web site: http://www.dhs.state.[2 letter state code].us
State Alzheimer’s Programs Several states have Alzheimer’s programs within their state Department of Human Services that provide a variety of services, including information and referral. These states include California, Connecticut, Delaware, Florida, Kansas, Massachusetts, Missouri, New Hampshire, New Jersey, New York, North Carolina, Pennsylvania, Texas, and Wisconsin.
Nursing Home Ombudsman Programs Information available regarding quality of care in nursing homes, lists of nursing homes, checklists, etc. Contact the nearest Area Agency on Aging for information on the ombudsman program in your area.
Other Sources of Information Alzheimer’s Forum Web site: http://www.alzforum.org A compendium of information for researchers, physicians, and the general public, the site includes news, articles, discussion forums, interviews, diagnostic and treatment guide, directory of drugs and clinical trials, and research advances. It also provides access to such unique tools as directory of genetic mutations, antibodies, patents, and conferences.
Caregiver’s Handbook Web site: http://www.biostat.wustl.edu/ALZHEIMER/care.html Although this handbook is not specific to Alzheimer’s disease, it is easily applicable to Alzheimer’s disease, provides good coverage on care for the caregiver, and is copyright free, making it an excellent training tool.
546
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Planning for Long-Term Care Web site: http://www.alzheimers.org/pubs/longterm.html This Web site from the National Institute on Aging explores the options for long-term care, with articles on planning ahead, making the right choice, and making a smooth transition.
Clinical Trials.gov Web site: http://www.clinicaltrials.gov Persons with Alzheimer’s disease, family members, and members of the public can find current trials and research. The searchable database provides information on the name of the study, the purpose, eligibility, and contact information. In addition, the site indicates whether the study is recruiting and includes citations from published works.
Index Page numbers printed in boldface type refer to tables or figures.
Acting out, as defense mechanism, 57, 190 Activities of daily living (ADL) Alzheimer’s disease and, 140 caregivers and, 329 nursing homes and, 382–389 Activity therapy, and management of cognitive impairment, 212 Acute intermittent porphyria, 158–159 Adenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination (ACE), 145–146 Adenosine triphosphate (ATP), and neuroimaging, 113 Administration on Aging (AOA), 541 Adrenoleukodystrophy, 167 Adult polyglucosan body disease, 167 Adult Protective Services, 337 Advance directives, and end-of-life issues, 366 Age and aging cognitive changes and normal process of, 1, 10–11, 286–287 concept of aging in place, 410–411 elderly as growing segment of society, 285 onset of Alzheimer’s disease and, 438 psychotropic drugs and considerations related to, 221 Age-associated memory impairment (AAMI), 11–13 Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, 381, 391 Agent-based ethics, 346 Aggression. See also Behavior; Violent behavior Alzheimer’s disease and, 141 lithium and, 249
ABC approach, to management of cognitive impairment , 200–201, 203, 211 Abdominal examination, 86 Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale (AIMS), 244, 539–540 Abstract attitude, and Alzheimer’s disease, 12 Abstract thought, and mental status examination, 35–36 Abulia, 70–71 Acceptance, and management of cognitive impairment, 192 Accessibility, and living environment, 414 Access to care, and community resources, 376 Accusations, and cognitive impairment, 189–190, 206. See also Suspiciousness Acetazolamide, 267 Acetylcholine, and Alzheimer’s disease, 260 Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, and Alzheimer’s disease, 454–455 Acetyl-L-carnitine, 260 Acquired hepatocerebral degeneration, 154 Acquired imbecility, 13 Acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS). See also Human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) cognitive dysfunction and, 2 family of patients with, 324, 333 psychotropic drugs for delirium and, 235 reversible dementia and, 159
547
548
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Aggression (continued) management of, 188, 190 nursing homes and, 396–397 Agitated Behavior in Dementia (ABID) Scale, 42, 525–527 Agitation. See also Behavior management of, 204 nursing homes and, 396–397 psychological and behavioral concomitants of dementing illness and, 65–66 psychotropic drugs and, 238–252 Agnosias, 141 Alcoholism and alcohol abuse cognitive dysfunction and, 2 patient history of, 308–309 reversible dementia and, 154–155, 160 Alcohol-related dementia, 5–6, 308–309 Alertness. See Hyperalertness Alprazolam, 237, 239–240 Altruism, and coping, 54, 189 Aluminum, and Alzheimer’s disease, 463 Alzheimer, Alois, 138, 145, 181 Alzheimer’s Association, 335, 541 Alzheimer’s disease (AD). See also Caregivers; Community resources; Dementia of the Alzheimer’s type; Management; Nursing homes; Safety; Treatment advances in research on, 433–447 advances in treatment of, 453–471 apathy and, 71 awareness of deficits in, 56 behavioral symptoms of, 141, 181 case examples of, 56, 57, 58–59, 220 as cause of death, 2, 141 Charles Bonnet syndrome and, 70 cognitive patterns of, 304–305 depression and, 27–28, 29, 59, 60, 141, 211, 311 Down syndrome and, 27
differential diagnosis of, 137–142, 306, 311 driving and, 208 electroencephalography and, 94 genetic testing and, 349 Lewy body variant of, 143 medications for, 259–267, 362 mild cognitive impairment and development of, 12 neuroimaging and, 103, 110–111, 114, 115, 122, 124 Parkinson’s disease and, 148–149 prevalence of, 1 progression of, 49 sleep and, 66 sundowning and, 20, 140 support groups for, 210–211 tests for incipient, 12 Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS-Cog), 40, 41, 259, 262, 264, 458, 503–512 Alzheimer’s Disease Education and Referral (ADEAR) Center, 542 Alzheimer’s Disease: A Handbook for Caregivers (Hamdy et al. 1998), 321 Alzheimer’s Early Stages: First Steps in Caring and Treatment (Kuhn and Bennett 1999), 321 Alzheimer’s Forum, 545 Amantadine, 242 American Academy of Neurology, 89, 92, 96, 104, 353 American Association of Retired Persons (AARP), 416, 542 American Geriatrics Society, 353 American Parkinson Disease Association, Inc. (APDA), 544 American Psychiatric Association, 3 Amitriptyline, 220, 229 Amnesia and amnestic disorders Alzheimer’s disease and, 304 clinical diagnosis of, 20 DSM-IV-TR classification of, 5 DSM-IV-TR criteria for, 21
Index Amphetamine-related disorders, 6 Amyloid biomarkers, for Alzheimer’s disease, 442–443 Amyloid processing, in Alzheimer’s disease, 434–437, 467–468 Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS), 147 Anemia, 89, 161 Anger, and stress in caregivers, 334 Angiography, 93 Anomic aphasia, 32 Anoxia, and reversible dementia, 159 Antiandrogens, 257 Anticholinergic agents delirium and, 230 Parkinson’s disease and, 149 Anticholinesterases, and Alzheimer’s disease, 260–264, 265–266 Anticipation, and coping, 54–55 Anticoagulants, and vascular dementia, 151 Anticonvulsants, and agitation, 249–251 Antidepressants. See also Medications; Tricylic antidepressants depression in elderly and, 29, 202 vascular dementia and, 151 Antihistamines, 220, 253 Antioxidants, and Alzheimer’s disease, 267, 462 Antiparkinson drugs, and tardive dyskinesia, 244 Antiplatelet drugs, and cerebrovascular disease, 267–268 Antipsychotics. See also Neuroleptics agitation and, 239, 240–244 depression in elderly and, 223–224 Anxiety. See also Behavior management of cognitive impairment and, 199, 203–204 psychological and behavioral concomitants of dementing illness and, 65 psychotropic drugs and, 236–238
549
Anxiety disorder due to a general medical condition, 22, 24–25 Apathy. See also Behavior; Social withdrawal management of cognitive impairment and, 207–208 psychological or behavioral concomitants of dementing illness, 70–71 psychotropic drugs and, 256 Apathy Evaluation Scale, 71 Aphasias, 31–32, 141 Apolipoprotein E (ApoE), and Alzheimer’s disease, 119–120, 434, 438–439, 441, 443–444, 447, 469 Apraxias, 302,m Aspergillosis, 170 Aspirin, 267–268 Assisted-living settings, 405, 417–421, 426 Atherosclerosis, 269 Attention, and neuropsychological tests, 290, 293–294 A2M polymorphisms, and Alzheimer’s disease, 447 Atypical neuroleptics, 224, 244–247. See also Neuroleptics Australia, and hostels for elderly, 406 Automobiles. See Driving Autonomy. See also Independence environment of older adult and, 408 management of cognitive impairment and, 193–194 personality and, 53 Awareness, and assisted-living settings, 418 Azotemia, 155 >-Adrenergic blockers, and agitation, 251 > -Amyloid. See Amyloid biomarkers; Amyloid processing Babinski reflex, 88 Baclofen, 165
550
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Barbiturates reversible dementia and, 154 sleep and, 253 withdrawal from medication and delirium, 235 Barrier interventions, and wandering, 397 Bathing, and nursing care, 384 Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), 303 Behavior. See also Agression; Agitation; Anxiety; Apathy; Irritability; Violent behavior ABC approach to management of, 200–201 Alzheimer’s disease and, 141, 181 control as ethical issue, 346, 365 executive functions and dyscontrol of, 65 explanation of dementia to family and, 322–324 measures of symptoms of dementing illness, 41–42 medical evaluation and, 83–84 nursing homes and, 395–399 psychotropic drugs and, 220, 258–269 Behavioral Pathology in Alzheimer’s Disease (BEHAVE-AD), 245 Beliefs, cognitive impairment and mistaken, 189–190 Benzodiazepines agitation and, 240 anxiety and, 236, 237 delirium and, 235–236 falls and, 390 reversible dementia and, 154 sleep-wake disturbance and, 253–254 tardive dyskinesia and, 244 Benztropine, 235, 242 Best interests of patient, and legal issues, 338 Binswanger’s disease, 149 Bioethics, 346
Biological integrity, and coping ability, 52 Biological vulnerability, to depression, 61 Bipolar disorder, and reversible dementia, 153 Blessed Dementia Rating Scale (BDRS), 14, 40, 140, 499 Blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) fMRI, 125, 126 Blood tests, and laboratory studies, 89–91. See also Cerebral blood flow Blood viscosity reducers, and cerebrovascular disease, 267–268 Body mass index (BMI), 385 Boston Naming Test, 300 Brain. See also Brain damage; Brain disorders atrophy and Alzheimer’s disease, 142 biopsy of, 93 maps of and neuroimaging, 117–119 volume of and neuroimaging, 111 Brain damage. See also Head injury depression and subclinical, 61 reversible dementia and traumatic, 163–164 Brain disorders, and reversible dementia, 162–164 Brain Injury Association of America, 542 Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale, 247, 249 Bright light therapy, 394, 397, 426. See also Lighting; Phototherapy Broca’s aphasia, 32 Bromocriptine, 165, 242 Bupropion, 228–229, 231, 232 Buschke Selective Reminding Test, 296 Buspirone, 238, 390 Butyrylcholinesterase, 456–457
Index Caffeine, and insomnia, 67 Caffeine-related disorders, 6 Calcium, reversible dementia and blood levels of, 157–158 California, and legal issues, 361, 420 California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), 296, 297, 298 Cambridge Examination for Mental Disorders of the Elderly, 1 Campbell v. Groves (1989), 343 Canada Cree tribe and Alzheimer’s disease, 443 guidelines for medical evaluations and, 96 special care units in, 423 Candidiasis, 170 Cannabis-related disorders, 6 Capacity to Consent to Treatment Instrument (CCTI), 354–359 Capgras’ syndrome, 59, 69 Carbamazepine, 249 Cardiovascular disease, and vascular dementia, 151 Cardiovascular examination, 86 Caregivers. See also Family; Nursing homes behavior control and continuity of, 365 communication and, 186, 382 community resources and, 376 digital technology and, 417 education for, 321–327 ethical issues for, 337–338 impact of dementia on, 330–333 insomnia and, 67 living environment and, 414 management of cognitive impairment and, 187, 189–201, 203–204 neuropsychological evaluation and interviews, 289 nursing homes and, 336–337 psychological point of view, 327–329
551
reports of deterioration in cognitive function, 373 sexuality and, 335–336 stress and, 334–335, 373–374 surrogates and relief for, 201 views of difficult patients, 50–51 Caregiver’s Handbook, 545 Carotid and transcranial sonography, 93–94 Case examples of ABC approach to behavior management, 201 of assisted living settings, 417–418 of caregiving environment, 411 of cognitive impairment, 50–51 of competency as legal issue, 342–343 of defense mechanisms in Alzheimer’s disease, 56, 57, 58–59 of delirium, 64 of depression, 62 of explanation of dementia to family, 322–324 of frontotemporal dementia, 37 of informed consent, 351 of medical evaluation, 80, 97 of nursing home as living environment, 421–422 of personality change due to a general medical condition, 25–26 of proxy decision makers, 364 of psychotic defense mechanisms, 57–58 of psychotic disorder due to a general medical condition, 21, 23 of psychotropic drugs and Alzheimer’s disease, 220 of subcortical dementia, 38–39 of temporoparietal dementia, 38 Catastrophic reactions, and defense mechanisms, 56–57 Catatonic disorder due to a general medical condition, 22–23
552
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Category Test, 295 Central nervous system disorders, and reversible dementia, 152 Cerebral blood flow, and neuroimaging, 113, 125 Cerebrolysin (FPF-1070), 460–461 Cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) Alzheimer’s disease and, 91 computed tomography and, 105 Cerebrovascular disease drugs for prevention and treatment of, 267–269 vascular dementia and, 149–150 Ceroid lipofuscinosis, 167 Certified nursing assistants (CNAs), 384–385, 386, 399 Charles Bonnet syndrome, 70 Chelating agents, 463–464 Chemical poisoning, and reversible dementia, 155 Childhood abuse, and adult depression, 60–61 Children, dementia and adult, 332–333. See also Family Chlordiazepoxide, 237 Chlorpromazine, 225 Chlorpropamide, 156 Choices, reduction of, 197 Cholesterol, and Alzheimer’s disease, 447, 469–470 Cholinergic hypothesis, of Alzheimer’s disease, 453–459. See also Cholinergic system Cholinergic-monoaminergic combinations, and Alzheimer’s disease, 460–464 Cholinergic system, drugs affecting, 259–264. See also Cholinergic hypothesis Cholinesterase inhibitors Alzheimer’s disease and, 142, 453–459 hallucinations and, 255 Parkinson’s disease and, 149 Chromosome 21, and Alzheimer’s disease, 436
Chromosome 17, and familial FTD, 147 Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD), 159, 254 Cisapride, 391 Cisternogram, 92–93 Citalopram, 222–223, 227, 231, 232, 252 Clinical care, and informed consent, 350–351 Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Scale, 40, 208, 260 Clinical Global Impression of Change, 250, 251, 259, 266 Clinical Practice Guideline on Urinary Incontinence in Adults: Acute and Chronic Management (Agency for Healthcare Quality and Research), 391 Clinical Trials.gov, 546 Clock drawing test, 298 Clomipramine, 223 Clonazepam, 239, 254 Clonidine, 460 Clopidogrel, 268 Clozapine agitation and, 244 dosage of, 225 hallucinations and, 255–256 side effects of, 225, 247 Cocaine-related disorders, 7 Coccidioidomycosis, 170 Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 381 Code of Federal Regulations (CFR), 351–352 Cognitive-behavioral therapy, for Alzheimer’s disease, 211 Cognitive disorders. See also Cognitive functioning; Cognitive impairment clinical diagnosis of, 32–36 cognitive disorder not otherwise specified and, 26 DSM-IV-TR classification of, 5
Index Cognitive functioning. See also Cognitive disorders; Cognitive impairment; Executive functions; Neuropsychological evaluation characterization of, 286 families of dementia patients and, 323–324 measures of, 40–41 model of, 50 patterns of various types of dementia, 304–313 Cognitive impairment. See also Cognitive disorders; Cognitive functioning; Executive functions; Memory case example of, 50–51 causes of in youth or early adulthood, 2 depression and, 28 medical evaluation of, 78, 79 medical illness and management of, 212–214 mild forms of, 11–13 normal aging process and, 1, 10–11, 286–287 personal environment and, 408–411 severity of as issue in management, 183–186 sleep-wake disturbances and, 66 Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (CMAI), 66, 241, 250, 396 Color schemes, and dementia care environments, 422–423 Communication. See also Language cognitive impairment and, 183, 194 neurolinguistic analysis of discourse and, 300–301 nursing homes and, 381–382 Community resources access to care and, 376 for caregivers, 334, 335 factors influencing utilization of, 374–376 informed decisions and, 376–378
553
recognition of need for help, 371–374, 372–373 services needed by patients and families, 372 Comorbidity, of depression and Alzheimer’s disease, 27–28, 29, 59, 60, 141, 211, 311 Compensatory mechanisms, and coping, 55 Competency. See also Decision making; Judgment housing and, 407 as legal issue, 341–345, 355, 356–357 Competitive inhibition, and cholinesterase inhibitors, 457 Complete Guide to Alzheimer’s Proofing Your Home, The (Warner 2000), 415 Compliance, and post-hospital medical treatment, 213–214 Comprehension, and mental status examination, 35 Computed tomography (CT), 104–106, 107, 142, 151, 165 Concentration, and neuropsychological tests, 293–294 Concrete operations, and cognitive maturation, 185 Concussive brain injury, 164 Confabulation, and memory impairment, 68–69 Conflict, and living environment, 414 Conflict-based ethics, 346 Congenital imbecility, 13 Congruence model, of personal environment, 407 Consent capacity, assessment of, 355–357 Conservation withdrawal, and depression, 61 Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD), 266, 293
554
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Continuity model, of person and environment, 407–408 Continuum, of levels of care, 410–411 Contract, and capacity as legal issue, 344 Controlled Word Association Test, 307 Control, and living environments, 407, 419 Conversation, and walking programs, 389 Coping humor and, 55, 189 management of dementia and, 188–189 personality function in dementia and, 52, 54–55 Copper chelating agents and Alzheimer’s disease, 464 Wilson’s disease and metabolism of, 90, 158 Corpus callosum, and Alzheimer’s disease, 110–111 Corsellis, J., 138 Cortical dementia, 36, 296, 307–308 Corticobasal degeneration (CBD), 147–148 Corticotropin-releasing hormone, 460 Cortisol concentration, and reversible dementia, 158 Cotard’s syndrome, 69 Cranial nerves, and neurological examination, 86–87 Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease brain biopsy and, 93 eletroencephalography and, 95 laboratory studies and diagnosis of, 91 neurological symptoms of, 165–166 progression of to dementia, 49 Cruzan v. Director, Missouri Dept. of Health (1990), 338 Cryptococcosis, 169–170 Culture, and end-of-life decisions, 366 Cushing’s syndrome, 158
Cyanocobalamin, 161–162 Cyclin-dependent kinase 5 (Cdk5), 442 Cycloergometer, 388 Cycloserine, 461 Dantrolene, 242 Day-care programs, 195 Death. See also Suicide Alzheimer’s disease as cause of, 2, 141 end-of-life decisions and ethical or legal issues, 338, 346–347, 364, 365–367 Decision making. See also Competency; Judgment assessment of capacity for, 354–359, 362 management of cognitive impairment and, 189 proxy decision makers and, 363–364 Defense mechanisms, and personality, 54, 56–59. See also Denial; Projection Delayed recall, 297 Delirium clinical diagnosis of, 16–20 coexistence of with dementia, 19 DSM-IV-TR and, 4, 19 hospitalization for medical illness and, 213 management of, 202–203 psychological and behavioral concomitants of dementing illness and, 63–64 psychotropic drugs and, 234–236 Delirium tremens, 235–236 Delis-Kaplan Executive Function Test, 295 Delusional projection, 57, 328 Delusions Alzheimer’s disease and, 141 as defense mechanisms, 58 management of cognitive impairment and, 205–206 nursing homes and, 398–399
Index psychological and behavioral concomitants of dementing illness and, 68, 69 psychotropic drugs and, 254–255 Dementia and dementing illness. See also Alzheimer’s disease; Behavior; Caregivers; Case examples; Cognitive impairment; Community resources; Management; Medical evaluation; Psychotropic drugs; Nursing homes; Safety; Treatment characterization of, 36–39 clinical diagnosis of, 13–16 cognitive patterns of various types, 304–313 delirium and coexistence of, 19 depression-related cognitive impairment and, 28 differential diagnosis of, 137–171 DSM-IV-TR classification of, 4 DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for, 15–16, 17–18 ethical issues and, 346–367 family and, 322, 330–333 individual differences in, 14, 49 legal issues and, 341–345 neuroimaging and, 103–131 neuropsychological testing and, 290 Parkinson’s disease and, 149 personality function in, 51–59 prevalence of, 1–2 psychological and behavioral management of, 181–214 as psychological process, 53–71 qualifying aspects of, 39–43 Dementia with Lewy bodies (LBV) cognitive patterns of, 306 differential diagnosis of, 142–144, 306 Dementia Questionnaire, 483–485 Dementia Rating Scale, 292, 299 Demonstrations, and management of cognitive impairment, 196
555
Denial Alzheimer’s disease and, 141 caregivers and, 327–329 as defense mechanism, 56 Dependence, and management of cognitive impairment, 193 Deprenyl, 459–460 Depression Alzheimer’s disease and, 27–28, 29, 59, 60, 141, 211, 311 apathy and, 71 behavioral interventions for, 211 in caregivers, 334, 373–374 clinical diagnosis of, 27–31 cognitive patterns of, 310–311 DSM-IV-TR criteria for, 29, 30 family of dementia patient and, 323 insomnia and, 67 management of, 202 medical conditions as cause of, 23 neuropsychological evaluation and, 303, 310–311 psychological and behavioral concomitants of, 59–62 psychotropic drugs and, 222–234 reversible dementia and, 153 stroke and, 28, 59, 163 suicidal ideation and, 62 vascular dementia and, 151 Desipramine, 229, 231, 232 Developmental disorders, and differential diagnosis of dementia, 27 Developmental history, and neuropsychological evaluation, 288 Dexamethasone suppression test, 29, 31 Dextroamphetamine, 234, 256–257 Diagnosis. See also Differential diagnosis; Medical evaluation; Mental status examination; Neuropsychological evaluation advantage of threshold diagnoses, 3 of Alzheimer’s disease, 104, 138, 140
556
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Diagnosis (continued) of amnestic disorder, 20 of anxiety disorder due to a general medical condition, 24–25 of cognitive disorder not otherwise specified, 26 of cognitive dysfunction, 32–36 of delirium, 16–20 of dementia, 13–16, 36–43 of depression, 27–31 DSM-IV-TR and classification of mental disorders, 3–10 of language disorders, 31–32 legal issues and, 344–345 of mental retardation, 26–27 of mood disorder due to a general medical condition, 23 neuroimaging and, 114 of personality change due to a general medical condition, 25–26 persons at risk for dementing illness and, 348–350 of psychotic disorder due to a general medical condition, 20, 23 of substance-induced mood disorder, 24 Diazepam, 237 Dicyano-dimethylamino-naphthalenpropene (DDNP), 122 Diet. See also Eating behavior; Nutritional status; Vitamin deficiencies monoamine oxidase inhibitors and, 233 vitamin C and E supplements and Alzheimer’s disease, 462–463 Differential diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease, 137–142, 306, 311 of corticobasal degeneration, 147–148 of dementia with Lewy bodies, 142–144
of frontotemporal dementia, 144–147 of mixed dementia, 151 of progressive supranuclear palsy, 148 of Parkinson’s disease, 148–149 of reversible dementias, 151–170 of vascular dementia, 149–151 Digital technology, and living environments, 416–417, 426 Dilemma-based ethics, 346 Diphenhydramine, 220, 235, 242 Dipyridamole, 268 Disinhibition, and family of dementia patient, 323 Displacement, and Alzheimer’s disease, 56 Disruptive vocalizations, and nursing homes, 398 Dissociative disorder not otherwise specified, 31 Distortion, as defense mechanism, 57 Distraction, and management of cognitive impairment, 200 Distress, and dementia, 25 Diuretics, and reversible dementia, 156 Divalproex, 240, 249–250 Donepezil, 261–262, 265–266, 454 Dosage, of psychotropic drugs. See also specific drugs of antipsychotics, 241 of beta-adrenergic blockers, 251 of benzodiazepines, 237 neuroleptics and, 225, 235 of tricyclic antidepressants, 230–233 Down syndrome, 27, 127–130, 436 Doxepin, 229 Drawings, and neuropsychological tests, 298–299 Dressing, and management of cognitive impairment, 189, 194, 384–385, 421 Driving, of automobiles caregivers and, 328 ethical and legal issues in, 361–362 management of cognitive impairment and, 208
Index Drug-drug interactions, and monoamine oxidase inhibitors, 233 DSM-IV-TR Alzheimer’s disease and, 138, 140 amnestic disorder and, 20, 21 classification of mental disorders in, 3–10 dementia and, 14, 15–16, 17–18 delirium and, 19 depression and, 29, 30 memory loss and, 287 mental disorders due to a general medical condition and, 22–23 personality disorders and, 52 psychotic disorder and, 20, 22 substance-induced mood disorder and, 24 Durable powers of attorney, 345 Duration, of Alzheimer’s disease, 141 Dysnomia, 300 Dysphoria, 25 Dystonia, and antipsychotic drugs, 243, 247–248 Eating behavior, and management of cognitive impairment, 197, 385–386. See also Diet Education. See also Learning for caregivers and family, 321–327 for nursing home staffs, 399 Electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), 224, 226–227 Electroencephalography (EEG) Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease and, 166 delirium and, 18 medical evaluation and, 94–95 Emotions measures of symptoms of dementing illness, 41–42 neuropsychological evaluation and, 303 Encephalitis, and reversible dementia, 170
557
End of life, and ethical issues, 338, 346–347, 364, 365–367. See also Death Endocrine disorders, and anxiety, 24–25 Environment assisted living settings and, 417–421 digital technology and, 416–417, 426 ethical issues in long term care and, 359–363 incontinence and, 392 management of cognitive impairment and constancy or predictability of, 195 medical evaluation and stress from, 83 nursing homes and, 421–426 private residences and, 411–416 theories of effects on cognitively impaired, 408–411 theories of effects on older adults, 406–408 Epilepsy, and electroencephalography, 95. See also Seizures Episodic memory, 296 Esquirol, J. E. D., 13 Estate plan, and legal issues, 345 Estrogen therapy Alzheimer’s disease and, 465–466 hypersexuality and, 257 Ethical issues assessment of decision-making capacity and, 354–359 at-risk situations and, 347–350 behavior control and, 365 for caregivers, 337–338 end-of-life care and, 365–367 informed consent and, 346, 350–354 long-term-care environment and, 359–363 models of, 346–347 proxy decision makers and, 363–364 Ethics committees, 366
558
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Etiologies, of reversible dementia, 152 European Community, and living environments for cognitively impaired, 415–416 Evaluation. See Medical evaluation; Neuropsychological evaluation Excitatory amino acid (EAA)-mediated toxicity, 461, 462 Executive functions. See also Cognitive functioning; Cognitive impairment Alzheimer’s disease and, 140 behavioral dyscontrol and, 65 neuropsychological tests and, 290, 294–295, 307 stimuli and context of, 183 Exercise, and nursing homes, 387–389, 392, 397 Expressive language, 299–300 Extrapyramidal symptoms, and parkinsonism, 87 Eye diseases, 395 Facial muscles, and neurological examination, 86–87 Failure-Free Activities for the Alzheimer’s Patient (Sheridan 1987), 198 Falls. See also Safety living environment and, 412 in nursing homes, 389–391 Familial fatal insomnia, 166 Familial Mediterranean fever, 467 Familial multiple system tauopathy, 147 Family. See also Caregivers; Children; Family history; Grandparents and grandchildren; Parents end-of-life issues and, 366 explanation of dementia to, 322 genetic testing and, 349–350 impact of dementia on, 330–333 living settings and, 405 neuropsychological evaluation and, 289, 312
Family history. See also Genetics medical evaluation and, 84 neuropsychological evaluation and, 288–289 Fantasy, as defense mechanism, 57 Fatigue, and stress in caregivers, 334 Fear, and hallucinations, 207 Fecal incontinence, 393 Feeding. See Eating behavior; Tube feedings Fever, and reversible dementias, 152 Financial issues, and family, 326–327 Finger tapping tests, 301 Floor effect, of psychotropic drugs, 239 Floor plans, for living environments, 414, 415 Fluctuations, in cognitive dysfunction, 82–83 Fluoxetine, 226, 231, 232 Fluphenazine decanoate, 242 Flurazepam, 154, 253–254 Folic acid, 162 Folsom, C. F., 137 Food and Drug Administration, 258, 455 Formal operations, and cognitive maturation, 185 Free radicals, and Alzheimer’s disease, 462 Free recall, 311. See also Recall Freud, Anna, 52 Frontal Assessment Battery, 145, 146 Frontal Behavioral Inventory, 145 Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) aphasias and, 32 case example of, 37 characterization of, 36 cognitive patterns of, 306–307, 308 differential diagnosis of, 144–147 neuroimaging and, 114, 116–117 Frontotemporal lobar degeneration, 144, 145 Functional Fitness for Long-Term Care (FFLTC), 389
Index Functionality, and environment of older adult, 408 Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), 124–126 Functional neuroimaging, 112–130 Fund of information, and mental status examination, 35 Funduscopic examination, 86 Gabapentin, 250–251 Gadolinium-enhanced MRI, 108–110 Gait, and neurological examination, 88 Galantamine, 261, 263–264, 455, 457–458 Ganser syndrome, 31, 83 Gender of caregiver, 373–374 feeding and nursing home patients, 385 Generalized anxiety disorder, 65 Genetics. See also Family history Alzheimer’s disease and, 138, 433–447 genetic testing for dementing illness, 348–349 Huntington’s disease and, 164–165 medical evaluation and markers, 92 metabolic diseases and, 167 Pick’s disease and, 147 risk to family of dementia patients, 325 Geriatric Depression Scale, 303 Gerstmann-Straussler-Scheinker disease, 166 Ginkgo biloba, 259, 463 Global aphasia, 32 Global function, measures of, 40, 290, 291–293 Global Positioning System (GPS), 417 Glutamatergic drugs, and Alzheimer’s disease, 461–462 Glycine, 461 Gottfries-Brane-Steen dementia rating scale, 223, 227
559
Grandchildren and grandparents, 333. See also Family Guanfacine, 460 Guardianship, as legal issue, 343, 344, 345 Guidelines. See also Standards for human-subjects research and informed consent, 352, 353–354 medical evaluations and, 96 for nursing homes and behavior control, 365 psychotropic drugs and, 219–222, 248 Guiding, and management of cognitive impairment, 196 Guilt, and depression, 62 Hallervorden-Spatz disease, 167 Hallucinations Alzheimer’s disease and, 141 management of cognitive impairment and, 206–207 medical evaluation and, 83–84 nursing homes and, 398–399 psychological and behavioral concomitants of dementing illness and, 69–70 psychotropic drugs and, 255–256 Hallucinogen-related disorders, 7 Haloperidol agitation and, 239–240, 241, 245 delirium and, 236 dosage of, 225, 235 psychotic depression and, 224 side effects of, 225 Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression, 222, 223 Hand dynamometer, 301 Head injury. See also Brain damage apathy and, 71 cognitive dysfunction and, 2 Health care. See Clinical care; End of life; Hospitalization; Medical care; Medications; Terminal care
560
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Health Care Financing Administration, 248, 365 Hearing and auditory acuity management of cognitive impairment and, 191–192 neurological examination and, 86 neuropsychological evaluation and, 302 nursing homes and, 395 Heidenhain’s variant, of CreutzfeldtJakob disease, 166 Hepatic encephalopathy, 156–157 Herbal medicines, 85 Hereditary metabolic diseases, 167 Herpes simplex virus (HSV) encephalitis, 170 Heterocyclic antidepressants, 228–229 Highly active antiretroviral therapy (HAART), 85 Hip fracture, and delirium, 16, 18 Hippocampus, and Alzheimer’s disease, 110 Histoplasmosis, 170 Histrionic personality disorder, 52 HIV-1-associated cognitive/motor complex, 168 HIV-1-associated dementia (HAD), 168–169 Home, as living setting, 405, 411–417 Homocysteine metabolism, 90 Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT), 296 Hormones. See Endocrine disorders; Estrogen therapy Hospice care, 365–366 Hospitalization for injuries from falls, 391 involuntary as legal issue, 344 management of cognitive impairment and medical illness, 213 as setting for medical evaluation, 80 Housing. See Environment; Living settings; Nursing homes
Human immunodeficiency virus (HIV). See also Acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS) apathy and, 71 medical evaluation and, 84, 85 reversible dementia and, 168–169 Human immunodeficiency virus type 1 (HIV-1)-associated dementia, 168–169, 256 Human-subject research, and informed consent, 351–354 Humor, and coping, 55, 189 Huntington’s disease electroencephalography and, 94 genetic testing for, 92, 349 reversible dementia and, 164–165 suicide and, 62–63 Huntington’s Disease Society of America (HDSA), 543 Huperzine A, 456 Hydroxycholoroquine, 467 Hyperalertness, 68 Hypercalcemia, 157 Hypercholesterolemia, 90 Hypercortisolemia, 158 Hyperglycemia, 156 Hyperhomocysteinemia, 90, 269 Hyperparathyroidism, 157–158 Hypersexuality, 257, 335. See also Sexuality Hypersomnia, 67 Hyperthyroidism, 157 Hypoglycemia, 156 Hypolipidemic agents, and cerebrovascular disease, 269 Hyponatremia, 156 Hypothyroidism, 157 Ibuprofen, 445–446 Idebenone, 462 Illusions, and cognitive impairment, 69 Imbecility, categories of, 13 Imipramine, 222, 229 Immature defense mechanisms, 57
Index Immunization therapy, for Alzheimer’s disease, 447, 468–469 Impaired attention, and delirium, 18 Impaired decision-making capacity (IDMC), 352–354 Incontinence, and nursing homes, 332, 391–393 Independence, and living environment, 360–361, 408, 414. See also Autonomy Individual differences, in dementing illness, 14, 49 Infections. See also Acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS); Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease; Human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) delirium and, 159 reversible dementias and, 152, 165–167, 169–170 Informal support systems, 374 Informed consent, 346, 350–354 Informed decisions, and community resources, 376–378 Inhalant-related disorders, 7–8 Inhalants, and reversible dementia, 155 In re Colyer (1983), 338 In re Conroy (1985), 338, 366 In re Dinnerstein (1978), 366 Insanity defense, 343–344 Insomnia, 67, 253 Intellectual ability, and neuropsychological tests, 290, 291–293 Interictal epileptiform discharges (IEDs), 95 Internet, as source of information, 545–546 Interventions. See also Management for caregiver burden and stress, 334 neuropsychological evaluation and recommendations for, 287–288
561
Interviews caregiver stress and, 334–335 neuropsychological evaluation and, 289 Inventory, of assets and liabilities, 345 I Remember When: Activity Ideas to Help People Reminisce (Thorsheim and Roberts 2000), 198 Irritability, and case example of dementia, 322–323 Joint Commission on Accreditation of Healthcare Organizations, 366 Judgment. See also Competency; Decision making mental status examination and, 35–36 psychotropic drugs and impaired, 257–258 Korsakoff’s syndrome, 20, 160–161 Kraepelin, E., 13, 153 Laboratory studies, and medical evaluation, 89–91 Landmarks, and assisted-living settings, 421 Language. See also Communication; Language disorders Alzheimer’s disease and impairments of, 141 mental status examination and, 35 neuropsychological tests and, 299–301 Language disorders, and clinical diagnosis, 31–32 Learning. See also Education; Overlearned skills management of cognitive impairment and avoidance of new, 199 neuropsychological tests and, 290
562
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Legal issues at-risk situations and, 347–350 competency and testamentary capacity as, 341–345 end-of-life issues and, 366–367 for family and caregivers, 326, 338 informed consent and, 350–354 nursing homes and injuries from falls, 389 Leuprolide, 257 Lewy bodies. See Dementia with Lewy bodies Life review, and psychotherapy, 211–212 Life support, artificial maintenance of, 338 Lighting, and nursing homes, 425–426. See also Bright light therapy Limbic encephalitis, 166–167 Lipid profile, and hypercholesterolemia, 90 Lipoprotein receptor-related protein, 440–441 Lithium, and agitation, 249 Liver disease, and reversible dementia, 155, 156–157 Liver function, and psychotropic drugs, 221 Living settings, for patients with cognitive impairment, 405–406. See also Environment Long term care. See also Nursing homes caregivers and placement issues, 336–337 ethical issues and environment of, 359–363 Lorazepam age-related considerations, 221 agitation and, 240 amnestic episodes and, 20 delirium and, 235, 236 dosage of, 237 Loss, and depression, 61–62
Lovastatin, 470 Low-density lipoprotein (LDL) receptor proteins, 441–442 Loxapine, 241 Lumbar puncture, 91, 213 Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), 106–112, 142, 151, 160, 165, 213 Magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS), 127–130 Malingering, and cognitive impairment, 31 Management, psychological and behavioral. See also Interventions; Treatment ABC approach to, 200–201, 203, 211 activity therapies and, 212 agitation and, 204 anxiety and, 203–204 apathy and withdrawal, 207–208 behavior control as ethical issue and, 365 delirium and, 202–203 depression and, 202 general principles of, 190–200 hallucinations and, 206–207 medical illness and, 212–214 memory training and retention, 208–210 physiological factors in, 182–187 psychological factors in, 188–190 psychotherapy and, 210–212 sleep-wake disturbances and, 205 suspiciousness and delusional thinking, 205–206 wandering and, 204–205, 365 Mania, and medical conditions, 23 Marchiafava-Bignami syndrome, 154 Marriage competence as legal issue and, 344 sexuality and elderly, 335–336 spouses as caregivers and, 331–332
Index Maryland, and nursing homes, 406 Mattis Dementia Rating Scale, 29, 85 Mecobalamin, 463 Medicaid, 418 Medical care, and ethical issues, 362–363. See also Clinical care; Compliance; Hospitalization; Medical illness; Medical records; Physicians; Terminal care Medical ethics, 346 Medical evaluation. See also Diagnosis; Neuroimaging; Neuropsychological evaluation angiography and, 93 brain biopsy and, 93 carotid and transcranial sonography, 93–94 of cognitive dysfunction, 78, 79 electroencephalography and, 94–95 frequency of, 96–97 goals of, 77 genetic markers and, 92 laboratory studies and, 89–91 level of workup and, 96 neurological examination and, 86–89 patient history and , 81–85 physical examination and, 86 reevaluation and, 97 setting for, 80 Medical illness. See also Acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS); Human immunodeficiency virus (HIV); Infections; Medical care management of cognitive impairment and, 212–214 neuropsychological evaluation and patient history of, 288 Medical powers of attorney, 345, 366 Medical records, and patient history, 33. See also Medical care; Medical illness Medicare, 326–327, 365–366
563
Medications. See also Antidepressants; Antipsychotics; Barbiturates; Benzodiazepines; Drug-drug interactions; Neuroleptics; Psychotropic drugs; Tranquilizers Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive enhancers, 362 cerebrovascular disease and, 267–269 delirium and, 64 digital technology and smart pill containers, 417 education of family and, 324 falls in nursing homes and, 390–391 insomnia and, 67 medical evaluation and, 85 medical illness and compliance, 213–214 reversible dementia and, 153–154 symptoms of dementing illness and, 220 Medroxyprogesterone, 257, 336 Memantine, 461 Memory. See also Cognitive impairment age-associated impairment of, 11–13, 286–287 Alzheimer’s disease and, 140 caregivers and, 323 confabulation and, 68–69 depression and, 311 DSM-IV-TR and loss of, 287 management of cognitive impairment and training in, 208–210 neuropsychological tests and, 290, 295–297 normal aging and, 11 prevalence of problems in elderly, 1 Mental disorder not otherwise specified due to a general medical condition, 23 Mental disorders due to a general medical condition, 22–23
564
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Mental retardation, and clinical diagnosis, 26–27 Mental status examination cognitive dysfunction and, 34–36 diagnosis of dementia and, 13 malingering and, 31 standardized form for, 487–494 Meprobamate, 154 Metabolic disorders, and reversible dementia, 152, 155–159, 167 Metachromatic leukodystrophy, 167 Methylphenidate, 151, 234, 256 Metoclopramide, 391 Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) Alzheimer’s disease and, 12 cognitive patterns of, 309–310 reversibility of, 14 Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE), 1, 40–41, 85, 138, 140, 145, 256, 262, 265, 266, 292, 356, 386 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 (MMPI-2), 303 Minor cognitive/motor disorder (MCMD), 168 Mirtazapine, 229, 231, 232 Mixed dementia, 151 Molecular linkages, and Alzheimer’s disease, 443–444 Molecular therapy, for Alzheimer’s disease, 444–447 Monoamine oxidase inhibitors, 227, 233 Monoaminergic drugs, and Alzheimer’s disease, 459–460 Mood disorder due to a general medical condition, 22, 23 Motion detectors, 416 Motor system. See also Psychomotor abilities Alzheimer’s disease and, 140–141 neurological examination and, 87 neuropsychological testing and, 301–302 Multidimensional Observation Scale for Elderly Subjects, 396
Multiple cueing, and management of cognitive impairment, 195–196 Multiple sclerosis, 209 Muscarinic agonists, 461 Music therapy, 212, 396–397, 425 Mycobacterium avium, 170 Myoclonus, 87 Naming tests, 35 National Adult Reading Test-Revised (NART-R), 292 National agencies, contact information for, 541–544 National Association of Professional Geriatric Care Managers, 542 National Bioethics Advisory Commission (NBAC), 352–353 National Council on the Aging, Inc., 543 National Health Information Center, 543 National Huntington’s Disease Research Roster, 62 National Medical Expenditure Survey, 406 National Organization for Rare Disorders (NORD), 544 National Parkinson Foundation (NPF), 544 National Stroke Association (NSA), 543 Native Americans, and Alzheimer’s disease, 443 Nefazodone, 228 Neprilysin, 436 Nerve growth factor (NGF), and Alzheimer’s disease, 464–465 Neuritic plaques, and Alzheimer’s disease, 139 Neuroacanthosis, 167 Neurobiology, of depression, 61 Neurochemical changes, and neuroimaging, 122 Neurocognitive testing, 289, 290. See also Neuropsychological evaluation
Index Neurodiagnosis, and cognitive assessment, 286–287 Neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs), and Alzheimer’s disease, 139, 441, 468 Neurogenerative diseases, and reversible dementia, 164–165 Neuroimaging. See also Medical evaluation functional types of, 112–130 future directions in, 130–131 HIV patients and, 169 recent advances in, 103 structural types of, 104–112 Neuroleptic malignant syndrome (NMS), 242, 247–248 Neuroleptics. See also Antipsychotics; Atypical neuroleptics agitation and, 240 delirium and, 234–235 dosages of, 225 nursing homes and, 248 psychotic depression and, 224 side effects of, 225 Neurology and neurological symptoms. See also Neuropsychological evaluation Alzheimer’s disease and, 141–142 medical evaluation and, 84, 86–89 standardized form for examination, 495–497 Neuropeptides, and Alzheimer’s disease, 460–461 Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI), 41–42, 71, 246, 250, 264, 265, 266, 513–523 Neuropsychological evaluation. See also Diagnosis; Neurocognitive testing; Neurology and neurological symptoms cognitive patterns of various types of dementia and, 304–313 components of comprehensive, 288–304 goals of, 286–287
565
recommendations for interventions and, 287–288 standardized testing and, 31, 290, 291–304 value of, 312–313 Neurosyphilis, 170 New Jersey, and end-of-life decisions, 338 New learning ability, 199 Nicotine-related disorders, 8 Nicotinic acid deficiency, 162 Nicotinic mechanisms, and Alzheimer’s disease, 457–458 Noise, and sleep in nursing homes, 394 Noncholinergic neurotransmitter replacement strategies, and Alzheimer’s disease, 459–460 Noncompetitive inhibition, and cholinesterase inhibitors, 457 Nonconfrontation, and management of cognitive impairment, 192–193, 205 Non-Pick lobar atrophy, 146–147 Nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs), 445–446, 467 Nonverbal communication, 382 Nonviral opportunistic infections, and reversible dementia, 169–170 Normalcy, and environment of older adult, 408 North Carolina, and community resources, 375 Nortriptyline, 229–230, 231, 232, 233 Nursing homes. See also Caregivers; Long term care activities of daily living and, 382–389 apathy and withdrawal in, 207–208 caregivers and placement, 336–337 communication and, 381–382 digital technology and, 426 dysfunctional behaviors and, 395–399 ethics and environment of, 359–363 falls and, 389–391
566
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Nursing homes (continued) family of dementia patients and, 326, 332, 334, 336–337 incontinence and, 332, 391–393 living environment and, 421–426 neuroleptics and, 248 sensory deficits and, 395 sleep and, 393–394 urban versus rural elders and, 375 wandering and, 205 Nursing-level dementia care (NLDC), 405, 406, 409, 410, 420, 422, 424–425 Nutritional status, of nursing home patients, 385. See also Diet; Eating behavior Olanzapine, 224, 244, 246, 247, 255 Olfaction, and neurological examination, 86 Omnibus Budget Reconciliation Act of 1987, 248 Onset, of symptoms of cognitive impairment, 81–82 Ophthalmologic examinations, 395 Opioid-related disorders, 8 Optimal autonomy, 193–194 Optimal stimulation, 197, 202–203 Organic compounds, and reversible dementia, 155 Orientation, and assisted-living settings, 418. See also Reality orientation Over-the-counter medications, 85 Overlearned skills, determining and using, 198–199, 212. See also Learning Oxazepam, 236–237, 254 Oxybutynin, 220 Panic attacks, and anxiety disorders, 24 Papaverine, 267 Paranoid ideation, and Alzheimer’s disease, 141 Parathyroid adenoma, 157
Parents, and dementia in adult child, 333. See also Family Parkinsonism, and neurological examination, 87 Parkinson’s disease competency as legal issue in, 356–358 dementia with Lewy bodies and, 143, 144 depression and, 28 differential diagnosis of, 148–149 Paroxetine, 226–227, 231, 232, 252 Pathology, of Alzheimer’s disease, 138 Patient history aphasia and, 32 diagnosis of dementia and, 13 diagnosis of mental retardation and, 27 medical evaluation and, 81–85 neuropsychological evaluation and, 288–289 personality and, 53 taking of, 33–34 PC12 neural cells, 436 Pellagrinous encephalopathy, 154 Pemoline, 234 Pentobarbital, 235 Perception, and living environments, 423. See also Sensory-perceptual abilities Pernicious anemia, 161 Personality changes in and characterization of dementia, 36 function of in dementia, 51–59 management of dementia and premorbid, 188 neuropsychological evaluation and, 303 Personality change due to a general medical condition, 23, 25–26 Personal preferences, and utilization of community resources, 374–376 Pertinent autonomy, 408 Phantom boarder syndrome, 68, 69
Index Phencyclidine-related disorders, 8–9 Phenelzine, 231, 232, 233 Phototherapy, 425. See also Bright light therapy Physical activity, and nursing homes, 386–389, 394. See also Walking programs Physical examination, and medical evaluation, 86 Physicians. See also Medical care community resources and, 376 driving and safety issues, 361 end-of-life issues and, 366 medical illness in cognitively impaired and, 213 neuroleptic use in nursing homes and, 248 neuropsychological evaluation and, 312 Physiological factors, in management of dementia, 182–187 Physiologic reaction, and depression, 61 Physostigmine, 230, 260 Piaget, J., 183, 185 Pick, Arnold, 145 Pick’s disease, 94, 145, 146, 147 Pinel, P., 13 Planning for Long-Term Care, 546 Pleasant events, and management of cognitive impairment, 198 Pleasant Events Schedule, 195, 211, 329 Polysubstance-related disorder, 9 Porphobilinogen, 159 Positron emission tomography (PET), 112–122, 142, 165 Position sense, and sensation, 87–88 Positive bonding, and surrogate caregiving, 201 Postoperative cognitive dysfunction (POCD), 26 Postoperative delirium, 236 Poststroke depression, 60 Postsynaptic agonist strategy, and cholinergic system, 458–459
567
Posture, and neurological examination, 88 Powers of attorney, 345 Praxis, and mental status examination, 35 Predominant functions, and physiological factors in management, 182–183 Preoperational stage, of cognitive maturation, 184 Presenilin genes, 437–438 Press, of environment, 407 Prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease, 1, 138 of anxiety in Alzheimer’s patients, 65 of dementia, 1–2 of dementia with Lewy bodies, 144 of depression in Alzheimer’s patients, 59 of depression in cognitively impaired, 28 Prevention programs, and falls in nursing homes, 389–390 Prichard, J. A. C., 13 Primary control, 407 Primary memory, 34 Prion diseases, and reversible dementia, 165–166 Privacy, and assisted-living settings, 418–419 Problem-solving skills, and neuropsychological testing, 295 Procedural discourse, 301 Professional Environmental Assessment Procedure (PEAP), 409, 410, 423 Prognosis, as issue for families, 324–325 Progression, of symptoms in cognitive impairment, 82–83, 186–187 Progressive multifocal leukoencephalopathy (PML), 170 Progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP), 148 Projection, as defense mechanism, 57, 327–329
568
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Progopagnosia, 206 Propranolol, 251 Prospective planning, 347–348 Protective genes, and Alzheimer’s disease, 443 Protein phosphatase 2A (PP2A), 441 Proxy decision makers (PDMs), 363–364 Pseudodementia, 28 Psychiatric disorders. See also Cognitive disorders; Depression; DSM-IV-TR; Schizophrenia medical evaluation and patient history of, 83 neuropsychological evaluation and patient history of, 288 reversible dementias and, 152, 153 Psychiatric symptoms, of Alzheimer’s disease, 141 Psychological factors, in management of dementia, 188–190 Psychological point of view, and caregivers, 327–329 Psychological process, dementing illness as, 53–59 Psychological symptoms, and psychotropic drugs, 258–269 Psychomotor abilities, and neuropsychological tests, 290, 301–302. See also Motor system Psychostimulants, 256. See also Stimulants Psychotherapy, and management of cognitive impairment, 210–212 Psychotic defense mechanisms, 57–58 Psychotic depression, 224 Psychotic disorder due to a general medical condition, 20, 22, 23 Psychotropic drugs. See also Antidepressants; Antipsychotics; Barbiturates; Benzodiazepines; Dosage; Medications; Neuroleptics; Side effects; Toxicity; Tranquilizers; Withdrawal agitation and, 238–252 anxiety and, 236–238
apathy and withdrawal, 256–257 behavioral disturbances attributable to effects of, 220 delirium and, 234–236 depression and, 222–234 general principles for use of, 219–222 hallucinations and, 255–256 hypersexuality and, 257 impaired judgment and, 257–258 real world use of for behavioral and psychological symptoms, 258–269 reversible dementia and, 154 sleep-wake disturbance and, 252–254 suspiciousness and delusions, 254–255 Public housing, for elderly, 406 Pyritinol, 463 Pyrroloporphyria, Swedish type, 158–159 Quality of life as issue for family and caregivers, 326, 337–338 measures of, 42–43 Quality of Life in Alzheimer’s Disease (QOL-AD) Scale, 42–43, 529–533 Quality of Life in Dementia (QUALID) Scale, 43, 535–537 Quetiapine, 225, 246–247, 248, 255 Radioisotopes, and SPECT, 123 Range of motion (ROM) exercises, 389 Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep behavior disorder, 254 Reaction formations, as defense mechanisms, 56 Reality orientation, and management of cognitive impairment, 198–199, 395. See also Orientation Recall, and normal aging, 11–13. See also Free recall; Memory
Index Receptive language, 299 Reducing choices, and management of cognitive impairment, 197 Reevaluation, and medical evaluations, 97 Reflexes, and neurological examination, 88 Regression, as defense mechanism, 58 Rehabilitation memory-training programs and, 209 nursing homes and physical activity, 388–389 Reinforcement, and management of cognitive impairment, 196–197 Religion, and end-of-life decisions, 366 Reminiscence, and life review, 211–212, 396 Remote memory, and Alzheimer’s disease, 12 Renal function, and psychotropic drugs, 221 Repetition, and management of cognitive impairment, 196 Reporting, of cognitive evaluation results, 303–304 Residential care facilities providing specialized dementia programs (RC-SDP), 420 Resources, contact information for, 541–546. See also Community resources Restraints, and nursing homes, 399 Reversibility, of cognitive impairment, 186–187 Reversible dementias causes of, 14, 16 differential diagnosis of, 151–170 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), 296 Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test, 297, 298 Risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease, 138 for dementing illness, 325, 346, 348–350
569
Risperidone, 224, 225, 245–246, 247 Rivastigmine, 261, 262–263, 266, 454–455, 456 Rocking, as therapeutic activity, 388 Rural areas, and nursing homes, 375, 376 Safety. See also Driving; Falls assisted-living settings and, 418 digital technology and, 417 as issue for family and caregivers, 326, 337–338 management of cognitive impairment and, 193, 208 screening of home environments for hazards and, 412–413, 414–415 Salt-wasting nephropathy, 156 Scapegoating, and long-term care placement, 336–337 Schizophrenia medical evaluation and cognitive impairment in, 83 onset of cognitive dysfunction in, 82 reversible dementia and, 153 Scopolamine, and memory, 260 Secondary control, 407 Secondary memory, 34–35 Secondary parkinsonism, 87 Secured Perimeters Pilot Program (California), 420 Security, and assisted-living settings, 418 Sedative-, hypnotic-, or anxiolyticrelated disorders, 9 Seizures electroencephalography and, 95 medical evaluation and neurological symptoms, 84 Selective attention, 294 Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), 226–228, 258, 336 Selegiline, 267, 459–460, 462
570
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Self. See also Self-esteem assisted-living settings and identity issues, 419 orientation to, 183 Self-awareness, and coping, 55 Self-esteem. See also Self caregivers and, 329 coping mechanisms and, 55 Self-sufficiency, and environment of older adult, 408 Semantic dementia, 145 Sensation, and neurological examination, 87–88 Sensorimotor stage, of cognitive maturation, 183–184 Sensory impairment, and management of cognitive impairment, 190–192, 395 Sensory-perceptual abilities, and neuropsychological tests, 302–303. See also Perception Sensory Perceptual Examination, 302–303 Serotonin agonists, and agitation, 251–252 Serotonin reuptake inhibitors, and reversible dementia, 156 Serotonin syndrome, 227 Sertraline, 223, 226, 231, 232 Setting, of medical evaluation, 80. See also Environment; Living settings Sexuality. See also Hypersexuality caregivers and, 335–336 management of inappropriate behavior and, 188 psychotropic drugs and, 257 Side effects, of psychotropic drugs. See also Medications; specific drugs age-related considerations and, 221 of antipsychotics, 241 of benzodiazepines, 237 of monoamine oxidase inhibitors, 233 neuroleptic malignant syndrome (NMS) and, 242, 247–248
of psychostimulants, 256 serotonin syndrome and, 227 tardive dyskinesia syndromes and, 243–244, 247–248 of tricyclic antidepressants, 230, 232 Simplification, and management of cognitive impairment, 194 Simulated Presence (SimPres), 396 Single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), 122–124, 142, 151, 160, 165 Skills. See also Coping activities of daily living and training in, 383–384 management of cognitive impairment and overlearned, 198–199, 212 Sleep and sleep-wake disturbances. See also Sleep apnea caregivers and, 332 management of cognitive impairment and, 205 nursing homes and, 393–394 psychological and behavioral concomitants of dementing illness and, 66–68 psychotropic drugs and, 252–254 Sleep apnea, 67, 253, 254, 394 Smart pill containers, 417 Smoke detectors, 416 Snoezelen, 212 Social contact, and assisted-living settings, 419 Social withdrawal. See also Apathy Alzheimer’s disease and, 71 management of cognitive impairment and, 207–208 Sodium levels, and reversible dementia, 156 Solvents, and reversible dementia, 155 Special care units (SCUs), 416, 422–424 Spinal fluid, and medical evaluation, 91
Index Splitting, as defense mechanism, 58–59 Standardized instruments assessment of decision-making capacity and, 354–359 diagnosis of dementing illness and, 39–43 neuropsychological evaluation and, 290, 291–303 Standards, for competence and testamentary capacity, 343–344. See also Guidelines State agencies, contact information for, 544–545 Statin drugs, 269, 447, 469–470 Stepwise progression, of symptoms in cognitive impairment, 82 Stimulants. See also Psychostimulants depression in elderly and, 234 sleep-wake disturbance and, 253 Stimulation, and management of cognitive impairment, 197, 202–203, 419 Stress, and caregivers, 334–335 Stroke depression after, 28, 59, 163 reversible dementia and, 162–163 vascular dementia and, 151 Stroop Color Word Test, 294 Structural neuroimaging, 104–112 Structure, and management of cognitive impairment, 195 Subcortical dementia case example of, 38–39 characterization of, 36–37 cognitive patterns of, 307–308 memory and, 296 Subdural hematoma, 163–164 Sublimation, and coping, 54 Substance-induced mood disorder, 24 Substance-induced persisting dementia, 18, 21 Substance intoxication delirium, 19 Substance-related disorders, DSM-IV-TR classification of, 5–10
571
Substance withdrawal delirium, 19 Suicide, and psychological and behavioral concomitants of dementing illness, 62–63 Sundowning Alzheimer’s disease and, 20, 140 cognitive impairment and optimal stimulation, 197 delirium and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, 159 Support groups, and Alzheimer’s disease, 210–211 Support services. See Community resources; Resources Suppression, and coping, 54 Supreme Court, and end-of-life issues, 338 Surrogate caregiving, and management of cognitive impairment, 201 Suspiciousness. See also Accusations cognitive impairment and, 68, 205–206 psychotropic drugs and, 254–255 Symbolic communication, and cognitive impairment, 183 Syphilis, 90, 170 Tacrine, 261, 455, 456, 465 Tardive dyskinesia syndromes, 243–244, 247–248 Tau protein, 147, 441–442, 468 Telephone, cognitive impairment and communication by, 194 Temazepam, 253 Temporoparietal dementia, 36, 38 Terminal care, as ethical issue, 347 Territory, as autonomy-related issue, 194 Testamentary capacity, as legal issue, 342, 343 Tetrahydroacridinamine. See Tacrine Texas Functional Living Scale (TFLS), 293
572
THE DEMENTIAS, THIRD EDITION
Thiamine Alzheimer’s disease and dietary supplements, 463 amnestic disorder and deficiency of, 20 reversible dementia and deficiency of, 160–161 Thioridazine, 241 Thiothixene, 241 36-Hour Day, The (Mace and Rabins 1999), 321, 416 Threshold diagnosis, 3 Threshold model, of dementing illness, 50 Thrombolytic agents, and stroke, 163 Thyroid laboratory tests for function, 90 reversible dementia and disease of, 157 Ticlopidine hydrochloride, 268 Toileting strategies, in nursing homes, 392 Toluene, 155 Tomlinson, B. E., 138 Toxicity, of psychotropic drugs in elderly, 229, 242–244 Toxins cholinergic system and, 458 cognitive dysfunction and exposure to environmental, 85 laboratory tests and, 90–91 reversible dementia and, 152, 153–154, 155 Toxoplasma gondii and toxoplasmosis 169 Trail Making Test, 307 Tranquilizers, for agitation or anxiety, 236–237, 239–240 Transcranial Doppler imaging, 94 Transgenic models, of Alzheimer’s disease, 439–440 Tranylcypromine, 231, 232, 233 Trazodone, 228, 239, 252, 253 Treatable dementia-producing conditions, 14, 16
Treatment. See also Interventions; Management; Psychotropic drugs advances in for Alzheimer’s disease, 453–471 of cerebrovascular disease, 267–269 neuropsychological evaluation and effects of medical, 312–313 of Parkinson’s disease, 149 of vascular dementia, 151 Triazolam, 253 Tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs). See also Antidepressants depression in elderly and, 223, 229–233 serotonin syndrome and, 227 Trifluoperazine, 225 Triggers, for agitation, 204 Trihexyphenidyl, 242 Trizolam, 20 Tube feedings, and nursing homes, 385–386 Tuberculosis, 170 Tyramine, and monoamine oxidase inhibitors, 233 Universal design, of living environments, 412 University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, 292, 433, 438 Urban areas, and nursing homes, 375 Uremic encephalopathy, 155 Urinalysis, and laboratory studies, 90, 91 Urinary incontinence, 391–392 Vaccine, development of for Alzheimer’s disease, 445, 468–469 Validation therapy, 211 Vascular dementia anxiety symptoms and, 65 cognitive patterns of, 305–306 differential diagnosis of, 149–151 DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for, 17
Index Venlafaxine, 227, 228, 231, 232 Verbal fluency, 300 Verbal memory, 296–297 Verbal Series Attention Test (VSAT), 294 Vibration sense, and neurological examination, 87–88 Violent behavior, management of, 190. See also Aggression; Behavior Viral infections, and reversible dementia, 170 Virtue ethics, 346 Vision and visual acuity management of cognitive impairment and, 190–191, 192 neurological examination and, 86 neuropsychological tests and, 302–303 nursing homes and, 395 Visits, and nursing homes, 337 Visual memory, 297 Visuospatial skills, and neuropsychological tests, 290, 298–299 Vitamin deficiencies. See also Diet Alzheimer’s disease and, 463 medical evaluation and, 89–90 nursing homes and, 387 reversible dementia and, 152, 160–162 Vitamin C, and Alzheimer’s disease, 462–463 Vitamin E Alzheimer’s disease and, 142, 267, 462–463 tardive dyskinesia and, 244 Voidable marriage, 344 Volume depletion, and reversible dementia, 156 Voxel-based morphometry, 111
573
Walking programs, and nursing homes, 387–388, 392, 397 Wandering, and management of cognitive impairment, 204–205, 365, 397 Warfarin, 267, 268 Washington University Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Scale, 40, 501–502 Water intoxication, 156 Web sites, and sources of information, 545–546 Wechsler, D., 137–138 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), 291, 300 Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS), 294, 297 Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome, 154 Wernicke’s aphasia, 32 Wernicke’s encephalopathy, 20 Wernicke’s syndrome, 160–161, 236 Wide Range Achievement Test, 3rd Edition (WRAT-3) Wills, and testamentary capacity, 343, 345 Wilson’s disease, 90, 158 Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, 295, 307 Withdrawal, from medications. See also Social withdrawal benzodiazepines and, 237 delirium and, 235 selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and, 227–228 stimulants and, 234 Word fluency, 35 Zaleplon, 254 Zinc, and chelating agents, 464 Ziprasidone, 247 Zolpidem, 254